Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-dnltx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-16T09:20:07.965Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

9 - Evolutionary diversification of mating behaviour: using artificial neural networks to study reproductive character displacement and speciation

from Part III - Artificial neural networks as models of perceptual processing in ecology and evolutionary biology

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 July 2011

Karin S. Pfennig
Affiliation:
University of North Carolina
Michael J. Ryan
Affiliation:
University of Texas
Colin R. Tosh
Affiliation:
University of Leeds
Graeme D. Ruxton
Affiliation:
University of Glasgow
Get access

Summary

Introduction

When species with similar sexual signals co-occur, selection may favour divergence of these signals to minimise either their interference or the risk of mis-mating between species, a process termed reproductive character displacement (Howard, 1993; Andersson, 1994; Servedio & Noor, 2003; Coyne & Orr, 2004; Pfennig & Pfennig, 2009). This selective process potentially results in mating behaviours that are not only divergent between species that co-occur but that are also divergent among conspecific populations that do and do not occur with heterospecifics or that co-occur with different heterospecifics (reviewed in Howard, 1993; Andersson, 1994; Gerhardt & Huber, 2002; Coyne & Orr, 2004; e.g., Noor, 1995; Saetre et al., 1997; Pfennig, 2000; Gabor & Ryan, 2001; Höbel & Gerhardt, 2003).

An oft-used approach to assessing whether reproductive character displacement has occurred between species relies on behavioural experiments that evaluate mate preferences from populations that do and do not occur with heterospecifics (sympatry and allopatry, respectively). In such experiments, individuals are presented the signals of heterospecifics and/or conspecifics to assess whether allopatric individuals are more likely to mistakenly prefer heterospecifics than are sympatric individuals (reviewed in Howard, 1993). The expectation is that individuals from sympatry should preferentially avoid heterospecifics, whereas those in allopatry should fail to distinguish heterospecifics from conspecifics (presumably because, unlike sympatric individuals, they have not been under selection to do so). Such patterns of discrimination have been observed, and they provide some of the strongest examples of reproductive character displacement (reviewed in Howard, 1993).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Andersson, M. 1994 Sexual Selection. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Barton, N. H. 2001. The role of hybridization in evolution. Mol Ecol 10, 551–568.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barton, N. H. & Hewitt, G. M. 1989. Adaptation, speciation and hybrid zones. Nature 341, 497–503.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butlin, R. 1987. Speciation by reinforcement. Trends Ecol Evol 2, 8–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conner, J. K. & Hartl, D. L. 2004. A Primer of Ecological Genetics. Sinauer Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
Coyne, J. A. & Orr, H. A. 2004. Speciation. Sinauer Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
Demuth, H. & Beale, M. 1997. Neural Network Toolbox. The Math Works, Inc.Google Scholar
Dobzhansky, T. 1940. Speciation as a stage in evolutionary divergence. Am Nat 74, 312–321.Google Scholar
Elman, J. L. 1990. Finding structure in time. Cogn Sci 14, 179–211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Enquist, M. & Ghirlanda, S. 2005. Neural Networks and Animal Behavior. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Falconer, D. S. & Mackay, T. F. C. 1996. Introduction to Quantitative Genetics. 4th edn. Longman Group Ltd.Google Scholar
Gabor, C. R. & Ryan, M. J. 2001. Geographical variation in reproductive character displacement in mate choice by male sailfin mollies. Proc R Soc B 268, 1063–1070.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerhardt, H. C. 1991. Female mate choice in treefrogs – static and dynamic acoustic criteria. Anim Behav 42, 615–635.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerhardt, H. C. 1994. Reproductive character displacement of female mate choice in the gray treefrog, Hyla chrysoscelis. Anim Behav 47, 959–969.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerhardt, H. C. & Huber, F. 2002. Acoustic Communication in Insects and Anurans: Common Problems and Diverse Solutions. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Hankison, S. J. & Morris, M. R. 2003. Avoiding a compromise between sexual selection and species recognition: female swordtail fish assess multiple species-specific cues. Behav Ecol 14, 282–287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Höbel, G. & Gerhardt, H. C. 2003. Reproductive character displacement in the acoustic communication system of green tree frogs (Hyla cinerea). Evolution 57, 894–904.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoskin, C. J., Higgie, M., McDonald, K. R. & Moritz, C. 2005. Reinforcement drives rapid allopatric speciation. Nature 437, 1353–1356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howard, D. J. 1993. Reinforcement: origin, dynamics, and fate of an evolutionary hypothesis. In Hybrid Zones and the Evolutionary Process (ed. Harrison, R. G.), pp. 46–69. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kelly, J. K. & Noor, M. A. F. 1996. Speciation by reinforcement: a model derived from studies ofDrosophila. Genetics 143, 1485–1497.Google ScholarPubMed
Kirkpatrick, M. & Servedio, M. R. 1999. The reinforcement of mating preferences on an island. Genetics 151, 865–884.Google ScholarPubMed
Liou, L. W. & Price, T. D. 1994. Speciation by reinforcement of premating isolation. Evolution 48, 1451–1459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noor, M. A. 1995. Speciation driven by natural selection inDrosophila. Nature 375, 674–675.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patterson, H. E. H. 1985. The recognition concept of species. In Species and Speciation, Transvaal Museum Monograph No. 4 (ed. E. Vrba), pp. 21–29. Transvaal Museum.Google Scholar
Pfennig, D. W. & Rice, A. M. 2007. An experimental test of character displacement's role in promoting postmating isolation between conspecific populations in contrasting competitive environments. Evolution 61, 2433–2443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pfennig, K. S. 1998. The evolution of mate choice and the potential for conflict between species and mate-quality recognition. Proc R Soc B 265, 1743–1748.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pfennig, K. S. 2000. Female spadefoot toads compromise on mate quality to ensure conspecific matings. Behav Ecol 11, 220–227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pfennig, K. S. & Pfennig, D. W. 2009. Character displacement: ecological and reproductive responses to a common evolutionary problem. Quart Rev Biol 84, 253–276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pfennig, K. S. & Ryan, M. J. 2006. Reproductive character displacement generates reproductive isolation among conspecific populations: an artificial neural network study. Proc R Soc B 273, 1361–1368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pfennig, K. S. & Ryan, M. J. 2007. Character displacement and the evolution of mate choice: an artificial neural network approach. Phil Trans R Soc B 36, 411–419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phelps, S. M. & Ryan, M. J. 1998. Neural networks predict response biases of female túngara frogs. Proc R Soc B 265, 279–285.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phelps, S. M. & Ryan, M. J. 2000. History influences signal recognition: neural network models of túngara frogs. Proc R Soc B 267, 1633–1639.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phelps, S. M., Ryan, M. J. & Rand, A. S. 2001. Vestigial preference functions in neural networks and túngara frogs. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98, 13161–13166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reeve, H. K. 1989. The evolution of conspecific acceptance thresholds. Am Nat 133, 407–435.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rodriguez, R. L., Sullivan, L. E. & Cocroft, R. B. 2004. Vibrational communication and reproductive isolation in the Enchenopa binotata species complex of treehoppers (Hemiptera: Membracidae). Evolution 58, 571–578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryan, M. J. & Getz, W. 2000. Signal decoding and receiver evolution – an analysis using an artificial neural network. Brain Behav Evol 56, 45–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ryan, M. J., Phelps, S. M. & Rand, A. S. 2001. How evolutionary history shapes recognition mechanisms. Trends Cogn Sci 5, 143–148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ryan, M. J. & Rand, A. S. 1993. Species recognition and sexual selection as a unitary problem in animal communication. Evolution 47, 647–657.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ryan, M. J., Rand, W., Hurd, P. L., Phelps, S. M. & Rand, A. S. 2003. Generalization in response to mate recognition signals. Am Nat 161, 380–394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saetre, G. P., Moum, T., Bures, S.et al. 1997. A sexually selected character displacement in flycatchers reinforces premating isolation. Nature 387, 589–592.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Servedio, M. R. & Kirkpatrick, M. 1997. The effects of gene flow on reinforcement. Evolution 51, 1764–1772.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Servedio, M. R. & Noor, M. A. F. 2003. The role of reinforcement in speciation: theory and data. Ann Rev Ecol Evol System 34, 339–364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sokal, R. R. & Rohlf, F. J. 1995. Biometry. W. H. Freeman and Co.Google Scholar
Stebbins, R. C. 2003. A Field Guide to Western Reptiles and Amphibians. Houghton Mifflin Company.Google Scholar
Wagner, W. E. 1998. Measuring female mating preferences. Anim Behav 55, 1029–1042.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiley, R. H. 1994. Errors, exaggeration, and deception in animal communication. In Behavioral Mechanisms in Ecology (ed. Real, L.), pp. 157–189. University of Chicago Press.
Zar, J. H. 1984. Biostatistical Analysis. Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×