Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-gtxcr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T01:50:24.373Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

What we can learn from second animal neuroscience

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 July 2013

Benjamin C. Nephew*
Affiliation:
Cummings School of Veterinary Medicine, Tufts University, North Grafton, MA 01536. bcnephew@aol.comhttp://vet.tufts.edu/facpages/nephew_b.html

Abstract

There are several facets of second-person neuroscience which can benefit from comparisons with animal behavioral neuroscience studies. This commentary addresses the challenges involved in obtaining quantitative data from second-person techniques, the role of stress in inducing robust responses, the use of interactive functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), and the value of applying interactive methods to studies of aggression and depression.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Caffrey, M. K., Nephew, B. C. & Febo, M. (2010) Central vasopressin V1a receptors modulate neural processing in mothers facing intruder threat to pups. Neuropharmacology 58(1):107–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Febo, M. & Ferris, C. F. (2007) Development of cocaine sensitization before pregnancy affects subsequent maternal retrieval of pups and prefrontal cortical activity during nursing. Neuroscience 148(2):400–12.Google Scholar
Febo, M., Numan, M. & Ferris, C. F. (2005) Functional magnetic resonance imaging shows oxytocin activates brain regions associated with mother-pup bonding during suckling. Journal of Neuroscience 25(50):11637–44.Google Scholar
Ferris, C. F., Kulkarni, P., Sullivan, J. M. Jr., Harder, J. A., Messenger, T. L. & Febo, M. (2005) Pup suckling is more rewarding than cocaine: Evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging and three-dimensional computational analysis. Journal of Neuroscience. 25(1):149–56.Google Scholar
Ferris, C. F., Stolberg, T., Kulkarni, P., Murugavel, M., Blanchard, R., Blanchard, D. C., Febo, M., Brevard, M. & Simon, N. G. (2008) Imaging the neural circuitry and chemical control of aggressive motivation. BMC Neuroscience 9(11):14712202.Google Scholar
Haller, J., Fuchs, E., Halasz, J. & Makara, G. B. (1998) Defeat is a major stressor in males while social instability is stressful mainly in females: Towards the development of a social stress model in female rats. Brain Research Bulletin 50(1):3339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hammen, C. (2005) Stress and depression. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology 1:293319.Google Scholar
Laurent, H. K. & Ablow, J. C. (2012) A cry in the dark: Depressed mothers show reduced neural activation to their own infants cry. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 7(2):125–34.Google Scholar
Nephew, B. C. & Bridges, R. S. (2011) Effects of chronic social stress during lactation on maternal behavior and growth in rats. Stress 14(6):677–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nephew, B. C., Caffrey, M. K., Felix-Ortiz, A. C., Ferris, C. F. & Febo, M. (2009) Blood oxygen level-dependent signal responses in corticolimbic “emotions” circuitry of lactating rats facing intruder threat to pups. European Journal of Neuroscience 30(5):934–45.Google Scholar
Strathearn, L., Fonagy, P., Amico, J. & Montague, P. R. (2009) Adult attachment predicts maternal brain and oxytocin response to infant cues. Neuropsychopharmacology 34:2655–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tamashiro, K. L. K., Nguyen, M. M. N. & Sakai, R. R. (2005) Social stress: From rodents to primates. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 26(1):2740.Google Scholar