Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-wq484 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T07:04:34.276Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The motivational dimension of language teaching

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  31 May 2017

Martin Lamb*
Affiliation:
University of Leeds, UKm.v.lamb@education.leeds.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Motivation is recognized as a vital component in successful second language learning, and has been the subject of intensive research in recent decades. This review focuses on a growing branch of this research effort, that which examines the motivational effects of language teaching. This is pertinent because, despite enhanced mobility and expanding access to foreign languages online, most learners’ early encounters with the second language (L2) still take place in classrooms, and these encounters may shape attitudes and determine students’ willingness to invest further in the L2. Four main types of research are reviewed: first, that which deliberately seeks to identify and evaluate strategies to motivate L2 learners; second, that which has tested the validity of psychological theories of motivation by applying their precepts in L2 classrooms; third, that which assesses the motivational effects of a pedagogical innovation or intervention; fourth, research on what has been too often the unintended outcome of language education, namely learner demotivation. The review highlights the complexity of the relationship between teaching and learner motivation but an attempt is made to articulate some emerging verities and to point towards the most promising avenues for future research.

Type
State-of-the-Art Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

1. Introduction

This article reviews the growing amount of research evidence about what language teachers can do to motivate their learners. It comes at a time when the language teaching profession faces profound motivational challenges. In many global contexts, the English language is fast becoming established as a basic skill on the national curriculum, increasingly taught at all levels from primary to tertiary, and assessed in high-stakes examinations (Graddol Reference Graddol2006). English teachers find themselves in high demand but also often find that their jobs are increasingly demanding of them; they are under pressure from their institutions to raise standards and respond to curricular innovations, and also from pupils, who are less accepting of a submissive role in class, and expect to be entertained as well as educated. In Anglophone countries, the global spread of English has undermined the raison d’être for foreign language teaching, reflected in a diminished role for languages in national curricula and shrinking enrolments for study at higher levels (Coleman Reference Coleman2009), and forcing institutions and teachers to provide learners with new motives for learning (Gallagher-Brett Reference Gallagher-Brett2004).

The review aims to address the motivational challenges faced by language teachers in all contexts, though as will be seen there is a preponderance of research on the teaching of English, especially in formal secondary and tertiary education, and much less on the teaching of modern foreign languages (MFL), of heritage languages, of host country languages for migrant adults, or other contexts of language teaching. Its scope will be limited to empirical studies of what is usually within the control of the teacher – for example particular teaching methods or techniques, decisions about learning materials and tasks, forms of classroom assessment, the deployment of new technologies, classroom language – or of their institutions – for example whether a language is taught as a separate subject or as a medium of instruction (e.g. in content and language integrated learning or CLIL). Excluded by this criterion are the motivational impact of national assessment regimes, decisions about when languages are introduced into the curriculum, the motivational results of teacher development initiatives and other policy issues.

The article is warranted because of the surge in publications related to the pedagogical aspects of motivation in the last decade. In the most authoritative recent review of L2 motivation, Dörnyei & Ushioda (Reference Dörnyei and Ushioda2011) comment that the ‘amount of research devoted to the question of motivating learners remains rather meagre relative to the total amount of research on L2 motivation’, but a forthcoming survey article (Boo, Dörnyei & Ryan Reference Boo, Dörnyei and Ryan2015) finds that approximately one third of 415 papers published in the period 2005–2014 had a main focus on ‘motivating’ as opposed to ‘motivation’. Yet it is only a generation ago that the eminent social psychologist Robert Gardner was able to assert that ‘[t]he learning of a second (or foreign) language in the school situation is often viewed as an educational phenomenon . . . such a perception is categorically wrong’ (Reference Gardner, Giles and Clair1979: 193). Instead the social psychologists’ primary interest was in learners’ attitudes towards the language and its speakers, and their motives for learning it. Perceptions began to change during the 1990s when, with the rise to prominence of social constructivist learning theory, it was recognized that ‘motivational sources closely related to the learners’ immediate classroom environment [had] a stronger impact on the overall L2 motivation complex than had been expected’ (Dörnyei & Ushioda Reference Dörnyei and Ushioda2011: 47), ushering in what came to be known as the ‘cognitive-situated’ phase of L2 motivation research. Longitudinal studies of L2 learner motivation (e.g. Chambers Reference Chambers1999, Nikolov Reference Nikolov1999) reinforced the view that teachers and teaching could play a central role in its development. Utilizing constructs from mainstream educational psychology, book length treatises appeared on how teachers can motivate their pupils to learn an L2 (Williams & Burden Reference Williams and Burden1997; Dörnyei Reference Dörnyei2001). Partly inspired by these works, and perhaps more recently swayed by an academic climate promoting ‘impact’, motivation researchers have increasingly made aspects of L2 pedagogy their main focus of study.

The first purpose of this review then is to chart the trajectory of this upsurge in research, prioritizing works published in major journals, monographs or edited books and identifying the main lines of inquiry. This provides the organizational structure of the article: the first section deals with research on motivational strategies (MotS), techniques deployed by teachers to deliberately enhance learner motivation; the second section examines research on L2 classrooms which adopts a particular theoretical orientation – two theories dominate here, self-determination theory (SDT) (Deci & Ryan Reference Deci and Ryan1985) and the L2 motivational self-system (L2MSS) (Dörnyei Reference Dörnyei, Dörnyei and Ushioda2009); the next group of studies are empirical investigations of L2 pedagogy which have a strong relevance for motivation – applications of new technology forming a significant majority; and then there is a review of research targeting the regrettably common phenomenon of learner demotivation i.e. where classroom processes actually diminish the motivation that learners initially bring with them. Only research papers addressing an aspect of pedagogy are reviewed; those focusing on learner L2 motivation are excluded unless their pedagogical implications are particularly compelling.

The second main purpose of the review is to draw out the main lessons that have been learned so far, to inform the training and education of language teachers, and to point researchers towards promising areas of further inquiry. The final section of the article therefore sets out an agenda for future research on the motivational dimension of language teaching, based on remaining gaps as well as on exciting new theoretical approaches to L2 motivation such as complex dynamic systems theory; it also makes suggestions for methods of investigation that could make the next decade even more productive of insights for motivating language learners.

It could be argued that any good teacher is, by definition, a motivator of learning. Yet there is clearly a motivational dimension to teaching that is distinct from simply ‘good teaching’. Where learners come to class already highly motivated – for example, when preparing for high-stakes exams – it is true that skillful teaching which enables them to achieve their goals as efficiently as possible will be felt to be motivational, even if the teacher makes no conscious effort to motivate. In many other educational contexts, learners enter classrooms with a modicum of motivation which good teachers can work with to produce acceptable results. But teachers who actually target learner motivation could nurture and strengthen it so that it promotes greater learning effort during the course, produces even better results, and perhaps even carries forward to future periods of study. In the most challenging kind of classrooms – this would include many secondary school MFL classes in Anglophone countries – learners may not have chosen to be there at all, and the generation of initial motivation is the teacher's most important role, one that is undeniably complex and difficult. Focus too much on the here-and-now, using traditional carrot and stick techniques, and teachers might preserve classroom discipline at the cost of long-term interest in the subject. Focus too much on the future, and their lessons may neglect learners’ current interests and identities. Such issues demand researchers’ serious attention.

2. Motivational strategies

2.1 Studies based on Dörnyei's (Reference Dörnyei2001) taxonomy

Whichever kind of classroom teachers occupy, most will have at some time or other employed strategies to boost their learners’ motivation. This section focuses on research which directly investigates the prevalence and, in some cases, the effectiveness of these kinds of practical decisions. Dörnyei & Ushioda (Reference Dörnyei and Ushioda2011: 103) define MotS as instructional techniques deployed ‘to consciously generate and enhance student motivation, as well as maintain ongoing motivated behaviour and protect it from distracting and/or competing action tendencies.’ This research has its origins in the The Modern Language Journal debates of the mid-1990s, in which a new generation of academics, concerned to make L2 motivation research more useful to education, were challenged by Gardner & Tremblay (Reference Gardner and Tremblay1994) to produce empirical evidence to support their claims that teachers could positively affect their pupils’ motivation. Dörnyei & Csizér's (Reference Dörnyei and Csizér1998) pioneering study of the views of Hungarian English as a foreign language (EFL) teachers was published just in time for inclusion in the last ‘State-of-the-Art’ review of L2 motivation (Dörnyei Reference Dörnyei1998). Their ‘10 Commandments for Motivating Language Learners’ represented groups of teaching techniques which Hungarian teachers claimed to use to motivate their pupils. Drawing on this study and on relevant theories of educational psychology, Dörnyei (Reference Dörnyei2001) expanded the list into a full taxonomy of 102 MotS (sometimes termed micro-strategies), grouped into 35 main strategies (or macro-strategies) and organized according to where in the teaching process they would likely be deployed, as shown in Figure 1.

Figure 1 Dörnyei's (Reference Dörnyei2001) taxonomy of motivational strategies

The clarity and comprehensiveness of Dörnyei's MotS framework attracted researchers to validate it empirically, first his own doctoral students (Cheng & Dörnyei Reference Cheng and Dörnyei2007; Guilloteaux & Dörnyei Reference Guilloteaux and Dörnyei2008) and then those beyond his immediate orbit (e.g. Sugita & Takeuchi Reference Sugita McEown and Takeuchi2010). Table 1 presents the most important research so far published. It shows how studies have become steadily more ambitious in their aims and more sophisticated in their approach, addressing some of the conceptual and methodological difficulties laid out by Gardner & Tremblay (Reference Gardner and Tremblay1994). The most straightforward type of study samples the views of teachers on the relative importance and frequency of use of given MotS (e.g. Cheng & Dörnyei Reference Cheng and Dörnyei2007; also see Guilloteaux Reference Guilloteaux2013). Other studies do this but also compare teachers’ views with those of students (e.g. Sugita McEown & Takeuchi Reference Sugita McEown and Takeuchi2014; also see Sugita McEown & Takeuchi Reference Sugita McEown and Takeuchi2010 and Ruesch, Bown & Dewey Reference Ruesch, Bown and Dewey2012). More sophisticated are those studies which actually evaluate the motivational effectiveness of MotS in terms of learners’ behaviour in class and their self-reported motivation (Guilloteaux & Dörnyei Reference Guilloteaux and Dörnyei2008; Papi & Abdollahzadeh Reference Papi and Abdollahzadeh2012; Wong Reference Wong2014). The most ambitious of all are the quasi-experiments (Moskovsky et al. Reference Moskovsky, Alrabai, Paolini and Ratcheva2012; Alrabai Reference Alrabai2016) which identify a set of contextually promising MotS, train teachers in using them, and then compare their motivational effects on classes of students against that of traditional teaching. This last type of study has only been conducted in Saudi Arabia to date, possibly because of the difficulties of gaining ethical approval in many contexts.

Table 1 Studies of language teachers’ MotS use, based on Dörnyei's Reference Dörnyei2001 framework

What have we learned so far? Some macro-strategies do appear to be valued universally by teachers and learners; these include displaying appropriate teacher behaviour (or ‘setting a good example’), fostering good teacher–student relations and promoting learner self-confidence. Instilling these principles in novice language teachers may be time well spent, though how they are realized in particular classrooms is something that teachers will still have to work out ‘on the job’ (see Section 6.2). Furthermore, as Table 2 shows, there is disagreement about the relative importance of different macro-strategies. There is some evidence, for example, that promoting learner autonomy is considered less important by teachers in Asia, but Asian teachers themselves differ in their valuing of a cohesive learner group (much favoured by Taiwanese but not Koreans or Chinese) and ‘offering rewards’ (by far the most popular MotS among Chinese school teachers) (Cheng & Dörnyei Reference Cheng and Dörnyei2007; Guilloteaux Reference Guilloteaux2013; Wong Reference Wong2014). There is also evidence that teachers and students’ views differ; for instance, Ruesch et al. (Reference Ruesch, Bown and Dewey2012) found that university students see motivational value in comparing themselves to other students – a practice denigrated by their tutors trained in ‘cooperative’ learning principles.

Table 2 Teachers’ views of importance of motivational macro-strategies in five different national contexts

*Being based on single items in surveys, these should be considered micro- rather than macro-strategies.

This ‘competition vs cooperation’ dilemma neatly captures a fundamental problem in the MotS approach; Dörnyei (Reference Dörnyei2001) himself seems to come down firmly on the side of ‘cooperation’, arguing that ‘there is nothing “healthy” about even a small dose of competition’ (p. 93), though in other sections he promotes the value of ‘intragroup competition’ (p. 44). Other work suggests that ‘competition within cooperation’ has the highest motivational value (Tauer & Harackiewicz Reference Tauer and Harackiewicz2004). The point is that it is simply not possible to reduce highly complex issues to pedagogical ‘dos or don'ts’. The successful motivator somehow learns when, where and how to deploy them in particular lessons.

Nevertheless, another major finding of this research is that MotS can work; that is, correlational studies show that they are associated with more motivated behaviour in class, in terms of paying attention and participating in class activities, and with more positive attitudes towards learning and the L2 (Guilloteaux & Dörnyei Reference Guilloteaux and Dörnyei2008; Papi & Abdollahzadeh Reference Papi and Abdollahzadeh2012). Admittedly, one can dispute what motivated classroom behaviour actually consists of (Ellis Reference Ellis2009, for example, proposes that ‘alertness’ would be a better criterion of engagement than ‘attention’), and correlation does not indicate a causal effect. But Guilloteaux & Dörnyei (Reference Guilloteaux and Dörnyei2008) point out that contrasting results for different (mixed-ability) classes in the same schools strongly suggests it is the teacher doing the motivating. Their argument has been strengthened by the subsequent quasi-experimental studies in which teachers specially trained in MotS produce positive changes in their learners’ motivation over the length of a course (Moskovsky et al. Reference Moskovsky, Alrabai, Paolini and Ratcheva2012) and even generate better L2 achievement, at least when compared to ‘traditional teaching’ as practised in Saudi Arabia (Alrabai Reference Alrabai2016).

Clearly, these are exciting results which merit further investigation. In particular, it would be valuable to see whether the deployment of particular MotS by L2 teachers have similar beneficial effects – on learner classroom behaviour, their longer-term motivation and, ultimately, on their achievement – in other global contexts. Education experts may be able to predict which MotS are lacking in, or would be especially valuable in, certain contexts, and these could be targeted by research. For example, we may find that the teaching of MFL in Anglophone countries requires different MotS than the teaching of English as an international language in non-Anglophone countries. Likewise, although cultural and educational traditions will continue to prove important in determining the value of MotS, there are likely to be more significant differences in the motivational needs of learners of different ages within a single country than between countries.

To build a knowledge base of use to educators, future research on MotS also needs to maintain conceptual clarity. Can we be sure that what an Iranian teacher or researcher means by (e.g.) ‘act naturally in front of students’ (Guilloteaux Reference Guilloteaux2013) is the same as what a Korean or American teacher or researcher means? It would seem sensible for research to focus on macro-strategies – general enough to have broad common meanings, and so enabling comparisons across settings – and allowing micro-strategies to be determined locally. So for instance a macro-strategy may relate to ‘Using classroom language sensitively’ – whether this means more use of the L2 (as in Saudi university classrooms, see Moskovsky et al. 2013) or less use of the L2 (as in Chinese elementary schools, see Rui & Chew Reference Rui and Chew2014), will depend on the particular pedagogic context under study.

2.2 Alternative approaches to MotS

There have been other attempts to classify and test out MotS in language education which are not based on the Dörnyei (Reference Dörnyei2001) taxonomy. These contribute to our understanding of MotS, but also cast doubt on the value of trying to build a definitive list of motivational teaching behaviours. Bernaus & Gardner (Reference Bernaus and Gardner2008) divided motivational teaching strategies into ‘traditional’ (centred on the teacher's behaviour e.g. ‘I make my students do dictations’) and ‘innovative’ (based on a learner-centred methodology e.g. ‘my students do self-evaluation and co-evaluation’) and investigated their use in Spanish secondary school English classes. The results are significant for suggesting that ‘it is not the actual use of strategies but their perceived use that has an effect on motivation and achievement’ (p. 399). That is, classes of students and teachers do not always agree on whether strategies are being used, and the strategies may only serve to motivate if the students recognize and appreciate them – which they may not do if, for instance, they dislike the teacher's personality or lack any other motives for learning the L2. Meanwhile, in a study of EFL learners in Hungarian schools, Mezei (Reference Mezei, Csizér and Magid2014) found that teachers’ use of motivational strategies did not impact on learners’ motivation directly but was mediated by the Ideal L2 self and self-regulation. In other words, students with a strong Ideal L2 self (see Section 3.2) and self-regulatory capacity were better able to benefit from the MotS, suggesting that teachers need to work on these other learner qualities too.

Maeng & Lee (Reference Maeng and Lee2015) based their investigation on a different MotS framework, using Keller's ARCS model (Attention, Relevance, Confidence, Satisfaction) to identify the strategies used by English teachers in micro-teaching sessions during in-service training in Korea. Their most noteworthy finding was that more experienced teachers used fewer strategies than less experienced teachers, again raising questions about how useful it is to categorize behaviours as positive or negative and then count their frequency. The study also draws our attention to the fact there are other potential frameworks for classifying MotS. Among the most significant is Williams & Burden's (Reference Williams and Burden1997) 12-point list, both because it is designed specifically for language teachers and because it has been elaborated on the basis of social constructivist learning theory. The general education literature provides other options, notably Anderman & Anderman (Reference Anderman and Anderman2010), Schunk, Meece & Pintrich (Reference Schunk, Meece and Pintrich2013) and Wentzel & Brophy (Reference Wentzel and Brophy2014).

Another approach to understanding MotS was taken by Lamb & Wedell (Reference Lamb and Wedell2015), who asked learners in China and Indonesia to reflect on the pedagogical qualities of inspiring language teachers they had had in the past. In this way they aimed to identify those aspects of L2 pedagogy that had a long-term motivational impact on learners, to complement the shorter-term perspective of MotS research. The most frequently cited qualities related to classroom practices, and again some systematic contextual differences were noted e.g. Indonesian learners valued teachers who made lessons entertaining, while Chinese learners were more appreciative of teachers who made lesson content interesting. However, they also found that about a third of comments related not to what teachers did but what they were like as persons, suggesting that personality can have a major long-term impact on individuals’ motivation to learn.

By focusing on the thinking and decision-making of individual teachers, qualitative researchers have begun to open up new perspectives on MotS. Glas (Reference Glas2015: 2), for example, deliberately eschews the use of a priori lists of MotS in order to take ‘a bottom-up approach towards understanding the role of teacher cognition and decision-making with respect to learner motivation’. What she finds is that Chilean teachers of English feel constrained in how far they can motivate their learners, both by limitations on many learners’ pre-existing motivation and by institutional and political restrictions on their ‘spaces for maneuver’ (e.g. in choice of materials, training opportunities, availability of IT). Nevertheless, some individual teachers do express more agency than others in developing their learners’ motivation, despite working in similar contexts. Investigating the beliefs and behaviours of eight successful ‘motivators’ in Indonesia, Lamb, Astuti & Hadisantosa (Reference Lamb, Astuti, Hadisantosa, Apple, Da Silva and Fellner2017) locate the source of their agency in their persistent willingness and ability to empathize with their learners – so while macro-strategies like ‘developing a close rapport with students’ and ‘providing enjoyable activities’ are valued by all, their classroom realization will differ according to language level, age, geographical location and other student factors, as well as the teachers’ own training and experience. They suggest that it can take years of reflective practice, as well as a high level of daily commitment, to develop this nuanced response to the needs of individuals and classes.

The weakness of the strategies approach to understanding motivational teaching has been well articulated by Ushioda (Reference Ushioda and Benson2007); it shares with much traditional L2 motivation research, she argues, a positivist, linear model of educational processes, in particular ‘a tendency to view pedagogical interventions in terms of methods or strategies, and to view motivation as the product in a chain of cause and effect’ (p. 23). Adopting a ‘person-in-context relational approach’ (Ushioda Reference Ushioda, Dörnyei and Ushioda2009), both the studies above, for example, serve to highlight the complexity of MotS and their inseparability from the relations of actual people in contexts of differing constraints and affordances. The categorization and listing of teaching behaviours could give the impression – to novice teachers, educational managers or other stakeholders – that ‘motivating’ is a matter of the teacher deploying the correct strategies, when in fact it is an intensely interactive process, where motivating lessons emerge (sometimes surprisingly) from the coming together and intense mutual engagement from moment to moment of teacher and learners. It is perhaps this capacity for responsiveness, relying on the personal quality of empathy but also built up over years of practice, which defines the successful motivator. This line of argument will be developed further in Section 6.3.

3. Theory-based research on L2 motivation

This section reviews research on L2 pedagogy and motivation which adopts a clearly identifiable theoretical framework. A number of psychological theories have been applied to L2 learning, but only three – SDT, the L2MSS, and social cognitive theory – have inspired the kind of intervention studies which are the main focus of this review. These are described in the following sections; other theories are included in a summary table (Table 3) to help future researchers who wish to develop and test pedagogical approaches that apply their precepts.

Table 3 Prominent theories of motivation applied to language education

3.1 SDT

Originally formulated by Deci &Ryan (Reference Deci and Ryan1985), SDT is one of the most enduring theories of motivation, actively applied in a wide range of human activities from education through healthcare to management, supported by a flourishing website (www.selfdeterminationtheory.org) and popularized in bestselling paperbacks (Pink Reference Pink2009). The theory is perhaps best known for elaborating the binary distinction between intrinsic motivation, where people learn because they enjoy the process of learning, extrinsic motivation, where they are doing it not for its own sake but for some alterior motive, which can be more or less ‘external’ i.e. from very externalized (e.g. to avoid punishment) to very internalized (e.g. doing it because it is part of ‘who you are’). In educational settings, the ideal scenario is where all pupils are intrinsically motivated to learn the subject, because then they will be naturally inclined to study hard, act responsibly and be receptive to new knowledge (Ryan & Deci Reference Ryan and Deci2000). In reality, this is unlikely to be the case, and SDT's key insight for teachers is that with skillful instruction, they can help to make their pupils’ motivation more intrinsic and more internalized. They can do this by trying to ensure that their classrooms satisfy three conditions necessary for human growth and learning: the need for autonomy (a feeling of being able to choose personally meaningful activities), for competence (a sense of gaining mastery of a subject area or skill) and for relatedness (feeling connected to and valued by others engaged in the activity).

As Stroet, Opdenakker & Minnaert's (Reference Stroet, Opdenakker and Minnaert2013) review article shows, there is now a wealth of research in general education showing that ‘need-supportive teaching’ does promote school age learners’ academic motivation, at least when students perceive the teaching in this way (similar to Bernaus & Gardner's Reference Bernaus and Gardner2008, previously mentioned finding about MotS). Attempts to apply SDT to L2 pedagogy were led by Noels and colleagues (Noels, Clément & Pelletier Reference Noels, Clément and Pelletier1999; Noels Reference Noels2001), who found that, in line with the theory, Spanish language teachers with a communicative style which supported their North American students’ sense of autonomy and competence tended to have learners with more intrinsic and internalized forms of motivation. In other words, when students perceive their teachers as giving them choice in what and how to learn, and as providing helpful friendly feedback on progress, they will be encouraged to put more effort into study. Noels (Reference Noels2001) speculated that such effects might be even stronger for school pupils, who are at a more impressionable age and usually interact more intensively with their teachers than university students.

Since those pioneering studies, a number of other researchers working in a variety of global contexts have confirmed the validity of this and other aspects of the theory. Carreira, Ozaki & Maeda (Reference Carreira, Ozaki and Maeda2014) investigated elementary school English classes in Japan and found that the children's perceptions of teacher autonomy support (e.g. the amount of praise and encouragement they give) contributed to their own sense of autonomy, competence and relatedness, and in turn their intrinsic motivation, especially in the middle grades (years 3–4). Pae & Shin (Reference Pae and Shin2011) compared the motivational impact of Korean EFL teachers using communicative methodology – presumed to be more supportive of learners’ autonomy, competence and relatedness – with those having a more traditional teaching style. They found that the extrinsic motivation of students was not affected by which methodology they were exposed to but, as expected, those in communicative classrooms tended to have higher intrinsic motivation and self-confidence. Two other studies had the common aim of enhancing students’ intrinsic motivation, but designed quite different teaching interventions suited to their contrasting higher education (HE) contexts. In the USA, Jones, Llacer-Arrastia & Newbill (Reference Jones, Llacer-Arrastia and Newbill2009) created a series of language learning tasks that enabled their foreign language (FL) students to feel renewed enjoyment for their work; in Japan, Fukuda, Sakata & Takeuchi (Reference Fukuda, Sakata and Takeuchi2011) introduced a ‘guided-autonomy’ syllabus deliberately to boost their students’ sense of autonomy for learning, which they believed was lacking, though they found that this worked only when student–teacher relatedness was also strong.

More recent studies by Noels and colleagues have countered the criticism that SDT has more relevance to individualist ‘western’ societies than to collectivist ‘eastern’ societies. Noels et al. (Reference Noels, Chaffee, Michalyk, Sugita McEown, Csizér and Magid2014), for example, compared the beliefs of Euro-Canadian and Asian-Canadian undergraduates and found no difference in their valuing of autonomy in the language classroom, though interestingly both groups preferred ‘reactive’ to ‘proactive’ autonomy – in other words, both appreciated the active guidance of their teachers throughout their course. Sugita McEown, Noels & Saumure (Reference Sugita McEown, Noels and Saumure2014) found that Canadian students’ perceptions of their teachers’ support for relatedness and competence did correlate with their level of self-determination (i.e. autonomous motivation) for FL learning, yet perception of autonomy support did not. They suggest that in environments where high levels of autonomy already exist, such as at North American colleges, this aspect of pedagogy may not be as salient as in contexts where regular teaching and institutional curricula are more controlling. Other recent research has thrown doubt on the view that more choice is always better – Mozgalina (Reference Mozgalina2015) found that German learners of Russian did not necessarily engage more fully on language learning tasks when they had chosen them themselves.

Although not always referencing SDT, a distinct strand of L2 motivation research has addressed the principle of ‘relatedness’. Assisted by authoritative works on positive group dynamics (Dörnyei & Murphey Reference Dörnyei and Murphey2003; Dörnyei Reference Dörnyei, Cummins and Davison2007), researchers have recently focused on whether and how teachers can motivate learners through deliberately fostering positive relationships in class. For example, both Chang (Reference Chang2010) and Koga (Reference Koga2010) found correlations between the cohesiveness of language classes and the motivation of individual class members. In a continuing programme of research at Japanese universities, Murphey and colleagues have demonstrated the reciprocal nature of the relationship between learner motivation and group dynamics; for example Murphey et al. (Reference Murphey, Falout, Fukuda and Fukada2014) report an action research cycle where lecturers deliberately tried to engineer more positive group dynamics in English major classes through an adaptation of possible selves theory (see Section 3.2); students were asked to generate idealizations of helpful classmates, and these visualizations in turn helped them to behave in cooperative ways with others, evidence – they argue – of positive emotional contagion.

3.2 The L2MSS

3.2.1 Future selves

Mainstream educational psychology has attributed an increasing role to the concepts of ‘self’ and ‘identity’ in motivating learners to put effort into academic study (Oyserman, Bybee & Terry Reference Oyserman, Bybee and Terry2006; Kaplan & Flum Reference Kaplan and Flum2009). In L2 education, this development has been reflected in the currently dominant theoretical framework, the ‘L2 motivational self-system’ (Dörnyei Reference Dörnyei, Dörnyei and Ushioda2009; for more on how this scheme built on earlier L2 motivation theory and on ‘self psychology’, see Dörnyei & Ushioda Reference Dörnyei and Ushioda2011). This proposes that L2 learners’ motivation can in most contexts be best predicted and explained through three main constructs:

  • The Ideal L2 self – the learner's personally valued vision of themselves as a competent user of the L2 in the future (in SDT terms, a strongly internalized motive, encouraging an active aspiration towards the desired goal)

  • The Ought-to L2 self – the learner's conception of what significant others believe they ‘ought to’ be like in the future (in SDT terms, a more extrinsic motive, where avoidance of undesirable end states is prioritized)

  • The L2 learning experience – the learner's attitudes to, and experiences of, the learning process, inside and outside of classrooms.

When investigated empirically in diverse global contexts, the Ideal L2 self and the L2 learning experience are consistently found to be more closely related to actual or intended learning effort than any other motivational constructs (such as integrative or instrumental orientations, cultural interest, parental encouragement – see Taguchi, Magid & Papi Reference Taguchi, Magid, Papi, Dörnyei and Ushioda2009; Kormos, Kiddle & Csizér Reference Kormos, Kiddle and Csizér2011).

From a pedagogical point of view, this framework has two potential sources of insight. First, the fact that the ‘L2 learning experience’ is found to be predictive of motivation to learn, and with younger learners may even be the most important factor (e.g. You & Dörnyei Reference You and Dörnyei2016), foregrounds the role of the teacher and their methodology as a decisive influence on motivation. To date, however, there has been little research exploring the interaction between this component and the future selves, and it is an issue that Ushioda (Reference Ushioda, Mercer and Williams2014: 134) claims is ‘undertheorized’. Second, and in contrast, promising work has already been done, both conceptually and empirically, to explore the pedagogical implications of the ‘Ideal L2 self’. Future selves have their origins in the social role models and media images available to young people in childhood and early adolescence; in many global societies, the valorization of English language skills and the association of the language with desirable cosmopolitan lifestyles means that by the time they enter secondary school, many pupils do already have incipient ideal English-speaking selves (Ryan Reference Ryan2006; Lamb Reference Lamb2012).

Two recent books (Hadfield & Dörnyei Reference Hadfield and Dörnyei2013; Dörnyei & Kubanyiova Reference Dörnyei and Kubanyiova2014) offer teachers practical ideas for developing learners’ L2-related visions, organized in a sequence of six stages:

  1. 1. Initially constructing the desired future self (where it is entirely lacking)

  2. 2. Enhancing the image e.g. through classroom visualization exercises

  3. 3. Making the image plausible e.g. through ensuring learner goals are realistic

  4. 4. Developing an action plan e.g. helping learners set proximal sub-goals

  5. 5. Activating the vision to keep it alive e.g. through regularly engaging with learners’ transportable identities

  6. 6. Counterbalancing the vision e.g. sensitively reminding learners of the dangers of failure.

Some empirical studies already offer evidence that these strategies can work. Magid & Chan (Reference Magid and Chan2012) describe simultaneous interventions with Chinese undergraduate students learning English (as a ‘minor’) in the UK and Hong Kong. The 3–4-month treatment involved guided imagery sessions where students were helped to visualize themselves in future using English, workshops and language counselling sessions where they developed action plans to achieve them, an ‘Ideal L2 self Tree’ activity to elaborate their L2 future selves (see Chan Reference Chan, Csizér and Magid2014). The results were s a strengthening of participants’ ideal L2 selves (see Magid Reference Magid, Csizér and Magid2014), and the strong qualitative endorsement of the sessions by students in interviews. Two semester-long studies with Japanese Majors in English, one an action research cycle involving various tasks designed to elaborate and strengthen their ideal L2 selves (Sampson Reference Sampson2012), the other involving extended role-playing as international ‘experts’ using English (Munezane Reference Munezane, Apple, Da Silva and Fellner2013) also provided positive results in terms of students’ affective response to the class activities themselves and the effects on their future L2 self-guides. Mackay (Reference Mackay, Magid and Csizér2014) conducted a quasi-experiment with upper-intermediate level Catalan students of English, exposing the treatment group to a series of sessions based on Hadfield & Dörnyei (Reference Hadfield and Dörnyei2013); quantitative evidence of success was limited but again qualitative data indicated that the activities were valued and did enrich students’ ideal L2 selves.

While these studies are encouraging, especially in suggesting that a treatment of about three months is sufficient to make a qualitative difference in L2 learner motivation, it should be noted that they are all relatively small-scale and were limited to HE students; we need trials with younger learners, first to find out if visualization and action-planning activities are popular with younger age groups, at what age learners can form realistic images of their future selves, and also to see whether enhanced motivational visions are sustainable over time. Teachers also need to be aware of research suggesting that individual learners may have different capacities for L2 self-imagery (Al-Shehri Reference Al-Shehri, Dörnyei and Ushioda2009; Dörnyei & Chan Reference Dörnyei and Chan2013).

3.2.2 Present selves and identities

One of the criticisms of the L2MSS is that it has directed too much attention towards future-oriented aspects of the self, and ignores the motivational consequences of other self-concepts and more immediately relevant identities. Taylor (Reference Taylor and Ushioda2013) has argued on the basis of her large-scale study of Romanian teenagers, for example, that many school pupils display ‘public selves’ in their English language classroom which are at odds with how they really see themselves as language learners (e.g. some trying to appear coolly detached to peers while actually ambitious, others demonstrating false ambition to the teacher). The result is dysfunctional school classrooms full of dissimulation. Instead, she argues, teenagers are desperate for teachers to treat them as real people with complex personalities and varied extra-curricular interests. Support for this perspective comes from other studies of classrooms where teachers try to do just that – for example Richards (Reference Richards2006) demonstrates how student class participation is enhanced when teachers engage with learners’ ‘transportable identities’, that is their out-of-class personas (e.g. as big sister, Manchester United supporter, player of Minecraft). As Ushioda (Reference Ushioda, Murray, Lamb and Gao2011a) points out, this mirrors to some extent the theme of ‘personalization’ within communicative language teaching, but Luk's (Reference Luk and Canagarajah2005) study shows how ‘off-the-shelf’ communicative tasks which might work in a western context fall flat in a Hong Kong school; instead, learner motivation is enhanced by more traditional teacher-fronted classes where the teacher makes a concerted effort to invoke and perhaps challenge learner identities in a humorous way – for similar examples see Sullivan (Reference Sullivan and Lantolf2000) in Vietnam, Forman (Reference Forman2011) in Thailand, Lamb & Budiyanto (Reference Lamb, Budiyanto and Ushioda2013) in Indonesia.

While the imagining of ideal future selves can provide a motivational impetus for study, the more immediate goal for teachers is perhaps the development of ‘L2-mediated identities’ (Block Reference Block2007) – helping learners to feel comfortable using the L2 and convincing them that it can become a vehicle for expressing their own voice. The Asian studies cited above show that nurturing individual identities can be a group enterprise. Programmes of research in Japanese university classrooms offer further evidence of this. Yashima & Zenuk-Nishide (Reference Yashima and Zenuk-Nishide2008) show how elaborate class role plays can promote the ‘international posture’ of their students and their readiness for authentic communication in English. Falout and colleagues (Falout et al. Reference Falout, Fukada, Murphey, Fukuda, Apple, Da Silva and Fellner2013) report on how their ‘present communities of imagining’ framework enables class members to collectively reprocess past negative educational experiences, share present learning activities and exchange and elaborate visions of their possible English-speaking futures, with quantifiable benefits for their learning motivation.

Researchers taking a more sociological and specifically poststructuralist approach to second language acquisition (SLA) offer other perspectives on learner identity, with different implications for teachers and institutions (Norton & Toohey Reference Norton and Toohey2011). In this view, identity is far more complex and dynamic than is captured in the neat categories of psychological theories, and Norton's term ‘investment’ is offered as a more contextually sensitive, politically responsive complement to the concept of motivation – for example, a learner may be motivated to learn a language yet not invested in a particular class, perhaps because they do not like the teacher's methodology or feel rejected by peers (Norton Reference Norton and Breen2001). Indeed, much of the research in this tradition has highlighted the problems that occur when teachers or their institutions do not recognize or respect the identities of classroom language learners, as in Toohey (Reference Toohey2000), Duff (Reference Duff2002) and Talmy (Reference Talmy2008), all case studies of English as a second language (ESL) learners in North American mainstream schools who reject the stigmatized ‘ESL’ identity imposed upon them and feel disempowered in relation to their ‘native’ peers. Examples from other educational settings are Cambodian women rejecting ESL classes because they felt their domestic and professional identities were not taken into account (Skilton-Sylvester Reference Skilton-Sylvester2002), and Japanese university students being side-lined in Canadian academic seminars (Morita Reference Morita2004). The research is also valuable in bringing to light systematic inequalities related to identity categories like race, gender and sexual orientation that potentially undermine L2 learning opportunities.

As Norton & Toohey (Reference Norton and Toohey2011) acknowledge, this research has so far been successful in revealing structural constraints on L2 learner motivation/investment in western educational contexts but more needs to be done to uncover the ‘problems and triumphs of language teaching’ in postcolonial and multilingual global settings (p. 437), not least the way social class is implicated in the distribution of opportunities to learn and use English in the developing world (Block Reference Block2013). Available evidence suggests, however, that to promote learner investment teachers need to view language not as a static linguistic system but ‘as a social practice in which experiences are organized and identities negotiated’, to offer learners ‘multiple identity positions from which to engage in the language practices of the classroom’ and to be constantly vigilant about ways in which they may be reinforcing subordinate student identities (Norton & Toohey Reference Norton and Toohey2011: 430).

3.3 Social cognitive theory

3.3.1 Self-efficacy

Bandura's (Reference Bandura1997) social cognitive theory emphasized the important role that self-evaluation plays in shaping a learner's approach to learning tasks, and introduced the concept of self-efficacy: ‘one of the most heavily researched and viable psychological constructs in different areas of human functioning’ (Schunk & Pajares Reference Schunk, Pajares, McInerney and Etten2004: 115). A person's self-efficacy beliefs are formed mainly through ‘mastery experiences’ (i.e. successful performance of an activity in the past) as well as from comparison with other people, other people's judgements of their ability, and positive emotions. In general education, learners’ beliefs about their own capability to carry out particular tasks have been found to affect their choice of activities, how much effort they put into them, and what they ultimately achieve (Mills Reference Mills, Mercer and Williams2014). Moreover, there is plenty of evidence that classroom experiences are an important source of self-efficacy beliefs (Wang & Pape Reference Wang and Pape2007) and so what the teacher does (e.g. the type of questions they ask, how they group individuals with other students, the kind of feedback they give) may contribute to shaping each learner's self-efficacy, with potentially long-term impact on their academic success.

Self-efficacy has been found to be a significant component of language learner motivation in a variety of L2 contexts, often mediating the impact of L2-related attitudes on motivated behaviour (e.g. as in Tremblay & Gardner's model, Reference Tremblay and Gardner1995). Recently, studies by Kormos et al. (Reference Kormos, Kiddle and Csizér2011) and Iwaniec (Reference Iwaniec, Csizér and Magid2013) in Chile and Poland respectively have suggested close links between the Ideal L2 self and self-efficacy beliefs i.e. one has to believe in one's capacity to learn and use the L2 in order to visualize a future L2-using self. Self-efficacy has also been shown to predict L2 achievement in school (Hsieh & Kang Reference Hsieh and Kang2010) and university settings (Mills, Pajares & Herron Reference Mills, Pajares and Herron2007). It is also notable that promoting students’ self-confidence is consistently found to be a teaching strategy valued by teachers in a variety of global contexts (see Table 2).

Research describing actual attempts to foster self-efficacy in the L2 classroom, and to monitor its effects on motivation and achievement, is limited but persuasive. In the UK, Graham & Macaro (Reference Graham and Macaro2008) hypothesized that learners’ low self-efficacy with French stemmed partly from difficulty with understanding French speech, and this could be ameliorated by training in a cluster of listening comprehension strategies (e.g. making predictions, identifying key words, recognizing word boundaries). A six-month intervention with two groups of low-intermediate students of French demonstrated lasting gains in L2 listening proficiency and in learner self-efficacy. In the USA, Mills & Peron (Reference Mills and Peron2009) used a ‘global simulation course’ to improve the writing self-efficacy of 148 college students of French – role-playing French characters, they hypothesize, helped the students ‘validate’ their French-speaking identities. In Japan, Matsumoto, Hiromori & Nakayama (Reference Matsumoto, Hiromori and Nakayama2013) tested the impact of instruction in key reading strategies (e.g. identifying the main idea, adjusting reading speed to test difficulty) on the motivation and beliefs of 360 Japanese undergraduate students of English (as a ‘minor’), producing positive results for extrinsic and intrinsic motivation and reading self-efficacy.

3.3.2 L2 self-confidence and self-esteem

Two related constructs have also been associated with L2 motivation and achievement. ‘Linguistic self-confidence’ has its origins in social psychological approaches to L2 education (Clément Reference Clément, Giles, Robinson and Smith1980) when it was hypothesized that students who experience close contact with the L2 community would have stronger confidence in their capacity to learn. More recent research in FL contexts suggests the relationship can be reversed, i.e. that more confident learners seek out intercultural contact, which in turn can improve L2 attitudes and lead to motivated learning behaviour (Csizér & Kormos Reference Csizér and Kormos2008). As Sampasivam & Clément argue (Reference Sampasivam, Clément, Mercer and Williams2014) the relationship is likely to be reciprocal, and offers a reason for educators to promote intercultural contact as a means of improving L2 learner motivation, whether directly through short-term study abroad projects (e.g. Hernandez Reference Hernandez2010; Reynolds-Case Reference Reynolds-Case2013) or through computer-mediated communication (see Section 4.1).

The more general construct of ‘self-esteem’ (the overall estimation of one's worth as an individual) has a quite different history in L2 education (Rubio Reference Rubio, Mercer and Williams2014). The threat that L2 learning can bring to a person's self-esteem (e.g. through depriving one of the capacity for self-expression) has fueled the popularity of humanistic approaches to language teaching, embodied in ‘alternative’ methods like Suggestopedia (Lozanov Reference Lozanov1978) as well as in teachers’ handbooks deliberately aimed at protecting or enhancing learner self-esteem (e.g. Rinvolucri Reference Rinvolucri2002; Andres & Arnold Reference Andres and Arnold2009). However, empirical evidence of the motivational effectiveness of these teaching techniques is lacking.

3.4 Further theories of motivation

A number of other theories, prominent in mainstream education, have attracted the attention of L2 researchers and been found to have explanatory power for L2 motivation. However, with one or two exceptions, they have not yet been systematically applied and empirically evaluated in L2 education. These are included in Table 3, along with references both to the original authors of the theories and to L2 educational publications which explain them.

4. Empirical studies of teaching innovations

This section reviews empirical studies in which the rationale for the research derives primarily from the motivational potential of an innovative practice or teaching approach, rather than from theory (though theory may well be invoked in support). Topics are presented in a rough order of frequency of publication in major journals over the past decade. From this it will quickly be noted that computer assisted language learning (CALL)-related research is a dominant theme, though this might be slightly exaggerated by the existence of several specialist journals dedicated to this aspect of L2 pedagogy.

The majority of studies share a common basic research methodology – a pedagogical innovation or intervention is conducted with a non-generalizable sample of students, followed by a survey measuring learner attitudes towards or feelings about the specific L2 learning experience and/or L2 learning generally. In some studies this is triangulated with other kinds of data, usually interviews; observational data, such as evaluating learners’ classroom behaviour (as in some of the MotS research detailed here), is rare. Even rarer are intervention studies with pre- and post-tests of motivation. Randomized control trials are almost unheard of in L2 motivation research. Another general characteristic of the research is that it is short-term – usually reporting on an innovation lasting one semester or less – and the results might therefore be vulnerable to a novelty effect (as acknowledged in many studies). In the descriptions below, research methodology is only mentioned if it departs from the norm, or if the evidence it produces is particularly strong or weak.

4.1 CALL/digital technology

Developments in digital technology are probably the most prolific source of innovation in L2 teaching methodology in contemporary times, at least in western or developed world contexts, and the motivational properties of each innovation are usually considered an important aspect of its instructional qualities – after all, if learners do not like the innovation, then it will not be taken up, however great its capacity for language learning or acquisition.

Indeed, in their systematic review Macaro, Handley & Walter (Reference Macaro, Handley and Walter2012) comment that the evidence for CALL promoting positive learner attitudes towards L2 learning is stronger than the evidence for beneficial linguistic outcomes.

For reasons of space it is not possible to review all studies reporting motivational impact from the use of CALL in L2 courses – readers are referred to recent authoritative review articles (Macaro et al. Reference Macaro, Handley and Walter2012; Stockwell Reference Stockwell and Ushioda2013; Golonka et al. Reference Golonka, Bowles, Frank, Richardson and Freynik2014; Bodnar et al. 2016). Instead, what follows is a synthesis of the main motivational benefits identified in studies of CALL innovations, which should be read in cognizance of Stockwell's (Reference Stockwell and Ushioda2013) point that students’ readiness to engage with new learning technologies will always be a product of a complex range of interacting factors, such as their familiarity with IT at home, the teacher's enthusiasm for and skills in IT, and the nature of the learners’ pre-existing motivation for learning a particular language.

4.1.1 Greater autonomy and individualization

One of the original rationales for CALL was its capacity for enhancing learners’ control over the learning process and hence for tailoring courses of instruction to learners’ individual needs (Warschauer Reference Warschauer and Warschauer1996). There is evidence that this potential is being realized. For example, Bhattacharya & Chauhan (Reference Bhattacharya and Chauhan2010) report how a blogging project enhanced their Indian postgraduate students’ autonomous motivation to write, as well as their metacognitive skills. In a controlled experiment, Sanprasert (Reference Sanprasert2010) shows how the introduction of an online course management system, with space for online communication and collaboration, improved her Thai students’ ability and willingness to learn independently. The individual convenience and personalization made available through mobile learning (Kim et al. Reference Kim, Rueckert, Kim and Seo2013) and ‘ubiquitous’ learning (Jung Reference Jung2014) have also been shown to increase learner satisfaction, though Kukulska-Hulme, Norris & Donohue (Reference Kukulska-Hulme, Norris and Donohue2015) warn that teachers need to ensure all learners in a class have access to similar devices and are willing for them to be used for educational purposes. On the basis of their own literature review, Golonka et al. (Reference Golonka, Bowles, Frank, Richardson and Freynik2014) argue that advances in automatic speech recognition software – offering finely tuned individual feedback for learning pronunciation – is one of the most promising uses of CALL, with evidence of learners showing increased confidence in their oral skills, when used regularly.

4.1.2 Enhanced opportunities for communication

IT can greatly expand the opportunities available for learners to try out their L2. This can be especially valuable in educational contexts where traditional classroom methodology does not offer much scope for communicative practice and few opportunities exist outside the classroom. Freiermuth & Huang (Reference Freiermuth and Huang2012) for example describe how an ‘intercultural online synchronic chat task’ involving undergraduate learners of English in Japan and Taiwan was very positively evaluated by the participants, though they stress that the intense L2 practice it engendered was a product not just of the technology but of careful task design. Even in a multilingual European context, Jauregi et al. (Reference Jauregi, de Graaff, van den Bergh and Kriz2012) show the motivational benefits of synchronous computer-mediated communication as the Czech learners of Dutch in their quasi-experiment engaged with enthusiasm in native-speaker–non-native-speaker (NS-NNS) video-conferencing and reported deep satisfaction with the intensive speaking practice this provided (see also Wu, Yen & Marek Reference Wu, Yen and Marek2011).

4.1.3 Identity development

Block (Reference Block2007), Kramsch (Reference Kramsch2009), Norton & Toohey (Reference Norton and Toohey2011) and others have stressed the potential of digital technologies for ‘trying out new and alternative identities and modes of self-presentation . . . without posing a threat to students’ real-world identities and private selves’ (Ushioda Reference Ushioda2011b: 207). The empirical evidence for this phenomenon remains thin, however. Klimanova & Dembovskaya (Reference Klimanova and Dembovskaya2013) show through discourse analysis how American learners of Russian as a heritage language asserted their identities as authentic Russian speakers in a two-month-long tele-collaboration project with Russian learners of English, though there is no information on whether this impacted their longer-term investment in the language. Gleason & Suvorov (Reference Gleason and Suvorov2012) found that the use of Wimba Voice for asynchronous oral language learning tasks, while popular with students, did not appear in the short-term to contribute to their future L2 selves.

4.1.4 Recognizing and utilizing learners’ existing IT skills

Henry (Reference Henry and Ushioda2013) offers a distinct motivational rationale for the use of CALL in L2 classrooms. In many contemporary societies, L2 learners are at least as ‘tech-savvy’ as their teachers, often rather more so, and therefore ‘teachers of English would profit from a better understanding of young people's leisure time activities’ (Henry Reference Henry and Ushioda2013: 151) in order to create classroom activities that feel more authentic for learners i.e. more like what they do out-of-class, and with scope for creativity and meaning-making in the L2. Examples in the literature include Freiermuth's (in press) smartphone ‘treasure hunt’ using geocaching software with Japanese undergraduates, and Wehner, Gump & Downey's (Reference Wehner, Gump and Downey2011) venture into the virtual world of Second Life with American students of Spanish, which both report highly positive affective responses from participants. There is likely to be a surge in publications on the use of digital games and ‘gamification’ in language learning in coming years.

To round off this section, the literature makes clear that CALL has the capacity to demotivate too, and such cases are probably under-reported for the obvious reason that innovators may prefer not to broadcast them. Stockwell (Reference Stockwell and Ushioda2013) cites two studies (Chen & Cheng Reference Chen and Cheng2008; Castellano, Mynard & Rubesch Reference Castellano, Mynard and Rubesch2011) which had adverse effects, and ‘give us insights into what is necessary to avoid inadvertently damaging learners’ motivation to study using technology’ (p. 165); these include making sure that learners have the necessary IT skills or L2 proficiency to use the technology, and providing sufficient guidance and support throughout the learning process.

4.2 Content-based instruction

CLIL, in its various forms, is an increasingly common feature of national language (and especially English language) curricula worldwide, particularly popular with curriculum designers in Spain and in elite private education systems in the Far East. In addition to its efficacy in promoting language acquisition, its motivational impact has also been subject to empirical investigation in recent years. Doiz, Lasagabaster & Sierra (Reference Doiz, Lasagabaster and Sierra2014) built on an earlier study by Lasagabaster (Reference Lasagabaster2011) to compare the L2 motivation of 393 Spanish secondary school students of English divided into CLIL and non-CLIL groups in the first and third school years; they found the former to have significantly higher intrinsic motivation, instrumental orientation and interest in the L2 (though also higher anxiety levels). Mearns (Reference Mearns2012) reports motivational gains from an experiment with English secondary school learners of German in the UK. Huang (Reference Huang2011) used Spada & Fröhlich's (Reference Spada and Fröhlich1995) Communicative Orientation of Language Teaching Observation scheme to compare the behaviour patterns of the same 26 Taiwanese six-year-olds in two content-based language instruction classes and two direct language instruction classes, and found they participated more actively in the former. Seikkula-Leino (Reference Seikkula-Leino2007) found a slight motivational advantage among CLIL vs non-CLIL primary school children in Finland, while Lasagabaster & Beloqui (Reference Lasagabaster and Beloqui2015) found stronger intrinsic and integrative motives for learning English among CLIL-taught pupils in a Spanish primary school.

Although these results are encouraging, it should be noted that the studies conducted so far are all of modest scale and do not control for intervening variables (except gender). What is more, there are some less encouraging reports of practice in other global contexts. Coleman (Reference Coleman2011) argued on the basis of a survey of English ‘bilingual education’ in three Asian countries that it was being implemented at the behest of ambitious school administrators and parents, with little concern for quality control, and could have damaging effects on young people's academic motivation and achievement. Kuchah (Reference Kuchah and Ushioda2013) raises similar questions about English-medium education in Francophone West Africa. The negative motivational impact of being educated in a second or third language is well documented in some multilingual developing world settings (e.g. Mohanty et al. Reference Mohanty, Panda, Phillipson and Skuttnab-Kangas2009), and more substantial research is warranted on its effect in privileged urban educational settings too.

4.3 Intercultural content

Calls for a greater integration of culture and language teaching have increased in recent years, partly from an enhanced awareness of the interconnectedness of language and culture but also from a belief that such an approach would, by enriching syllabus content, be more motivating for learners (e.g. Kramsch Reference Kramsch2009). Two recent studies have tested this hypothesis in relation to MFL teaching in Anglophone countries. Acheson, Nelson & Luna (Reference Acheson, Nelson and Luna2015) report a two-year study with 391 students of Spanish in two American secondary schools in which an experimental group were given direct instruction and practice activities in intercultural competence during their Spanish language lessons – post-tests revealed significant gains in their attitudes to Hispanic languages and people and greater motivation for L2 learning. However, Peiser & Jones (Reference Peiser and Jones2013) suggest the issue is complex. In a mixed-method study of over 700 UK secondary school pupils, they found that their attitudes towards having more ‘intercultural understanding’ (IU) in the MFL curriculum were influenced by a range of societal, social, cognitive and institutional factors (e.g. more academic groups showed less interest in IU) and wrote ‘we cannot conclude from our study that a greater focus on IU is a panacea to motivation issues in the MFL curriculum’ (p. 354).

The cultural content of ‘authentic’ materials has long been recognized as having motivational potential (Peacock Reference Peacock1997). In the case of English as a global language, though, the question arises as to which culture is relevant, given that the language is used across a multitude of cultural and national borders, and what kind of communication is authentic, given that native speakers make up a minority of actual users (Pinner Reference Pinner2014). On the one hand, it can be argued that learners may be motivated by their (and their teachers) being convinced that local varieties of English are acceptable, even desirable, models to aspire to (Jenkins Reference Jenkins2007) and that NNS-NNS communication in international settings is just as authentic as NS-NS communication (Hall Reference Hall2013). On the other hand, there is empirical evidence that many learners still put a high value on prestige, NS varieties of English (Pan & Block Reference Pan and Block2011; Sung Reference Sung2014) and can be motivated by exposure to NS teachers (Lamb & Budiyanto Reference Lamb, Budiyanto and Ushioda2013), a perception that still drives employment practices in some education systems (see Hu & Mackay Reference Hu and McKay2012).

4.4 Designing motivating tasks

Just as tasks had become a central unit of analysis in the study of SLA, Dörnyei (Reference Dörnyei and Robinson2002) argued that they could become a logical focus of study for a more situated, education-friendly approach to L2 motivation. As it turns out, there has been a rather limited amount of research conducted on task motivation in subsequent years. Broadly speaking, a distinction can be made between studies which have examined the interaction between task design features and learners’ actual engagement on the task, and those looking at the potential longer-term motivational effects of repeated use of a certain type of task. In the latter category can be included research studies which have empirically tested the assumption made by so many practising teachers in the ‘communicative’ era, viz. that learners of all ages can be motivated through the use of intrinsically stimulating and more personalized activities in class. Thus, small-scale studies by Ajibade & Ndububa (Reference Ajibade and Ndububa2008) in Nigerian secondary schools and Chou (Reference Chou2014) in Taiwanese primary schools provide some evidence for the positive motivational impact of using culturally appropriate word games, songs and stories in English classes. In an innovative research article, Kao & Oxford (Reference Kao and Oxford2014) argue for the motivational properties of hip hop music in the English language classroom based on the first author's personal experience as father and teacher. Shaaban (Reference Shaaban2006) describes how cooperative (‘jigsaw’) reading tasks improved the motivation to read of Year 5 students of English in Lebanon, while Lo & Hyland (Reference Lo and Hyland2007) report an action research study in a ‘well-established’ Hong Kong primary school which produced gains in young learners’ motivation to write English by (a) introducing more personally relevant topics and (b) providing new audiences beyond the teacher.

The more micro-level analysis of learner engagement on language learning tasks was pioneered by Julkunen (Reference Julkunen, Dörnyei and Schmidt2001), Dörnyei (Reference Dörnyei and Robinson2002) and Dörnyei & Kormos (Reference Dörnyei and Kormos2000), whose work brought to light the formidably complex range of situational factors, learner characteristics and dynamic processes which shape any individual's motivation on task. Indeed, this very complexity may have deterred further empirical inquiry (though see Dörnyei & Tseng Reference Dörnyei, Tseng, Mackey and Polio2009 and Yanguas Reference Yanguas2011) until recently, when complex dynamic systems theory has offered a promising new frame of reference in which the learner's task engagement could be viewed as a complex system within an ‘attractor state’ subject to various ‘control parameters’ like task topic, learner proficiency level, relations with peers, degree of teacher support, and intended outcomes. Some of the most interesting work is on oral tasks and the learner's ‘willingness to communicate’ (e.g. MacIntyre & Serroul Reference MacIntyre, Serroul, Dörnyei, MacIntyre and Henry2015), which is beyond the remit of this review, but recent analyses of the dynamics of task (Poupore Reference Poupore2013) and lesson motivation (Waninge, Dörnyei & de Bot Reference Waninge, Dörnyei and de Bot2014) suggest this approach may eventually produce insights of value to teachers.

4.5 Increasing the relevance of the L2

A number of studies have looked at whether enhancing learners’ perception of the L2’s relevance for their lives can increase their motivation to learn it. In a rare example of a randomized control trial, Donitsa-Schmidt, Inbar & Shohamy (Reference Donitsa-Schmidt, Inbar and Shohamy2004) report how teaching the oral form of Arabic, rather than the literary form, increased the motivation of Israeli Jewish primary school learners, because it was deemed to be more useful for pragmatic and cultural reasons. Fryer et al. (Reference Fryer, Ozono, Carter, Nakao and Anderson2014) argue that utility value also plays an important motivational role among the large numbers of tertiary level students worldwide who are forced to study English on non-elective courses, and Johnson's (Reference Johnson, Apple, Da Silva and Fellner2013) longitudinal study showed that awareness of its instrumental value for work was the single most important motivator among second year Japanese university students of Engineering, suggesting that teaching should try to reinforce the relevance by, for example, focusing on vocationally relevant genres. Meanwhile in Anglophone countries, perceptions of low relevance are claimed to be a major reason for learner demotivation (Coleman Reference Coleman2009). Taylor & Marsden (Reference Taylor and Marsden2014) describe an experiment in UK secondary schools in which 13–14 year-olds were exposed to one of two different ‘advocacy’-based interventions: a panel of external speakers who had all benefited from their knowledge of an L2, or a lesson in the language with an external tutor who had a strong message to convey about their experience with that language. Results showed that participation in the intervention did have a modest beneficial effect on uptake of the L2 in later years, and this was related to more positive perception of its personal relevance.

4.6 Alternative forms of assessment

The relationship between motivation and assessment is under-researched in mainstream education as in L2 education; a popular view in the teaching profession is that traditional summative pen and paper tests can provide some short-term motivation for more confident pupils (see Huang Reference Huang2012, for evidence of this effect in Taiwanese English classes) but can also undermine long-term intrinsic motivation to study by encouraging all to value grades more than new knowledge and skills. Indeed, high-stakes testing regimes feature as a prominent demotivating factor in some research (see Section 5). Some empirical evidence is emerging on the motivational impact of more learner-centred forms of assessment. The introduction of self-assessment has been shown to boost learner motivation in Japanese university settings through a structured project (Kato Reference Kato2009) and with intermediate Iranian EFL learners through written journals (Birjandi & Tamjid Reference Birjandi and Tamjid2010). Lam (Reference Lam2014) presents testimony and a theoretical model of how portfolio assessment of L2 writing can promote student motivation and self-regulated learning. Other research is examining how formative feedback can be optimized to promote learners’ short- and long-term motivation to write in the L2. Busse (Reference Busse2013) for example found that UK university students of German had a strong preference for informational feedback expressed in an encouraging tone, though in a controlled experiment with Dutch HE students, Duijnhouwer, Prins & Stokking (Reference Duijnhouwer, Prins and Stokking2012) found no effect of such feedback on learners’ L2 writing self-efficacy.

4.7 Motivating boys

Gender has long featured as a significant individual difference in L2 motivation research, with females generally scoring higher for integrativeness and self-efficacy (e.g. Williams, Burden & Lanvers Reference Williams, Burden and Lanvers2002), for having ideal L2 selves (e.g. Henry & Cliffordson Reference Henry and Cliffordson2013) for intended learning effort (e.g. Ryan Reference Ryan, Dörnyei and Ushioda2009), for participation in foreign exchange programmes (Taylor Reference Taylor2000) and so on. In fact, there is a common perception globally that language is a ‘feminine subject’, taught by female teachers. Debate continues over whether the underlying source of these differences is biological, social or educational (Carr & Pauwels Reference Carr and Pauwels2006) but there have been several noteworthy attempts to overcome the male deficit. One approach has been to separate the sexes for language classes, in the belief that L2 use invokes teenagers’ gendered identities in ways that other subjects do not. Results of actual experiments are mixed. Chambers (Reference Chambers2005), for example, found in a UK comprehensive school pilot project that pupils’ overall enjoyment and confidence increased but teachers complained of difficulties teaching mixed-ability classes (they were normally streamed by ability) and of dealing with bad behaviour in all-male classes. By contrast, in Canada, Kissau, Quach & Wang (Reference Kissau, Quach and Wang2009) found that boys flourished in all-male classes but girls did not enjoy their all-female classes. A different approach was taken in the Score in French project in a UK secondary school (McCall Reference McCall2011), which unashamedly aimed to make the language classroom more ‘masculine’ by creating a French curriculum based on football. Through sensitive task design, exploiting the appeal not just of the game but of certain French players, the pilot project was successful in increasing the L2 motivation of both boys and girls.

5. Demotivation

Such is the global allure of English that, in urban settings at least, few young people enter secondary school without a modicum of desire to learn it, and for many the desire is deeply felt and persuasive (Motha & Lin Reference Motha and Lin2014). So, as Littlejohn (Reference Littlejohn2008) has argued, in some ways the English teacher's main task is not to motivate learners but to prevent their demotivation, that is, the gradual loss of a pre-existing motivation to learn. Moreover, it is a well-established truth that more intrinsic forms of motivation tend to decline during the early years of secondary school (see Yeung, Lau & Nie Reference Yeung, Lau and Nie2011, for confirmation of this trend for English language learners in Singapore) and learner motivation is particularly vulnerable at transition points like primary to secondary (Burns et al. Reference Burns, Morris-Adams, Garton and Copland2013) or foundation course to HE (Woodrow Reference Woodrow and Ushioda2013). It is not surprising then that during the 2000s a research literature began to appear on demotivation in L2, ably summarized in Dörnyei & Ushioda (Reference Dörnyei and Ushioda2011).

As they make clear, some sources of learner demotivation are beyond the immediate control of teachers. For example, societal discourses may negate the value of learning an L2 (e.g. see Lanvers & Coleman (Reference Lanvers and Coleman2013) on UK media attitudes towards L2 learning); nationally imposed curricula can curtail the freedom of teachers and pupils to pursue local interests in class (e.g. Wedell Reference Wedell2009); high-stakes assessment regimes can raise anxiety levels and displace intrinsic motives for learning with a pursuit of grades and qualifications (e.g. Cheng, Watanabe & Curtis Reference Cheng, Watanabe and Curtis2004); low investment in language education by governments results in poorly resourced classrooms and poorly rewarded teachers (e.g. in many developing countries, see Bennell & Akyeampong Reference Bennell and Akyeampong2007). However, there is also plenty of evidence to suggest that learners are sometimes demotivated by what happens inside the language classroom. In fact, in some, studies teachers have been clearly identified as the main source of demotivation (e.g. Falout & Maruyama Reference Falout and Maruyama2004). A non-exhaustive list of negative practices identified in recent research is presented in Table 4.

Table 4 A selection of demotivating aspects of L2 classroom teaching

It is important to note another point emphasized by Dörnyei & Ushioda (Reference Dörnyei and Ushioda2011); demotivation is as complex as motivation, in that learners react differently to the same classroom procedures, and the same learner may react differently to the same stimulus depending on their stage of learning, their mood, the disposition of peers and so on. One-off studies have already shown that different factors will tend to demotivate learners of high versus low proficiency (Falout & Maruyama Reference Falout and Maruyama2004) and high versus low pre-existing motivation (Sakai & Kikuchi Reference Sakai and Kikuchi2009). Longitudinal studies suggest that for some learners a spiral of decline can develop, when frustrating classroom experiences engender negative attitudes which discourage extra-curricular practice, lowering confidence further, and so on (e.g. Lamb Reference Lamb and Coleman2011; Busse & Walter Reference Busse and Walter2013). Other learners seem able to have negative classroom experiences without letting them affect their overall longer-term motivation (Campbell & Storch Reference Campbell and Storch2011). As Dörnyei & Ushioda (Reference Dörnyei and Ushioda2011: 156) write, ‘the current shift towards socio-dynamic perspectives on L2 motivation research . . . seems well suited to investigating the “dark side” of motivation’, because of its capacity to identify patterns of interaction over time between different elements in the classroom environment and beyond. Very recent studies which have adopted this approach do indeed present vivid pictures of fluctuation in learner motivation, from the scale of one academic year (Kikuchi in press) to single language lessons (Waninge et al. Reference Waninge, Dörnyei and de Bot2014); it is perhaps too soon to draw strong pedagogical implications, beyond a heightened awareness of complexity and dynamism, and of the importance of ‘initial conditions’ i.e. getting off to a good start.

One further issue raised by research on demotivation is that of teacher motivation, for evidence suggests the two are closely connected. The teacher factors which repeatedly appear in lists of potential demotivators (cf. Dörnyei & Ushioda Reference Dörnyei and Ushioda2011; Kikuchi Reference Kikuchi, Apple, Da Silva and Fellner2013) – monotonous methodology, low enthusiasm for the subject, unfriendly demeanour, lack of attention to individual needs – are often a symptom of an underlying lack of work motivation. Conversely, inspiring teachers are often those who are highly motivated themselves, since it takes great energy and commitment to offer constant variety, and show unwavering enthusiasm, approachability and adaptability (Lamb & Wedell Reference Lamb and Wedell2015). In a rare attempt to link learner motivation with teacher motivation, Bernaus, Wilson & Gardner (Reference Bernaus, Wilson and Gardner2009) found that the relationship was indeed mediated by teachers’ use (or not) of MotS, concluding: ‘Our results suggest that both students and teachers need to be motivated. If teachers are motivated, students are more actively involved in class activities and feel more motivated’ (p. 33). The few published studies that have looked directly at L2 teacher motivation (e.g. Karavas Reference Karavas2010, in Greece; Aydin Reference Aydin2012, in Turkey; Wyatt Reference Wyatt2013, in Oman) portray it as an area of concern, both for its own sake and for the knock-on effects of learner motivation.

6. Summary

This review has identified well over 200 pieces of research published in academically respectable outlets in the last 15 years that have addressed the motivational dimension of language teaching. The research can be classified into four broad types: investigations of the strategies that teachers deliberately use to motivate classes of students, a line of research that is gradually growing in sophistication (Section 2); studies that apply theoretical insights about human motivation to L2 educational contexts, and here SDT and the L2 motivational self-system dominate (Section 3); studies that have described the motivational impact of various pedagogical innovations, the majority of which have involved the introduction of learning technology (Section 4); and research into the phenomenon of learner demotivation (Section 5), often located in Anglophone countries or in Japan. It is time to pause for reflection: what can we state with confidence about the effects of teaching on L2 learner motivation? What should teacher educators do with this knowledge? And what areas of new knowledge should future researchers prioritize?

6.1 What do we know?

6.1.1 Teachers can motivate

Danziger (Reference Danziger1997) points out that ‘motivation’ as a field of psychology has its origins in the expansion of mass education in the early decades of the twentieth century, and the recognition that people, whether children in classrooms or consumers on the high street, could not be manipulated by force; instead ‘[o]ne had to play upon what individuals wanted, what they were interested in, what they privately wished for’ (p. 113). We now have sufficient research evidence to suggest that language teachers are able to influence their learners’ motivation, both for better and worse. This probably accords with the experiences of most teachers, who will have registered the impact of their work on their learners’ views and feelings about the subject. The bulk of the research evidence, however, is indirect, deriving from non-controlled studies of learner attitudes in response to some kind of short-term innovation in teaching; there have been very few controlled experiments measuring the impact of teachers, and few that have included the direct observation of learner behaviour, in class or outside. What is more, the focus has almost always been on immediate outcomes. There is as yet very little evidence about the long-term impact of teachers on learner motivation, apart from in retrospective accounts by learners recalling favoured teachers in the past (e.g. Shoaib & Dörnyei Reference Shoaib, Dörnyei, Benson and Nunan2005; Lamb & Wedell Reference Lamb and Wedell2015).

6.1.2 The personal is paramount

The aspects of teacher behaviour which appear to have the most motivational impact on language learners are those that relate to the human side of teaching. In the MotS research, the strategies most consistently valued by teachers as well as learners emphasize the connections between people: learners have to respect the teacher as a professional, and ideally like them as a person; the teacher has to understand the needs, goals and desires that learners bring to class from their lives outside and from prior experiences of learning; they have to create a group dynamic which accommodates and excites all of them; they need the professional dedication to continually respond to, and sometimes try to change, those needs, goals and desires. Teacher–student and peer relations have been found to be important to learner motivation in general education (Wentzel Reference Wentzel, Wentzel and Wigfield2009), but it is possible that they are even more central to language teaching, which necessarily involves more frequent interpersonal communication.

6.1.3 Methods matter, but so does context

To maintain good relations while also ensuring that sufficient language learning takes place to give learners a sense of progress, takes considerable technical skill. Much of the evidence reviewed here relates to creative classroom practice, techniques for providing learners with stimulating and satisfying tasks. Novelty and variety may themselves be motivating, but also presenting classroom activities so that learners know why they are doing them, setting just the right level of challenge, and providing informative feedback afterwards are all key motivational skills. These actions must always be made to fit the precise context too – appropriate to the local educational culture, meeting broader curriculum goals, matching the preferences and interests of particular learner groups, and wherever possible adapted to individual learner needs, wants and identities (present and future).

6.1.4 Learner control

As if all this was not difficult enough, the paradox of good teaching is that it must be done while allowing learners to feel in control of events. It is not a coincidence that the common theme running through all the motivational theories reviewed here is that people need to feel they are learning for their own reasons, in a self-determined way, autonomously, efficaciously, in pursuit of an ideal self perhaps, or at least an internalized goal. The research evidence in favour of ‘autonomy-supportive’ teaching in general education is increasingly persuasive (Reeve, Deci & Ryan Reference Reeve, Deci, Ryan, McInerney and Van Etten2004). In L2 education, the research evidence is mostly negative in nature; that is, experienced teachers and motivated learners do not mention reward schemes or other overt forms of manipulation as being likely to motivate learners; entertaining classroom activities might be valued, but only when offered strategically as part of a course of constructive learning. This is the reason why some commentators have suggested that the key question teachers need to ask themselves is not ‘how can I motivate my students?’ but ‘How can I create the conditions under which students will be able to motivate themselves?’ (Reeve et al. Reference Reeve, Deci, Ryan, McInerney and Van Etten2004: 53).

6.2 The role of teacher educators

Indeed, this might be one of the key questions for novice teachers to answer while in training. If success in language learning is partly determined by the learner's motivation – and research suggests that it is associated with 18–33% of the variation in achievement (Masgoret & Gardner Reference Masgoret and Gardner2003) – and the teacher is an important influence on that motivation, then it is reasonable to argue that the motivational dimension should be an essential component in any course of training for language teachers. In fact, Wigfield, Cambria & Eccles (Reference Wigfield, Cambria, Eccles and Ryan2012: 474) argue that ‘with the current press for more assessments and evaluation of student performance, and teacher and principal accountability for student performance on those tests’ it becomes even more imperative that motivation research makes a contribution to teachers’ professional development.

However, the existing research also tells us that there are many cultural and contextual factors which mediate the motivational impact of teacher behaviour. What works in one educational context may not work in another. The moderate results usually obtained in L2 motivation research also reminds us of the significance of individual learner differences, since pedagogical innovations rarely gain universal approval – what works for one learner may not work for another. Therefore, language teachers-in-training need to become aware of general principles of L2 motivational practice – the four earlier points (6.1.1 – 6.1.4) are a place to start – and of sets of available strategies, as so clearly presented in Dörnyei (Reference Dörnyei2001); yet they also need to be made keenly aware that the effective operationalization of principles and strategies is contingent on local conditions and the needs, goals and desires of the actual persons involved.

This matches what we now know about the complexities of teacher learning. It is recognized that training novice teachers or persuading experienced teachers to use a pre-ordained set of instructional strategies is a recipe for failure if the strategies do not align with teachers’ underlying beliefs and values (Borg Reference Borg2003; Kubanyiova Reference Kubanyiova2012). Even the four broad ‘truths’ about motivation elucidated here may present a challenge to teachers’ existing beliefs. For example, it is possible that many language teachers have what might be termed a ‘fixed mindset’ (Dweck Reference Dweck1999), believing that learners either are or are not motivated for languages, just as they either have or do not have L2 aptitude, and that this is not open to change. Other teachers may resist the notion that warm personal relations are important for motivation, preferring instead to maintain a disciplinary distance and authority. They may well have firm views on what practices ‘work’ in the classroom and be resistant to methodological innovation. And even when convinced that learner motivation is their own responsibility, there is the danger that they will assume too dominant a role, undermining learners’ internalized motivation for the subject. In their motivational intervention study in the USA, Turner, Warzon & Christensen (Reference Turner, Warzon and Christensen2011) found that maths teachers’ ability to reflect on their own beliefs and understand the rationale behind motivational practice was crucial to their ability to change.

In addition to motivational awareness-raising, teachers might benefit from training in recognizing learner motivation and demotivation. As argued above, successful practice involves constantly responding to learners’ needs, goals and desires, over the timescale of lessons and courses. This, in turn, requires the ability to infer what learners dislike and predict what they might like. This can be developed through training in classroom observation, the use of devices like needs analysis questionnaires, lesson reaction slips and course evaluation surveys, journal writing and other techniques of reflective practice (Farrell Reference Farrell2008).

6.3 Directions for future research

This final section will consider how research on the motivational dimension of language teaching can be improved in quality, so we can have more confidence in its results, and can be made more useful to the language teaching profession so that as educators we can more readily provide the conditions for students to motivate themselves.

6.3.1 Teachers’ beliefs, motivation and practice

As discussed above, teachers’ beliefs and values profoundly influence their practice, and would mediate any attempts to make their teaching more motivating. It is true that the MotS research has brought important insights into the specific strategies that teachers value, but because it has largely been done through surveys based on researchers’ own conceptions of motivational practice, we cannot be sure that they represent teachers’ own core beliefs about how to motivate learners. We also lack evidence about how teachers’ conceptions of motivation are reflected in their practice. A recent study by Muñoz & Ramirez (Reference Muñoz and Ramirez2015), for instance, using an SDT theoretical framework, found that Colombian university language centre teachers appeared to recognize the motivational value of giving learners choice and introducing meaningful language tasks, yet rarely did either during observed classes. Kubanyiova's (Reference Kubanyiova2006) study of a training course designed to help teachers move towards a more ‘motivational teaching practice’ found that, although the participants were enthusiastic about the course and recognized the value of the proposed MotS, they did not adopt them in their teaching; she ascribed this ‘failure’ to both contextual constraints and to the nature of the participants’ own motivation to teach. Yet Feryok & Oranje (Reference Feryok and Oranje2015) show that even when teachers’ main focus is on other aspects of pedagogy, such as an intercultural class project, a desire to interest and motivate learners is a recurring motif in their lesson planning and moment-to-moment classroom decisions,

If we want to understand why and how teachers adopt and adapt MotS, researchers need to engage with the literature on teacher cognition, and conduct in-depth qualitative studies of individual teachers as ‘persons-in-context’ (Ushioda Reference Ushioda, Dörnyei and Ushioda2009). They need to recognize the complexity of teachers’ mental lives by enquiring into teachers’ prior educational experiences, as learners, as teacher trainees, and as novice teachers, to see how their thinking has evolved over time, how they orient to the profession, and how they perceive the affordances and constraints of their particular context (Kubanyiova & Feryok Reference Kubanyiova and Feryok2015). They need to observe classroom events closely (for example using stimulated recall techniques) to understand teachers’ ‘thinking-in-action’ and the way they interactively create engaging experiences in class, or alternatively fail to do so. Such methods would also help to overcome another flaw noted in the MotS approach, namely uncertainty over the meaning of particular strategies.

6.3.2 Motivating young language learners

L2 education is beginning at ever younger ages in many global contexts, and one of the main rationales for its introduction into the primary school curriculum is motivational – the belief that teachers can foster positive attitudes towards L2 learning which will pay dividends later (Enever Reference Enever2011; Heinzmann Reference Heinzmann2013). But most of the research reviewed here has been concerned with motivating older learners. The future self- and identity-based theories which have recently dominated thinking about L2 motivation are of questionable value in informing young learner teaching, for psychological evidence suggests that ideal selves only take shape in early adolescence (Zentner Reference Zentner and Renaud2007). Children's learning behaviour is likely to be much more contingent on immediate classroom processes, emphasizing the key role of the teacher, yet the MotS taxonomy was built up on the views of experienced teachers working mainly at secondary and tertiary levels. A priority for future L2 motivation research is therefore to analyse the classroom experiences of young L2 learners and teachers, and this is likely to require innovative methods such as metaphor analysis (Jin et al. Reference Jin, Liang, Jiang, Zhang, Yuan and Xie2014) and the use of system logs to track learners’ online game playing (Bodnar et al. Reference Bodnar, Cucchiarini, Strik and van Hout2014). There are suggestions that the initial positive effect generated in early years classrooms may be undermined as L2 learning becomes more challenging (Enever Reference Enever2011), so longitudinal studies would be especially valuable.

6.3.3 Intervention studies

Understanding what teachers and learners currently do is important, but motivation researchers should also be pushing the field forward through theoretically informed, empirically robust interventions, working with groups of teachers to help make their practice more motivating for their learners. The practical and ethical challenges involved in such research no doubt explain why quasi-experimental interventions – involving attempts to control intervening variables and gaining permission form diverse stakeholders (e.g. as in Moskovsky et al. Reference Moskovsky, Alrabai, Paolini and Ratcheva2012; Alrabai Reference Alrabai2016) – make up such a tiny proportion of the research reviewed in this paper, yet such studies are to be encouraged because they offer the most persuasive evidence of motivational impact. They do not need to be large-scale, as long as the intervention itself is theoretically sound and implemented consistently, and appropriate statistical procedures are used (see Rosenzweig & Wigfield (Reference Rosenzweig and Wigfield2016) for an authoritative review of motivation interventions in science, technology, engineering and maths subjects).

The new theoretical concept of directed motivational currents (DMCs) (Dörnyei, Henry & Muir Reference Dörnyei, Henry and Muir2016) – periods of intense activity in pursuit of a short-term goal – may prove to be a fertile source of interventions. The rise of digital technology, as well as aspirations towards more radically learner-centred teaching, has led to renewed interest in project work across the curriculum (e.g. see Patton Reference Patton2012). Dörnyei et al. (Reference Dörnyei, Henry and Muir2016) present a series of frameworks for class projects in language learning based on their understanding of the essential features of a group DMC:

  • It should start with a clear collective goal, for which everyone feels a sense of ownership

  • There are sub-goals and progress checks built into the process

  • It generates positive emotionality in the group

  • It has a demonstrable outcome, in the form of a performance/exhibition/production

An alternative form of intervention is exemplified in Turner et al. (Reference Turner, Christensen, Kackar-Cam, Trucano and Fulmer2014), who worked with a number of US high school teachers over a three-year period to help them apply a set of four motivational principles (partly based on SDT) and found that repeated class observations and post-class discussions did help to make the teaching style of some (but not all) teachers more supportive of their pupils’ engagement. The longitudinal nature of this study is worth remarking on: recognizing the complexity of classrooms, the researchers were not seeking immediate or linear cause and effect relations between teacher behaviour and learner engagement, but working towards the gradual emergence of new patterns of teacher–student relations.

6.3.4 Teachers (and learners) as (co-)researchers

Any successful intervention study requires the sustained cooperation of teachers, not just to grant access to their classrooms but because only a teacher will know exactly how motivational principles or strategies should be operationalized in any particular classroom. There are other good reasons why future research on L2 motivation should be done with rather than on teachers. First, experienced teachers build huge funds of knowledge over their professional lifetime which can and should inform theory. In a chapter on the motivational power of having learners ‘speak as themselves’ in class, Ushioda (Reference Ushioda, Murray, Lamb and Gao2011a) acknowledges that ‘communicative’ and ‘humanistic’ language teachers had discovered this for themselves at least two decades ago. In this case, academics were adding a psychological rationale for a practice that was more usually justified in psycholinguistic terms, for producing ‘enhanced output’. In other areas motivational theorizing is guiding pedagogy, for instance in developing L2 future self-imagery (Hadfield & Dörnyei Reference Hadfield and Dörnyei2013), or has the capacity to do so. The point is that greater understanding of the motivational dimension of language teaching can only come through close collaboration between researchers and teachers.

A further reason why practising teachers need to be involved in L2 motivation research is that the process of researching can itself be motivating, for teachers and learners. Proponents of action research and exploratory practice (e.g. Farrell Reference Farrell2008; Allwright & Hanks Reference Allwright and Hanks2009) claim that systematic investigation of their own classes helps teachers develop feelings of control and competence, a stance supported by some of the practitioner researchers cited in this review (e.g. Mearns Reference Mearns2012; Sampson Reference Sampson2012); learners too can be motivated by involvement in classroom research as they gain insight into aspects of their own practices and thought processes (e.g. Murphey & Falout Reference Murphey and Falout2010; Coyle Reference Coyle, Lasagabaster, Doiz and Sierra2014).

6.3.5 Topics of controversy

Debates have raged in mainstream educational psychology that have barely caused a flicker of interest in L2 motivation circles, possibly because we are still profoundly influenced by Gardnerian views (Reference Gardner, Giles and Clair1979) on the distinctiveness of desire to learn language. For teachers working in language classes in schools or colleges, however, there are fundamental motivational issues that they share with teachers of other subjects, and these deserve more empirical investigation. One example is the use of rewards, particularly prevalent in primary/elementary schools where language teaching is increasingly based, but also common in the certification systems of private language schools, and most recently in the gamification of digital learning tasks. The success of such methods would appear to contravene the tenets of SDT, in that the regular offer of extrinsic rewards for school work may undermine the development of long-term intrinsic motivation to study, but this view has also been challenged (e.g. Cameron, Banko & Pierce Reference Cameron, Banko and Pierce2001). Likewise, the way teachers commonly praise pupils, focusing on the person rather than performance of the task-in-hand, has been criticized for substituting short-term emotional rewards for more enduring forms of motivation (Dweck Reference Dweck1999). Summative assessment is another ubiquitous extrinsic motivator, undeniably stimulating in certain contexts, as all experienced teachers know, yet also perceived as controlling by students and frequently a source of anxiety (Pulfrey, Darnon & Butera Reference Pulfrey, Darnon and Butera2013). Notwithstanding their complexity, these are surely issues that should interest L2 motivation researchers because they are deeply embedded in the practices of language teachers.

7. Conclusion

The surge of interest among academics in the motivational dimension of language teaching is to be broadly welcomed. It is serving to mediate between L2 motivation theory and classroom practice, enhancing the real-world impact of the former, expanding the motivational awareness of pedagogical innovators, and potentially providing novice teachers with useful guidance on this important aspect of their job. It is helping us to understand the nature of real-world problems too: why so many state school pupils are demotivated in their study of English when it could be so valuable in their lives; how English mother tongue pupils can be persuaded to learn FL when their value is not immediately obvious; how people who do have L2 goals can be pushed to invest even more effort into learning than they currently do; why teachers need to find a balance between short-term stimulation, through entertainment, rewards or the threat of sanctions, and inspiring a long-term dedication to learn the L2 through the nurturing of internal motives.

However, at this stage in its evolution, quality needs prioritizing over quantity. This could be served by following the directions outlined above, as well as by upscaling sample sizes, implementing bolder interventions, deliberately targeting areas of pedagogical or theoretical challenge. The groundwork has been done; it is time for more ambitious projects to build our understanding of how teaching motivates and demotivates language learners.

Martin Lamb is a Senior Lecturer in TESOL at the School of Education, University of Leeds, having previously worked as an English teacher or trainer in Sweden, Indonesia and Bulgaria. He teaches courses in language teaching methodology, assessment, and the psychology and sociology of language learning. His main research interest is learner and teacher motivation, and their interaction with each other and with other elements of social context.

References

Acheson, K., Nelson, M. & Luna, K. (2015). Measuring the impact of instruction in intercultural communication on secondary Spanish learners’ attitudes and motivation. Foreign Language Annals 48.2, 203217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ajibade, Y. & Ndububa, K. (2008). Effects of word games, culturally relevant songs, and stories on students’ motivation in a Nigerian English language class. TESL Canada Journal 25.2, 2748.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alrabai, F. (2014). Motivational practices in English as a foreign language classes in Saudi Arabia: Teachers’ beliefs and learners’ perceptions. Arab World English Journal 5.1, 224246.Google Scholar
Alrabai, F. (2016). The effects of teachers' in-class motivational intervention on learners' EFL achievement. Applied Linguistics 37.3, 307333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Al-Shehri, A. S. (2009). Motivation and vision: The relation between the Ideal L2 self, imagination and visual style. In Dörnyei, Z. & Ushioda, E. (eds.), 164–171.Google Scholar
Allwright, D. & Hanks, J. (eds.) (2009). The developing language learner: An introduction to exploratory practice. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderman, E. M. & Anderman, L. H. (2010). Classroom motivation. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education.Google Scholar
Andres, V. d. & Arnold, J. (2009). Seeds of confidence: Self-esteem activities for the EFL classroom. Rum: Helbling Languages.Google Scholar
Apple, M. T., Da Silva, D. & Fellner, T. (eds.) (2013). Language learning motivation in Japan. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Apple, M. T., Da Silva, D., Fellner, T. (eds.) (2017). L2 selves and motivations in Asian contexts. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Aydin, S. (2012). Factors causing demotivation in EFL teaching process: A case study. Qualitative Report 17.51, 113.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Freeman.Google Scholar
Bennell, P. & Akyeampong, K. (2007). Teacher motivation in Sub-Saharan Africa and South Asia. London: Department for International Development.Google Scholar
Benson, P. (2011). Teaching and researching autonomy (2nd edn.). Harlow: Pearson Education.Google Scholar
Bernaus, M. & Gardner, R. C. (2008). Teacher motivation strategies, student perceptions, student motivation, and English achievement. The Modern Language Journal 92.2, 387401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernaus, M., Wilson, A. & Gardner, R. C. (2009). Teachers’ motivation, classroom strategy use, students’ motivation and second language achievement. Porta Lingarum 12, 2536.Google Scholar
Bhattacharya, A. & Chauhan, K. (2010). Augmenting learner autonomy through blogging. ELT Journal 64.4, 376384.Google Scholar
Birjandi, P. & Tamjid, N. H. (2010). The role of self-assessment in promoting Iranian EFL learners’ motivation. English Language Teaching 3.3, 211220.Google Scholar
Block, D. (2007). Second language identities. London: Continuum.Google Scholar
Block, D. (2013). Social class in applied linguistics. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Bodnar, S., Cucchiarini, C., Strik, H. & van Hout, R. (2014). Evaluating the motivational impact of CALL systems: Current practices and future directions. Computer Assisted Language Learning 29.1, 186–127.Google Scholar
Boo, Z., Dörnyei, Z. & Ryan, S. (2015). L2 motivation research 2005–2014: Understanding a publication surge and a changing landscape. System 55, 147157.Google Scholar
Borg, S. (2003). Teacher cognition in language teaching: A review of research on what language teachers think, know, believe, and do. Language Teaching 36.2, 81109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breen, M. (ed.) (2001). Learner contributions to language learning: New directions in research. Harlow: Pearson Education.Google Scholar
Burns, A., Morris-Adams, M., Garton, S. & Copland, F. (2013). Key factors and challenges in transition from primary to secondary schooling in ELT: An international perspective. London: British Council Research Papers.Google Scholar
Busse, V. (2013). How do students of German perceive feedback practices at university? A motivational exploration. Journal of Second Language Writing 22.4, 406424.Google Scholar
Busse, V. & Walter, C. (2013). Foreign language learning motivation in higher education: A longitudinal study of motivational changes and their causes. The Modern Language Journal 97.2, 435456.Google Scholar
Cameron, J., Banko, K. M. & Pierce, W. D. (2001). Pervasive negative effects of rewards on intrinsic motivation: The myth continues. The Behavior Analyst/MABA 24.1, 144.Google Scholar
Campbell, E. & Storch, N. (2011). The changing face of motivation: A study of second language learners’ motivation over time. Australian Review of Applied Linguistics 34.2, 166192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carr, J. & Pauwels, P. (2006). Boys and foreign language learning: Real boys don't do languages. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Carreira, J. M., Ozaki, K. & Maeda, T. (2014). Motivational model of English learning among elementary school students in Japan. System 41, 706719.Google Scholar
Castellano, J., Mynard, J. & Rubesch, T. (2011). Student technology use in a self-access center. Language Learning and Technology 15.3, 1227.Google Scholar
Chambers, G. (1999). Motivating language learners. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Chambers, G. (2005). Teaching modern foreign languages in single-sex classes in a co-educational context – review of a project in a North Yorkshire comprehensive school. The Language Learning Journal 32.1, 4554.Google Scholar
Chan, L. (2014). Effects of an imagery training strategy on Chinese university students’ possible second language selves and learning experiences. In Csizér, K. & Magid, M. (eds.), 357–376.Google Scholar
Chang, L. Y.-H. (2010). Group processes and EFL learners’ motivation: A study of group dynamics in EFL classrooms. TESOL Quarterly 44.1, 129154.Google Scholar
Chen, C.-F. & Cheng, W.-Y. E. (2008). Beyond the design of automated writing evaluation: Pedagogical practices and perceived learning effectiveness in EFL writing classes. Language Learning and Technology 12.2, 94112.Google Scholar
Cheng, H.-F. & Dörnyei, Z. (2007). The use of motivational strategies in language instruction: The case of EFL teaching in Taiwan. Innovation in Language Learning and Teaching 1.1, 153174.Google Scholar
Cheng, L., Watanabe, Y. & Curtis, A. (eds.) (2004). Washback in language testing: Research context and methods. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Chou, M.-H. (2014). Assessing English vocabulary and enhancing young English as a foreign language learners’ motivation through games, songs, and stories. Education 3–13 42.3, 284297.Google Scholar
Clément, R. (1980). Ethnicity, contact and communicative competence in a second language. In Giles, H., Robinson, W. P. & Smith, P. M. (eds.), Language: Social psychological perspectives. Oxford: Pergamon, 147154.Google Scholar
Coleman, H. (2011). Allocating resources for English: The case of Indonesia's English medium international standard schools. In Coleman, H. (ed.), 87–111.Google Scholar
Coleman, H. (ed.) (2011). Dreams and realities: Developing countries and the English language. London: The British Council.Google Scholar
Coleman, J. A. (2009). Why the British do not learn languages: Myths and motivation in the United Kingdom. Language Learning Journal 37.1, 111127.Google Scholar
Coyle, D. (2014). Motivating teachers and learners as researchers. In Lasagabaster, D., Doiz, A. & Sierra, J. M. (eds.), Motivation and foreign language learning. Amsterdam: John Benjamin, 5169.Google Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Csizér, K. & Kormos, J. (2008). The relationship of intercultural contact and language learning motivation among Hungarian students of English and German. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development 29.1, 3048.Google Scholar
Csizér, K. & Magid, M. (eds.) (2013). The impact of self-concept on language learning. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Danziger, K. (1997). Naming the mind: How psychology found its language. London: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Deci, E. L. & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behaviour. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Doiz, A., Lasagabaster, D. & Sierra, J. M. (2014). CLIL and motivation: The effect of individual and contextual variables. Language Learning Journal 42, 209224.Google Scholar
Donitsa-Schmidt, S., Inbar, O. & Shohamy, E. (2004). The effects of teaching spoken Arabic on students’ attitudes and motivation in Israel. The Modern Language Journal 88.2, 217228.Google Scholar
Dörnyei, Z. (1998). Motivation in second and foreign language learning. Language Teaching 31.3, 117135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dörnyei, Z. (2001). Motivational strategies in the language classroom. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dörnyei, Z. (2002). The motivational basis of language learning tasks. In Robinson, P. (ed.), Individual differences and instructed language learning. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 137158.Google Scholar
Dörnyei, Z. (2007). Creating a motivating classroom environment. In Cummins, J. & Davison, C. (eds.), International handbook of English language teaching. New York: Springer US. Google Scholar
Dörnyei, Z. (2009). The L2 motivational self system. In Dörnyei, Z. & Ushioda, E. (eds.), 9–42.Google Scholar
Dörnyei, Z. & Chan, L. (2013). Motivation and vision: An analysis of future L2 self images, sensory styles, and imagery capacity across two target languages. Language Learning 63.3, 437462.Google Scholar
Dörnyei, Z. & Csizér, K. (1998). Ten commandments for motivating language learners: Results of an empirical study. Language Teaching Research 2.3, 203229.Google Scholar
Dörnyei, Z. & Kormos, J. (2000). The role of individual and social variables in oral task performance. Language Teaching Research 4.3, 275300.Google Scholar
Dörnyei, Z. & Kubanyiova, M. (2014). Motivating learners, motivating teachers: Building vision in the language classroom. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dörnyei, Z. & Murphey, T. (2003). Group dynamics in the language classroom. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dörnyei, Z. & Schmidt, R. W. (eds.) (2001). Motivation and second language acquisition. Hawai'i: University of Hawai'i.Google Scholar
Dörnyei, Z. & Tseng, W.-T. (2009). Motivational processing in interactional tasks. In Mackey, A. & Polio, C. (eds.), Multiple perspectives on interaction: Second language research in Honor of Susan M. Gass. London: Routledge, 117134.Google Scholar
Dörnyei, Z. & Ushioda, E. (eds.) (2009). Motivation, language identity and the L2 self. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Dörnyei, Z. & Ushioda, E. (2011). Teaching and researching motivation (2nd edn.). Harlow: Pearson Education.Google Scholar
Dörnyei, Z., Henry, A. & Muir, C. (2016). Motivational currents in language learning: Frameworks for focussed interventions. Abingdon: Routledge.Google Scholar
Dörnyei, Z., MacIntyre, P. & Henry, A. (eds.) (2015). Motivational dynamics in language learning. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Duff, P. A. (2002). The discursive co-construction of knowledge, identity and difference: An ethnography of communication in the high school mainstream. Applied Linguistics 23, 289322.Google Scholar
Duijnhouwer, H., Prins, F. J. & Stokking, K. M. (2012). Feedback providing improvement strategies and reflection on feedback use: Effects on students’ writing motivation, process, and performance. Learning and Instruction 22.3, 171184.Google Scholar
Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development. Philadelphia: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Egbert, J. (2003). A study of flow theory in the language classroom. The Modern Language Journal 87.4, 499518.Google Scholar
Ellis, R. (2009). A reader responds to Guilloteaux and Dörnyei's ‘Motivating language learners: A classroom-oriented investigation of the effects of motivational strategies on student motivation’. TESOL Quarterly 43.1, 105109.Google Scholar
Enever, J. (2011). ELLiE: Early language learning in Europe. London: The British Council.Google Scholar
Erler, L. & Macaro, E. (2011). Decoding ability in French as a foreign language and language learning motivation. The Modern Language Journal 95.4, 496518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erten, I. H. & Burden, R. L. (2014). The relationship between academic self-concept, attributions, and L2 achievement. System 42, 391401.Google Scholar
Falout, J. & Maruyama, M. (2004). A comparative study of proficiency and learner demotivation. The Language Teacher 28, 39.Google Scholar
Falout, J., Elwood, J. & Hood, M. (2009). Demotivation: Affective states and learning outcomes. System 37.3, 403417.Google Scholar
Falout, J., Fukada, Y., Murphey, T. & Fukuda, T. (2009). What's working in Japan? Present communities of imagining. In Apple, M., Da Silva, D. & Fellner, T. (eds.), 245–267.Google Scholar
Farrell, T. (2008). Reflective language teaching: From research to practice. London: Continuum.Google Scholar
Farrell, T. (2015). Reflecting on teacher-student relations in TESOL. ELT Journal 69.1, 2634.Google Scholar
Feryok, A. & Oranje, J. O. (2015). Adopting a cultural portfolio project in teaching German as a foreign language: Language teacher cognition as a dynamic system. The Modern Language Journal 99.3, 546564.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forman, R. (2011). Humorous language play in a Thai EFL classroom. Applied Linguistics 32.5, 541565.Google Scholar
Freiermuth, M. R. (in press). ‘I found it!’: A smartphone GPS treasure-hunting game in a flipped English class. Innovation in Language Learning and Teaching.Google Scholar
Freiermuth, M. R. & Huang, H.-C. (2012). Bringing Japan and Taiwan closer electronically: A look at an intercultural online synchronic chat task and its effect on motivation. Language Teaching Research 16.1, 6188.Google Scholar
Fryer, L. K., Ozono, S., Carter, P., Nakao, K. & Anderson, C. J. (2014). Instrumental reasons for studying in compulsory English courses: I didn't come to university to study English, so why should I? Innovation in Language Learning and Teaching 8.3, 239256.Google Scholar
Fukuda, S. T., Sakata, H. & Takeuchi, M. (2011). Facilitating autonomy to enhance motivation: Examining the effects of a guided-autonomy syllabus. Electronic Journal of Foreign Language Teaching 8.1, 7186.Google Scholar
Gallagher-Brett, A. (2004). 700 reasons for studying languages. Southampton: Subject Centre for Languages, Linguistics and Area Studies, University of Southampton.Google Scholar
Gardner, R. C. (1979). Social psychological aspects of second language acquisition. In Giles, H. & Clair, R. S. (eds.), Language and social psychology. Oxford: Basil Blackwell, 193220.Google Scholar
Gardner, R. C. (1985). Social psychology and second language learning: The role of attitude and motivation. London: Edward Arnold.Google Scholar
Gardner, R. & Tremblay, P. F. (1994). On motivation, research agendas and theoretical frameworks. The Modern Language Journal 78.3, 359368.Google Scholar
Glas, K. (2015). Opening up ‘spaces for manoeuvre’: English teacher perspectives on learner motivation. Research Papers in Education, 120.Google Scholar
Gleason, J. & Suvorov, R. (2012). Learner perceptions of asynchronous oral computer-mediated communication: Proficiency and second language selves. The Canadian Journal of Applied Linguistics 15.1, 100121.Google Scholar
Golonka, E. M., Bowles, A. R., Frank, V. M., Richardson, D. L. & Freynik, S. (2014). Technologies for foreign language learning: A review of technology types and their effectiveness. Computer Assisted Language Learning 27.1, 70105.Google Scholar
Graddol, D. (2006). English next. Plymouth: The British Council.Google Scholar
Graham, S. (2004). Giving up on foreign languages? Students’ perceptions of learning French. The Modern Language Journal 88.2, 171191.Google Scholar
Graham, S. & Macaro, E. (2008). Strategy instruction in listening for lower-intermediate learners of French. Language Learning 58.4, 747783.Google Scholar
Guilloteaux, M.-J. (2013). Motivational strategies for the language classroom: Perceptions of Korean secondary school English teachers. System 41.1, 314.Google Scholar
Guilloteaux, M.-J. & Dörnyei, Z. (2008). Motivating language learners: A classroom-oriented investigation of the effects of motivational strategies on student motivation. TESOL Quarterly 42, 5577.Google Scholar
Hadfield, J. & Dörnyei, Z. (2013). Motivating learning. Harlow: Longman.Google Scholar
Hall, C. J. (2013). Cognitive contributions to pluralithic views of English and other languages. Applied Linguistics 34.2, 211231.Google Scholar
Heinzmann, S. (2013). Young language learners’ motivation and attitudes. London: Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
Henry, A. (2013). Digital games and ELT: Bridging the authenticity gap. In Ushioda, E. (ed.), 133–155.Google Scholar
Henry, A. & Cliffordson, C. (2013). Motivation, gender, and possible selves. Language Learning 63.2, 271295.Google Scholar
Hernandez, T. A. (2010). The relationship among motivation, interaction, and the development of second language oral proficiency in a study-abroad context. The Modern Language Journal 94.4, 600617.Google Scholar
Hsieh, P. & Kang, H. (2010). Attribution and self-efficacy and their interrelationship in the Korean EFL context. Language Learning 60.3, 606627.Google Scholar
Hu, G. & McKay, S. L. (2012). English language education in East Asia: Some recent developments. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development 33.4, 345362.Google Scholar
Huang, K.-M. (2011). Motivating lessons: A classroom-oriented investigation of the effects of content-based instruction on EFL young learners’ motivated behaviours and classroom verbal interaction. System 39, 186201.Google Scholar
Huang, S.-C. (2012). Pushing learners to work through tests and marks: Motivating or demotivating? A case in a Taiwanese university. Language Assessment Quarterly 9.1, 6077.Google Scholar
Iwaniec, J. (2013). Self-constructs in language learning. In Csizér, K. & Magid, M. (eds.), 189–205.Google Scholar
Jauregi, K., de Graaff, R., van den Bergh, H. & Kriz, M. (2012). Native/non-native speaker interactions through video-web communication: A clue for enhancing motivation? Computer Assisted Language Learning 25.1, 119.Google Scholar
Jenkins, J. (2007). English as a lingua franca: Attitude and identity. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Jin, L., Liang, X., Jiang, C., Zhang, J., Yuan, Y. & Xie, Q. (2014). Studying the motivations of Chinese young EFL learners through metaphor analysis. ELT Journal 68.3, 286298.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. (2013). A longitudinal perspective on EFL learning motivation in Japanese engineering students. In Apple, M., Da Silva, D. & Fellner, T. (eds.), 189–205.Google Scholar
Jones, B. D., Llacer-Arrastia, S. & Newbill, P. B. (2009). Motivating foreign language students using self-determination theory. Innovation in Language Learning and Teaching 3.2, 171189.Google Scholar
Julkunen, K. (2001). Situation- and task-specific motivation in foreign language learning. In Dörnyei, Z. & Schmidt, R. W. (eds.), 29–41.Google Scholar
Jung, H.-J. (2014). Ubiquitous learning: Determinants impacting learners’ satisfaction and performance with smartphones. Language Learning and Technology 18.3, 97119.Google Scholar
Kao, T.-A & Oxford, R. L. (2014). Learning language through music: A strategy for building inspiration and motivation. System 43, 114120.Google Scholar
Kaplan, A. & Flum, H. (2009). Motivation and identity: The relations of action and development in educational contexts – an introduction to the special issue. Educational Psychologist 44.2, 7377.Google Scholar
Karavas, E. (2010). How satisfied are Greek EFL teachers with their work? Investigating the motivation and job satisfaction levels of Greek EFL teachers. Porta Linguarum 14, 5978.Google Scholar
Kato, F. (2009). Student preferences: Goal-setting and self-assessment activities in a tertiary education environment. Language Teaching Research 13.2, 177199.Google Scholar
Kikuchi, K. (2013). Demotivators in the Japanese EFL context. In Apple, M., Da Silva, D. & Fellner, T. (eds.), 206–225.Google Scholar
Kikuchi, K. (in press). Re-examining demotivators and motivators: A longitudinal study of Japanese freshmen's dynamic system in an EFL context. Innovation in Language Learning and Teaching.Google Scholar
Kim, D., Rueckert, D., Kim, D.-J. & Seo, D. (2013). Students’ perceptions and experiences of mobile learning. Language Learning and Technology 17.3, 5273.Google Scholar
Kissau, S., Quach, L. & Wang, C. (2009). The impact of single-sex instruction on student motivation to learn Spanish. Canadian Journal of Applied Linguistics 12, 5478.Google Scholar
Klimanova, L. & Dembovskaya, S. (2013). L2 identity, discourse and social networking in Russian. Language Learning and Technology 17.1, 6988.Google Scholar
Koga, T. (2010). Dynamicity of motivation, anxiety and cooperativeness in a semester course. System 38.2, 172184.Google Scholar
Kormos, J., Kiddle, T. & Csizér, K. (2011). Systems of goals, attitudes, and self-related beliefs in second-language-learning motivation. Applied Linguistics 32.5, 495516.Google Scholar
Kramsch, C. (2009). The multilingual subject. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kubanyiova, M. (2006). Developing a motivational teaching practice in EFL teachers in Slovakia: Challenges of promoting teacher changes in EFL contexts. TESL-EJ: Teaching English as a Second or Foreign Language 10.2, 17.Google Scholar
Kubanyiova, M. (2012). Teacher development in action: Understanding language teachers’ conceptual change. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Kubanyiova, M. & Feryok, A. (2015). Language teacher cognition in applied linguistics research: Revisiting the territory, redrawing the boundaries, reclaiming the relevance. The Modern Language Journal 99.3, 435449.Google Scholar
Kuchah, H. (2013). From bilingual Francophones to bilingual Anglophones: The role of teachers in the rising ‘equities’ of English-medium education in Cameroon. In Ushioda, E. (ed.), 60–81.Google Scholar
Kukulska-Hulme, A., Norris, L. & Donohue, J. (2015). Mobile pedagogy for English language teaching: A guide for teachers. London: British Council Research Papers.Google Scholar
Lam, R. (2014). Promoting self-regulated learning through portfolio assessment: Testimony and recommendations. Assessment and Evaluation in Higher Education 39.6, 699714.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lamb, M. (2011). A Matthew Effect in English language education in a developing country context. In Coleman, H. (ed.), 186–206.Google Scholar
Lamb, M. (2012). A self-system perspective on young adolescents’ motivation to learn English in urban and rural settings. Language Learning 62.4, 9971023.Google Scholar
Lamb, M. & Budiyanto, (2013). Cultural challenges, identity and motivation in state school EFL. In Ushioda, E. (ed.), 18–34.Google Scholar
Lamb, M. & Wedell, M. (2015). Cultural contrasts and commonalities in inspiring language teaching. Language Teaching Research 19.2, 207224.Google Scholar
Lamb, M., Astuti, S. P. & Hadisantosa, N. (2017). ‘In their shoes’: What successful Indonesian school teachers do to motivate their pupils. In Apple, M., Da Silva, D. & Fellner, T. (eds.), L2 selves and motivation in Asian contexts. Bristol: Multilingual Matters, 195216.Google Scholar
Lantolf, J. & Genung, P. (2002). ‘I'd rather switch than fight’: An activity-theoretic study of power, success, and failure in a foreign language. In Kramsch, C. (ed.), Language acquisition and language socialization: Ecological perspectives. London: Continuum, 175196.Google Scholar
Lanvers, U. & Coleman, J. A. (2013). The UK language learning crisis in the public media: A critical analysis. The Language Learning Journal, 1–23.Google Scholar
Lasagabaster, D. (2011). English achievement and student motivation in CLIL and EFL settings. Innovation in Language Learning and Teaching 5.1, 318.Google Scholar
Lasagabaster, D. & Beloqui, R. L. (2015). The impact of type of approach (CLIL versus EFL) and methodology (book-based versus project work) on motivation. Porta Linguarum 23, 4157.Google Scholar
Littlejohn, A. (2008). The tip of the iceberg: Factors affecting learner motivation. RELC Journal 39.2, 214225.Google Scholar
Lo, J. & Hyland, F. (2007). Enhancing students’ engagement and motivation in writing: The case of primary students in Hong Kong. Journal of Second Language Writing 16, 219237.Google Scholar
Lou, N. M. & Noels, K. A. (2016). Changing language mindsets: Implications for goal orientations and responses to failure in and outside the second language classroom. Contemporary Educational Psychology 46, 2233.Google Scholar
Lozanov, G. (1978). Suggestology and outlines of suggestopedy. London: Gordon and Breach Science Publishers Inc.Google Scholar
Luk, J. (2005). Voicing the self through the ‘other’ language’: Exploring communicative language teaching for global communication. In Canagarajah, A. S. (ed.), Reclaiming the local in language policy and practice. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 247268.Google Scholar
Macaro, E., Handley, Z. & Walter, C. (2012). A systematic review of CALL in English as a second language: Focus on primary and secondary education. Language Teaching 45.1, 143.Google Scholar
MacIntyre, P. & Serroul, A. (2015). Motivation on a per-second timescale: Examining approach-avoidance motivation during L2 task performance. In Dörnyei, Z., MacIntyre, P. & Henry, A. (eds.), 109–138.Google Scholar
Mackay, J. (2014). Applications and implications of the L2 motivational self-system in a Catalan EFL context. In Magid, M. & Csizér, K. (eds.), 377–400.Google Scholar
Maeng, U. & Lee, S.-M. (2015). EFL teachers’ behaviour of using motivational strategies: The case of teaching in the Korean context. Teaching and Teacher Education 46, 2536.Google Scholar
Magid, M. (2014). A motivational programme for learners of English: An application of the L2 motivational self-system. In Csizér, K. & Magid, M. (eds.), 333–356.Google Scholar
Magid, M. & Chan, L. (2012). Motivating English learners by helping them visualise their Ideal L2 self: Lessons from two motivational programmes. Innovation in Language Learning and Teaching 6.2, 113125.Google Scholar
Masgoret, A. M. & Gardner, R. (2003). Attitudes, motivation and second language learning: A meta-analysis of studies conducted by Gardner and associates. Language Learning 53.1, 123163.Google Scholar
Matsumoto, H., Hiromori, T. & Nakayama, A. (2013). Toward a tripartite model of L2 reading strategy use, motivations, and learner beliefs. System 41.1, 3849.Google Scholar
McCall, I. (2011). ‘Score in French’: Motivating boys with football in Key Stage 3. Language Learning Journal 39.1, 518.Google Scholar
McInerney, D. & Van Etten, S. (eds.) (2004). Big theories revisited. Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
Mearns, T. L. (2012). Using CLIL to enhance pupils’ experience of learning and raise attainment in German and health education: A teacher research project. Language Learning Journal 40.2, 175192.Google Scholar
Mercer, S. & Williams, M. (eds.) (2015). Multiple perspectives on the self in SLA. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Mezei, G. (2014). The effect of motivational strategies on self-related aspects of student motivation and second language learning. In Csizér, K. & Magid, M. (eds.), 289–309.Google Scholar
Mills, N. (2014). Self-efficacy in second language acquisition. In Mercer, S. & Williams, M. (eds.), 6–22.Google Scholar
Mills, N., Pajares, F. & Herron, C. (2007). Self-efficacy of college intermediate French students: Relation to achievement and motivation. Language Learning 57.3, 417442.Google Scholar
Mills, N. & Peron, M. (2009). Global simulation and writing self-beliefs of college intermediate French students. International Journal of Applied Linguistics 156, 239273.Google Scholar
Mohanty, A. K., Panda, M., Phillipson, R. & Skuttnab-Kangas, T. (eds.) (2009). Multilingual education for social justice: Globalising the local. New Delhi: Orient Longman.Google Scholar
Morita, N. (2004). Negotiating participation and identity in second language academic communities. TESOL Quarterly 38.4, 574603.Google Scholar
Moskovsky, C., Alrabai, F., Paolini, S. & Ratcheva, S. (2012). The effects of teachers’ motivational strategies on learners’ motivation: A controlled investigation of second language acquisition. Language Learning 63.1, 3462.Google Scholar
Motha, S., & Lin, A. (2014). ‘Non-coercive rearrangements’: Theorizing desire in TESOL. TESOL Quarterly 48.2, 331359.Google Scholar
Mozgalina, A. (2015). More or less choice? The influence of choice on task motivation and task engagement. System 49.1, 120132.Google Scholar
Munezane, Y. (2013). Motivation, Ideal L2 self and valuing of global English. In Apple, M., Da Silva, D. & Fellner, T. (eds.), 152–168.Google Scholar
Muñoz, A. & Ramirez, M. (2015). Teachers’ conceptions of motivation and motivating practices in second-language learning: A self-determination theory perspective. Theory and Research in Education 13.2, 198220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murphey, T. I. M. & Falout, J. (2010). Critical participatory looping: Dialogic member checking with whole classes. TESOL Quarterly 44.4, 811821.Google Scholar
Murphey, T., Falout, J., Fukuda, T. & Fukada, Y. (2014). Socio-dynamic motivating through idealizing classmates. System 45, 242253.Google Scholar
Murray, G. (2013). Pedagogy of the possible: Imagination, autonomy, and space. Studies in Second Language Learning and Teaching 3.3, 377396.Google Scholar
Nikolov, M. (1999). ‘Why do you learn English?’ ‘Because the teacher is short.’ A study of Hungarian children's foreign language learning motivation. Language Teaching Research 3.1, 3356.Google Scholar
Noels, K. A. (2001). Learning Spanish as a second language: Learners’ orientations and perceptions of their teachers’ communicative style. Language Learning 51, 107144.Google Scholar
Noels, K. A. (2013). Learning Japanese, learning English: Promoting motivation through autonomy, competence and relatedness. In Apple, M., Da Silva, D. & Fellner, T. (eds.), 15–34.Google Scholar
Noels, K. A., Chaffee, K. E., Michalyk, M. & Sugita McEown, M. (2014). Culture, autonomy and the self in language learning. In Csizér, K. & Magid, M. (eds.), 131–154.Google Scholar
Noels, K. A., Clément, R. & Pelletier, L. G. (1999). Perceptions of teachers’ communicative style and students’ intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. The Modern Language Journal 83.1, 2334.Google Scholar
Norton, B. (2001). Non-participation, imagined communities, and the language classroom. In Breen, M. (ed.), 159–171.Google Scholar
Norton, B. & Toohey, K. (2011). Identity, language learning, and social change. Language Teaching 44.4, 412446.Google Scholar
Oxford, R. L. (2001). ‘The bleached bones of a story’: Learners’ constructions of language teachers. In Breen, M. (ed.), 86–111.Google Scholar
Oyserman, D., Bybee, D. & Terry, K. (2006). Possible selves and academic outcomes: How and when possible selves impel action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 91.1, 188204.Google Scholar
Pae, T. I. & Shin, S. K. (2011). Examining the effects of differential instructional methods on the model of foreign language achievement. Learning and Individual Differences 21.2, 215222.Google Scholar
Pan, L. & Block, D. (2011). English as a ‘global language’ in China: An investigation into learners’ and teachers’ language beliefs. System 39.3, 391402.Google Scholar
Papi, M. & Abdollahzadeh, E. (2012). Teacher motivational practice, student motivation, and possible L2 selves: An examination in the Iranian EFL context. Language Learning 62.2, 571594.Google Scholar
Patton, A. (2012). Work that matters: The teacher's guide to project-based learning. London: Paul Hamlyn Foundation.Google Scholar
Peacock, M. (1997). The effect of authentic materials on the motivation of EFL learners. ELT Journal 51.2, 144156.Google Scholar
Peiser, G. & Jones, M. (2013). The significance of intercultural understanding in the English modern foreign languages curriculum: A pupil perspective. The Language Learning Journal 41.3, 340356.Google Scholar
Pink, D. H. (2009). Drive: The surprising truth about what motivates us. Edinburgh: Canongate.Google Scholar
Pinner, R. (2014). The authenticity continuum: Towards a definition incorporating international voices. English Today 30.4, 2227.Google Scholar
Poupore, G. (2013). Task motivation in process: A complex systems perspective. The Canadian Modern Language Review 69.1, 91116.Google Scholar
Pulfrey, C., Darnon, C. & Butera, F. (2013). Autonomy and task performance: Explaining the impact of grades on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology 105.1, 3957.Google Scholar
Reeve, J., Deci, E. L. & Ryan, R. M. (2004). Self-determination theory: A dialectical framework for understanding sociocultural influences on student motivation. In McInerney, D. & Van Etten, S. (eds.), 31–60.Google Scholar
Reynolds-Case, A. (2013). The value of short-term study abroad: An increase in students’ cultural and pragmatic competency. Foreign Language Annals 46.2, 311322.Google Scholar
Richards, K. (2006). ‘Being the teacher’: Identity and classroom discourse. Applied Linguistics 27.1, 5177.Google Scholar
Rinvolucri, M. (2002). Humanizing your coursebook. London: DELTA Publishing.Google Scholar
Rosenzweig, E. Q. & Wigfield, A. (2016). Stem motivation interventions for adolescents: A promising start, but further to go. Educational Psychologist 51.2, 146163.Google Scholar
Rubio, F. (2014). Self-esteem and self-concept in foreign language learning. In Mercer, S. & Williams, M. (eds.), 41–58.Google Scholar
Ruesch, A., Bown, J. & Dewey, D. P. (2012). Student and teacher perceptions of motivational strategies in the foreign language classroom. Innovation in Language Learning and Teaching 6.1, 1527.Google Scholar
Rui, T. & Chew, P. G.-L. (2014). Pedagogical use of two languages in a Chinese elementary school. Language, Culture and Curriculum 26.3, 317331.Google Scholar
Ryan, R. M. & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist 55.1, 6878.Google Scholar
Ryan, S. (2006). Language learning motivation within the context of globalisation: An L2 self within an imagined global community. Critical Inquiry in Language Studies, 3.1, 2345.Google Scholar
Ryan, S. (2009). Self and identity in L2 motivation in Japan: The Ideal L2 self and Japanese learners of English. In Dörnyei, Z. & Ushioda, E. (eds.), 120–143.Google Scholar
Ryan, S. & Mercer, S. (2012). Implicit theories: Language learning mindsets. In Mercer, S., Ryan, S. & Williams, M. (eds.), Psychology for language learning. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan, 7489.Google Scholar
Sakai, H. & Kikuchi, K. (2009). An analysis of demotivators in the EFL classroom. System 37.1, 5769.Google Scholar
Sampasivam, S. & Clément, R. (2014). The dynamics of second language confidence: Contact and interaction. In Mercer, S. & Williams, M. (eds.), 23–40.Google Scholar
Sampson, R. (2012). The language-learning self, self-enhancement activities, and self-perceptual change. Language Teaching Research 16.3, 317335.Google Scholar
Sanprasert, N. (2010). The application of a course management system to enhance autonomy in learning English as a foreign language. System 38.1, 109123.Google Scholar
Schunk, D. H. & Pajares, F. (2004). Self-efficacy in education revisited: Empirical and applied evidence. In McInerney, D. M. & Etten, S. Van (eds.), 115–138.Google Scholar
Schunk, D. H., Meece, J. L. & Pintrich, P. R. (2013). Motivation in education: Theory, research, and applications. ( 4th edn.). Harlow: Pearson Education.Google Scholar
Seikkula-Leino, J. (2007). CLIL learning: Achievement levels and affective factors. Language and Education 21, 328341.Google Scholar
Shaaban, K. (2006). An initial study of the effects of cooperative learning on reading comprehension, vocabulary acquisition, and motivation to read. Reading Psychology 27.5, 377403.Google Scholar
Shoaib, A. & Dörnyei, Z. (2005). Affect in life-long learning: Exploring L2 motivation as a dynamic process. In Benson, P. & Nunan, D. (eds.), Learners’ stories. Cambridge: University of Cambridge, 2241.Google Scholar
Skilton-Sylvester, E. (2002). Should I stay or should I go? Investigating Cambodian women's participation and investment in adult ESL programs. Adult Education Quarterly 53.1, 926.Google Scholar
Spada, N. & Fröhlich, M. (1995). Communicative orientation of language teaching observation scheme: Coding conventions and applications. Sydney, Australia: Macquarie University, National Centre for English Language Teaching and Research.Google Scholar
Stockwell, G. (2013). Technology and motivation in English-language teaching and learning. In Ushioda, E. (ed.), 156–175.Google Scholar
Stroet, K., Opdenakker, M.-C. & Minnaert, A. (2013). Effects of need supportive teaching on early adolescents’ motivation and engagement: A review of the literature. Educational Research Review 9.0, 6587.Google Scholar
Sugita McEown, M. & Takeuchi, O. (2010). What can teachers do to motivate their students? A classroom research on motivational strategy use in the Japanese EFL context. Innovation in Language Learning and Teaching 4.1, 2135.Google Scholar
Sugita McEown, M. & Takeuchi, O. (2014). Motivational strategies in EFL classrooms: How do teachers impact students’ motivation? Innovation in Language Learning and Teaching 8.1, 2038.Google Scholar
Sugita McEown, M., Noels, K. A. & Saumure, K. D. (2014). Students’ self-determined and integrative orientations and teachers’ motivational support in a Japanese as a foreign language context. System 45, 227241.Google Scholar
Sullivan, P. (2000). Playfulness as mediation in communicative language teaching in a Vietnamese classroom. In Lantolf, J. P. (ed.), Sociocultural theory and second language learning. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 115132.Google Scholar
Sung, C. C. M. (2014). Hong Kong university students’ perceptions of their identities in English as a lingua franca contexts. Journal of Asian Pacific Communication 24.1, 94112.Google Scholar
Taguchi, T., Magid, M. & Papi, M. (2009). The L2 motivational self system among Japanese, Chinese and Iranian learners of English: A comparative study. In Dörnyei, Z. & Ushioda, E. (eds.), 66–97.Google Scholar
Talmy, S. (2008). The cultural productions of the ESL student at Tradewinds High: Contingency, multidirectionality, and identity in L2 socialization. Applied Linguistics 29.4, 619644.Google Scholar
Tauer, J. M. & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2004). The effects of cooperation and competition on intrinsic motivation and performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 86.6, 849861.Google Scholar
Taylor, A. (2000). Boy-free zone? The Language Learning Journal 21.1, 37.Google Scholar
Taylor, F. (2013). Listening to Romanian teenagers: Lessons in motivation and ELT methodology. In Ushioda, E. (ed.), 35–59.Google Scholar
Taylor, F. & Marsden, E. J. (2014). Perceptions, attitudes, and choosing to study foreign languages in England: An experimental intervention. The Modern Language Journal 98.4, 902920.Google Scholar
Toohey, K. (2000). Learning English at school: Identity, social relations and classroom practice. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Trang, T. T. T. & Baldauf, R. B. (2007). Demotivation: Understanding resistance to English language learning – the case of Vietnamese students. The Journal of Asia TEFL 4.1, 79105.Google Scholar
Tremblay, P. F. & Gardner, R. C. (1995). Expanding the motivation construct in language learning. The Modern Language Journal 79.4, 505520.Google Scholar
Turner, J. C., Warzon, K. B. & Christensen, A. (2011). Motivating mathematics learning: Changes in teachers’ practices and beliefs during a nine-month collaboration. American Educational Research Journal 48, 718762.Google Scholar
Turner, J. C., Christensen, A., Kackar-Cam, H. Z., Trucano, M. & Fulmer, S. M. (2014). Enhancing students’ engagement: Report of a 3-year intervention with middle school teachers. American Educational Research Journal 51.6, 11951226.Google Scholar
Ushioda, E. (2007). Motivation, autonomy and sociocultural theory. In Benson, P. (ed.), Learner autonomy 8: Teacher and learner perspectives. Dublin: Authentik, 524.Google Scholar
Ushioda, E. (2009). A person-in-context relational view of emergent motivation, self and identity. In Dörnyei, Z. & Ushioda, E. (eds.), 215–228.Google Scholar
Ushioda, E. (2011a). Motivating learners to speak as themselves. In Murray, G., Lamb, T. & Gao, X. (eds.), Identity, motivation and autonomy: Exploring their links. Bristol: Multilingual Matters, 1124.Google Scholar
Ushioda, E. (2011b). Language learning motivation, self and identity: Current theoretical perspectives. Computer Assisted Language Learning 24.3, 199210.Google Scholar
Ushioda, E. (ed.) (2013). International perspectives on motivation: Language learning and professional challenges. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Ushioda, E. (2014). Motivational perspectives on the self in SLA: A developmental view. In Mercer, S. & Williams, M. (eds.), 127–141.Google Scholar
Wang, C. & Pape, S. J. (2007). A probe into three Chinese boys’ self-efficacy beliefs learning English as a second language. Journal of Research in Childhood Education 21, 364377.Google Scholar
Waninge, F., Dörnyei, Z. & de Bot, K. (2014). Motivational dynamics in language learning: Change, stability, and context. The Modern Language Journal 98.3, 704723.Google Scholar
Warschauer, M. (1996). Motivational aspects of using computers for writing and communication. In Warschauer, M. (ed.), Telecollaboration in foreign language learning. Honolulu, HI: University of Hawai'i Second Language Teaching and Curriculum Center, 2946.Google Scholar
Wedell, M. (2009). Planning educational change: Putting people and their contexts first. London: Continuum.Google Scholar
Wehner, A. K., Gump, A. W. & Downey, S. (2011). The effects of second life on the motivation of undergraduate students learning a foreign language. Computer Assisted Language Learning 24.3, 277289.Google Scholar
Weiner, B. (1986). An attributional theory of motivation and emotion. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Wentzel, K. R. (2009). Students’ relationships with teachers as motivational constructs. In Wentzel, K. R. & Wigfield, A. (eds.), Handbook of motivation at school. New York: Routledge, 301322.Google Scholar
Wentzel, K. R. & Brophy, J. (2014). Motivating students to learn (4th edn.). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Wigfield, A., Cambria, J. & Eccles, J. S. (2012). Motivation in education. In Ryan, R. M. (ed.), The Oxford handbook of human motivation. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 463478.Google Scholar
Williams, M. & Burden, R. L. (1997). Psychology for language teachers. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Williams, M., Burden, R. L. & Lanvers, U. (2002). ‘French is the language of love and stuff’: Student perceptions of issues related to motivation in learning a foreign language. British Educational Research Journal 28.4, 504528.Google Scholar
Wong, R. M. H. (2014). An investigation of strategies for student motivation in the Chinese EFL context. Innovation in Language Learning and Teaching 8.2, 132154.Google Scholar
Woodrow, L. (2013). Motivation and the transition to university. In Ushioda, E. (ed.), 117–132.Google Scholar
Wu, W.-C., Yen, L. L. & Marek, M. (2011). Using online EFL interaction to increase confidence, motivation, and ability. Educational Technology & Society 14.3, 118129.Google Scholar
Wyatt, M. (2013). Motivating teachers in the developing world: Insights from research with English language teachers in Oman. International Review of Education 59.2, 217242.Google Scholar
Yanguas, I. (2011). The dynamic nature of motivation during the task: Can it be captured? Innovation in Language Learning and Teaching 5.1, 3561.Google Scholar
Yashima, T. (2013). Individuality, imagination and community in a globalizing world. In Benson, P. & Cooker, L. (eds.), The applied linguistic individual. London: Equinox Publishing, 4658.Google Scholar
Yashima, T. & Zenuk-Nishide, L. (2008). The impact of learning contexts on proficiency, attitudes, and L2 communication: Creating an imagined international community. System 36.4, 566585.Google Scholar
Yeung, A. S., Lau, S. & Nie, Y. (2011). Primary and secondary students’ motivation in learning English: Grade and gender differences. Contemporary Educational Psychology 36, 246256.Google Scholar
Yi Tsang, S. (2012). Learning Japanese as a foreign language in the context of an American university: A qualitative and process-oriented study on de/motivation at the learning situation level. Foreign Language Annals 45.1, 130163 Google Scholar
You, C. J. & Dörnyei, Z. (2016). Language learning motivation in China: Results of a large-scale stratified survey. Applied Linguistics 37.4, 495519.Google Scholar
Zentner, M. & Renaud, O. (2007). Origins of adolescents’ ideal self: An intergenerational perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 92.3, 557574.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1 Dörnyei's (2001) taxonomy of motivational strategies

Figure 1

Table 1 Studies of language teachers’ MotS use, based on Dörnyei's 2001 framework

Figure 2

Table 2 Teachers’ views of importance of motivational macro-strategies in five different national contexts

Figure 3

Table 3 Prominent theories of motivation applied to language education

Figure 4

Table 4 A selection of demotivating aspects of L2 classroom teaching