Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-wq2xx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T15:23:23.469Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Semantic and phonemic sequence effects in random word generation: A dissociation between Alzheimer's and Huntington's disease patients

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 May 2005

KIRSTEN I. TAYLOR
Affiliation:
Neuropsychology Unit, Department of Neurology, University Hospital Zürich, Zürich, Switzerland
DAVID P. SALMON
Affiliation:
Department of Neurosciences, School of Medicine, University of California at San Diego, California
ANDREAS U. MONSCH
Affiliation:
Memory Clinic—NPZ, University Hospital Basel, Basel, Switzerland
PETER BRUGGER
Affiliation:
Neuropsychology Unit, Department of Neurology, University Hospital Zürich, Zürich, Switzerland

Abstract

Alzheimer's disease (AD) patients perform worse on category than letter fluency tasks, while Huntington's disease (HD) patients show the reverse pattern or comparable impairment on both tasks. We developed a random word generation task to further investigate these deficits. Twenty AD and 16 HD patients and 20 elderly and 16 middle-aged controls guessed which of three pictures (hat, cat, or dog) landed on a die's top face sixty times. Three consecutive response pairings were possible: semantic (cat–dog), phonemic (hat–cat), and neutral (hat–dog). Since healthy individuals avoid repeating meaningful associates (“repetition avoidance”), an increased pairing frequency reflects processing deficits. AD patients produced more semantic and HD patients more phonemic pairings compared to their respective control groups, indicating selective semantic and phonemic processing deficits in AD and HD patients, respectively. (JINS, 2005, 11, 303–310.)

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2005 The International Neuropsychological Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association Press.
Auchterlonie, S., Phillips, N.A., & Chertkow, H. (2002). Behavioral and electrical brain measures of semantic priming in patients with Alzheimer's disease: Implications for access failure versus deterioration hypotheses. Brain and Cognition, 48, 264267.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory and language: An overview. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36, 189208.Google Scholar
Barr, A. & Brandt, J. (1996). Word-list generation deficits in dementia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 18, 810822.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Binetti, G., Magni, E., Cappa, S.F., Padovani, A., Bianchetti, A., & Trabucchi, M. (1995). Semantic memory in Alzheimer's disease: An analysis of category fluency. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 17, 8289.Google Scholar
Brandt, J., Spencer, M., McSorley, P., & Folstein, M.F. (1988). Semantic activation and implicit memory in Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 2, 112119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brugger, P. (1997). Variables that influence the generation of random sequences: An update. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 84, 627661.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brugger, P., Monsch, A.U., Salmon, D.P., & Butters, N. (1996). Random number generation in dementia of the Alzheimer type: A test of frontal executive functions. Neuropsychologia, 34, 97103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brugger, P., Regard, M., Landis, T., & Graves, R.E. (1995). The roots of meaningful coincidence. Lancet, 345, 13061307.Google Scholar
Butters, N. (1984). The clinical aspects of memory disorders: Contributions from experimental studies of amnesia and dementia. Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 6, 1736.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butters, N., Granholm, E., Salmon, D.P., & Grant, I. (1987). Episodic and semantic memory: A comparison of amnesic and demented patients. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 9, 479497.Google Scholar
Butters, N., Wolfe, J., Martone, M., Granholm, E., & Cermak, L.S. (1985). Memory disorders associated with Huntington's disease: Verbal recall, verbal recognition and procedural memory. Neuropsychologia, 23, 729743.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chan, A.S., Butters, N., Salmon, D.P., & McGuire, K.A. (1993). Dimensionality and clustering in the semantic network of patients with Alzheimer's disease. Psychology and Aging, 8, 411419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chertkow, H. & Bub, D. (1990). Semantic memory loss in dementia of Alzheimer type: What do various measures measure? Brain, 113, 397417.Google Scholar
Elfgren, C.I. & Risberg, J. (1998). Lateralized frontal blood flow increases during fluency tasks: Influence of cognitive strategy. Neuropsychologia, 36, 505512.Google Scholar
Glosser, G. & Friedman, R.B. (1991). Lexical but not semantic priming in Alzheimer's disease. Psychology and Aging, 6, 522527.Google Scholar
Gourovitch, M.L., Kirkby, B.S., Goldberg, T.E., Weinberger, D.R., Gold, J.M., Esposito, G., Van Horn, J.D., & Berman, K.F. (2000). A comparison of rCBF patterns during letter and semantic fluency. Neuropsychology, 14, 353360.Google Scholar
Ho, A.K., Sahakian, B.J., Robbins, T.W., Barker, R.A., Rosser, A.E., & Hodges, J.R. (2002). Verbal fluency in Huntington's disease: A longitudinal analysis of phonemic and semantic clustering and switching. Neuropsychologia, 40, 12771284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hodges, J.R., Salmon, D.P., & Butters, N. (1990). Differential impairment of semantic and episodic memory in Alzheimer's and Huntington's diseases: A controlled prospective study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 53, 10891095.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hodges, J.R., Salmon, D.P., & Butters, N. (1991). The nature of the naming deficit in Alzheimer's and Huntington's disease. Brain, 114, 15471558.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hodges, J.R., Salmon, D.P., & Butters, N. (1992). Semantic memory impairment in Alzheimer's disease: Failure of access or degraded knowledge? Neuropsychologia, 30, 301314.Google Scholar
Jahanshahi, M., Profice, P., Brown, R.G., Ridding, M.C., Dirnberger, G., & Rothwell, J.C. (1998). The effects of transcranial magnetic stimulation over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex on suppression of habitual counting during random number generation. Brain, 121, 15331544.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kempler, D. (1991). Language changes in dementia of the Alzheimer type. In R. Lubinski (Ed.), Dementia and communication, pp. 98114. Philadelphia, Pennsylvania: B.C. Decker.
Kempler, D., Curtiss, S., & Jackson, C. (1987). Syntactic preservation in Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 30, 343350.Google Scholar
Lazzara, M.M., Yonelinas, A.P., & Ober, B.A. (2001). Conceptual implicit memory performance in Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychology, 15, 483491.Google Scholar
Martin, A. & Fedio, P. (1983). Word production and comprehension in Alzheimer's disease: The breakdown of semantic knowledge. Brain and Language, 19, 124141.Google Scholar
Martin, A., Wiggs, C.L., Lalonde, F., & Mack, C. (1994). Word retrieval to letter and semantic cues: A double dissociation in normal subjects using interference tasks. Neuropsychologia, 32, 14871494.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mattis, S. (1976). Mental status examination for organic mental syndrome in the elderly patient. In L. Bellak & T.B. Karasu (Eds.), Geriatric psychiatry. New York: Grune and Stratton.
McKhann, G., Drachman, D., Folstein, M., Katzman, R., Price, D., & Stadlan, E.M. (1984). Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: Report of the NINCDS-ADRDA work group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer's disease. Neurology, 34, 939944.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mickanin, J., Grossman, M., Onishi, K., Auriacombe, S., & Clarke, C. (1994). Verbal and nonverbal fluency in patients with probable Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychology, 8, 385394.Google Scholar
Monsch, A.U., Bondi, M.W., Butters, N., Paulsen, J.S., Salmon, D.P., Brugger, P., & Swenson, M.R. (1994). A comparison of category and letter fluency in Alzheimer's and Huntington's disease. Neuropsychology, 8, 2530.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Monsch, A.U., Bondi, M.W., Butters, N., Salmon, D.P., Katzman, R., & Thal, L.J. (1992). Comparisons of verbal fluency tasks in the detection of dementia of the Alzheimer type. Archives of Neurology, 49, 12531258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Monsch, A.U., Bondi, M.W., Salmon, D.P., Butters, N., Thal, L.J., Hansen, L.A., Wiederholt, W.C., Cahn, D.A., & Klauber, M.R. (1995). Clinical validity of the Mattis Dementia Rating Scale in detecting Dementia of the Alzheimer type. A double cross-validation and application to a community-dwelling sample. Archives of Neurology, 52, 899904.Google Scholar
Moss, H.E., Ostrin, R.K., Tyler, L.K., & Marslen-Wilson, W.D. (1995). Accessing different types of lexical semantic information: Evidence from priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 863883.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nebes, R.D. (1989). Semantic memory in Alzheimer's disease. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 377394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nebes, R.D. & Brady, C.B. (1990). Preserved organization of semantic attributes in Alzheimer's disease. Psychology and Aging, 5, 574579.Google Scholar
Newcombe, F. (1969). Missile wounds of the brain. London: Oxford University Press.
Pasquier, F., Lebert, F., Grymonprez, L., & Petit, H. (1995). Verbal fluency in dementia of frontal lobe type and dementia of Alzheimer type. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 58, 8184.Google Scholar
Pujol, J., Vendrell, P., Deus, J., Kulisevsky, J., Marti-Vilalta, J.L., Garcia, C., Junque, C., & Capdevila, A. (1996). Frontal lobe activation during word generation studies by functional MRI. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 93, 403410.Google Scholar
Randolph, C., Braun, A.R., Goldberg, T.E., & Chase, T.N. (1993). Semantic fluency in Alzheimer's, Parkinson's, and Huntington's disease: Dissociation of storage and retrieval failures. Neuropsychology, 7, 8288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rende, B., Ramsberger, G., & Miyake, A. (2002). Commonalities and differences in the working memory components underlying letter and category fluency tasks: A dual-task investigation. Neuropsychology, 16, 309321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rich, J.B., Troyer, A.K., Bylsma, F.W., & Brandt, J. (1999). Longitudinal analysis of phonemic clustering and switching during word-list generation in Huntington's disease. Neuropsychology, 13, 525531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rohrer, D., Salmon, D.P., Wixted, J.T., & Paulsen, J.S. (1999). The disparate effects of Alzheimer's disease and Huntington's disease on semantic memory. Neuropsychology, 13, 381388.Google Scholar
Rosser, A. & Hodges, J.R. (1994). Initial letter and semantic category fluency in Alzheimer's disease, Huntington's disease, and progressive supranuclear palsy. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 57, 13891394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salmon, D.P., Heindel, W.C., & Lange, K.L. (1999). Differential decline in word generation from phonemic and semantic categories during the course of Alzheimer's disease: Implications for the integrity of semantic memory. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 5, 692703.Google Scholar
Schulman, H.G. (1971). Similarity effects in short-term memory. Psychological Bulletin, 75, 399415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snyder, L.S., Holland, A.K., & Forbes, M. (1996). Lexical decisions in patients with Alzheimer's disease: Some notes on automatic versus controlled processing. Journal of Communication Disorders, 29, 398399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sotrel, A., Paskevich, P.A., Kiely, D.K., Bird, E.D., Williams, R.S., & Myers, R.H. (1991). Morphometric analysis of the prefrontal cortex in Huntington's disease. Neurology, 41, 11171123.Google Scholar
Suhr, J.A. & Jones, R.D. (1998). Letter and semantic fluency in Alzheimer's, Huntington's, and Parkinson's dementias. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 13, 447454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tröster, A.I., Salmon, D.P., McCullough, D., & Butters, N. (1989). A comparison of the category fluency deficits associated with Alzheimer's and Huntington's disease. Brain and Language, 37, 500513.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vecera, S.P., Rothbart, M.K., & Posner, M.I. (1991). Development of spontaneous alternation in infancy. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 351354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weingartner, H.J., Kawas, C., Rawlings, R., & Shapiro, M. (1993). Changes in semantic memory in early stage Alzheimer's disease patients. The Gerontologist, 33, 637643.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiegersma, S. (1982). Can repetition avoidance in randomization be explained by randomness concepts? Psychological Research, 44, 189198.Google Scholar
Wiegersma, S. (1984). Acoustic and semantic similarity effects on repetition avoidance in produced sequences. Memory and Cognition, 12, 190194.Google Scholar