We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
Online ordering will be unavailable from 17:00 GMT on Friday, April 25 until 17:00 GMT on Sunday, April 27 due to maintenance. We apologise for the inconvenience.
To save content items to your account,
please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies.
If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account.
Find out more about saving content to .
To save content items to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org
is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings
on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part
of your Kindle email address below.
Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations.
‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi.
‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
Patients and their families often ask clinicians to estimate when full-time care (FTC) will be needed after Alzheimer's Disease (AD) is diagnosed. Although a few algorithms predictive algorithms for duration to FTC have been created, these have not been widely adopted for clinical use due to questions regarding precision from limited sample sizes and lack of an easy, user friendly prediction model. Our objective was to develop a clinically relevant, data-driven predictive model using machine learning to estimate time to FTC in AD based on information gathered from a) clinical interview alone, and b) clinical interview plus neuropsychological data.
Participants and Methods:
The National Alzheimer's Coordinating Center dataset was used to examine 3,809 participants (M age at AD diagnosis = 76.05, SD = 9.76; 47.10% male; 87.20% Caucasian) with AD dementia who were aged >50 years, had no history of stroke, and not dependent on others for basic activities of daily living at time of diagnosis based on qualitative self or informant report. To develop a predictive model for time until FTC, supervised machine learning algorithms (e.g., gradient descent, gradient boosting) were implemented. In Model 1, 29 variables captured at the time of AD diagnosis and often gathered in a clinical interview, including sociodemographic factors, psychiatric conditions, medical history, and MMSE, were included. In Model 2, additional neuropsychological variables assessing episodic memory, language, attention, executive function, and processing speed were added. To train and test the algorithm(s), data were split into a 70:30 ratio. Prediction optimization was examined via cross validation using 1000 bootstrapped samples. Model evaluation included assessment of confusion matrices and calculation of accuracy and precision.
Results:
The average time to requiring FTC after AD diagnosis was 3.32 years (Range = 0.53-14.57 years). For the clinical interview only model (Model 1), younger age of onset, use of cholinesterase inhibitor medication, incontinence, and apathy were among the clinical variables that significantly predicted duration to FTC, with the largest effects shown for living alone, a positive family history of dementia, and lower MMSE score. In Model 2, the clinical predictors remained significant, and lower Boston Naming Test and Digit-Symbol Coding scores showed the largest effects in predicting duration to FTC among the neuropsychological measures. Final prediction models were further tested using five randomly selected cases. The average estimated time to FTC using the clinical interview model was within an average of 5.2 months of the recorded event and within an average of 5.8 months for the model with neuropsychological data.
Conclusions:
Predicting when individuals diagnosed with AD will need FTC is important as the transition often carries significant financial costs related to caregiving. Duration to FTC was predicted by clinical and neuropsychological variables that are easily obtained during standard dementia evaluations. Implementation of the model for prediction of FTC in cases showed encouraging prognostic accuracy. The two models show promise as a first step towards creation of a user friendly prediction calculator that could help clinicians better counsel patients on when FTC after AD diagnosis may occur, though the development of separate models for use in more diverse populations will be essential.
A diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) requires memory complaint and objective memory impairment. However, some individuals report subjective memory complaints (SMC) despite having intact memory performance, while others demonstrate subtle impairment on memory testing but have no memory complaints; neither case would meet criteria for MCI. This study aimed to compare memory performances over time in individuals who do not meet traditional MCI criteria to those with normal cognition and those who converted to MCI.
Participants and Methods:
Diagnoses for a longitudinal sample from the Texas Alzheimer’s Research and Care Consortium were reviewed by a consensus panel of neuropsychologists and neurologists and reclassified at time of last visit. Diagnostic categories included SMC (i.e., memory complaint but no impairment on testing), objective cognitive impairment but no complaint (Impaired but not MCI), normal control (NC), MCI, and dementia. In this study, 827 participants were divided into 4 groups: 1) NC over 5 visits (n=511, 71% female; 42% Latinx/Hispanic), 2) baseline NC to amnestic MCI (n=62; 63% female; 57% Latinx/Hispanic), 3) SMC at last visit (n=133; 58% female; 70% Latinx/Hispanic), and 4) impaired but not MCI at last visit (n=121; 71% female; 60% Latinx/Hispanic). A memory composite (z-score) was created from the CERAD list-learning task (immediate, delayed, and recognition-discrimination) and Wechsler Memory Scale (Immediate and Delayed Logical Memory and Visual Reproduction) to evaluate memory performance over time. A linear mixed-model adjusting for age, education, sex, ethnicity, and number of APOE e4 alleles evaluated memory performance across 5 visits for the groups. To assess if depression followed a similar course, a linear mixed-model evaluated Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS) scores over time.
Results:
At baseline, groups differed by age (F=22.82; p<.001), education (F=8.60; p<.001), MMSE scores (F=9.38; p<.001), GDS-30 scores (F=3.56; p=.015), and memory composites (F=24.29; p<.001). A significant group X time interaction was observed (F=4.83, p<.001). Memory performance improved in both the SMC and the NC groups, remained stable in the impaired but not MCI group, and declined (as expected) in those who converted to amnestic MCI. Depression scores also showed a significant group X time interaction (F=2.43; p=.004), in which the NC to MCI group endorsed slightly more depression symptoms over time, while other groups declined or remained stable.
Conclusions:
Memory trajectories in this diverse sample differed across groups. Individuals with SMC but without objective memory impairment and normal controls showed some improvement in memory over time, presumably due to practice effects. Those with subtle memory impairments but no complaint (i.e., did not meet MCI criteria) remained stable and those who converted to amnestic MCI had worse memory across time. The stability of memory performances in the impaired not MCI group suggests these subtle memory inefficiencies may be longstanding or unperceived. However, because our sample achieved retrospective diagnoses of SMC and impaired not MCI, it will be important for future studies to prospectively follow these groups to determine which risk factors may predict progression to MCI and what impact ethnicity may have on these trajectories.
Episodic memory functioning is distributed across two brain circuits, one of which courses through the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC). Thus, delivering non-invasive neuromodulation technology to the dACC may improve episodic memory functioning in patients with memory problems such as in amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI). This preliminary study is a randomized, double-blinded, sham-controlled clinical trial to examine if high definition transcranial direct current stimulation (HD-tDCS) can be a viable treatment in aMCI.
Participants and Methods:
Eleven aMCI participants, of whom 9 had multidomain deficits, were randomized to receive 1 mA HD-tDCS (N=7) or sham (N=4) stimulation. HD-tDCS was applied over ten 20-minute sessions targeting the dACC. Neuropsychological measures of episodic memory, verbal fluency, and executive function were completed at baseline and after the last HD-tDCS session. Changes in composite scores for memory and language/executive function tests were compared between groups (one-tailed t-tests with a = 0.10 for significance). Clinically significant change, defined as > 1 SD improvement on at least one test in the memory and non-memory domains, was compared between active and sham stimulation based on the frequency of participants in each.
Results:
No statistical or clinically significant change (N-1 X2; p = 0.62) was seen in episodic memory for the active HD-tDCS (MDiff = 4.4; SD = 17.1) or sham groups (MDiff = -0.5; SD = 9.7). However, the language and executive function composite showed statistically significant improvement (p = 0.04; MDiff = -15.3; SD = 18.4) for the active HD-tDCS group only (Sham MDiff = -5.8; SD = 10.7). Multiple participants (N=4) in the active group had clinically significant enhancement in language and executive functioning tests, while nobody in the sham group did (p = 0.04).
Conclusions:
HD-tDCS targeting the dACC had no direct benefit for episodic memory deficits in aMCI based on preliminary findings for this ongoing clinical trial. However, significant improvement in language and executive function skills occurred in response to HD-tDCS, suggesting HD-tDCS in this configuration has promising potential as an intervention for language and executive function deficits in MCI.
Higher baseline dispersion (intra-individual variability) across neuropsychological test scores at a single time-point has been associated with more rapid cognitive decline, onset of Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) and Alzheimer’s disease (AD), faster rates of hippocampal and entorhinal atrophy, and increased AD neuropathology. Comparison between predictions made from test score dispersion within a cognitive domain versus global, cross-domain dispersion is understudied. Global dispersion may be influenced by ability-and test-specific characteristics. This study examined the performance of global versus domain-specific dispersion metrics to identify which is most predictive of cognitive decline over time.
Participants and Methods:
Data for baseline and five follow-up visits of 308 participants with normal cognition (Mage=73.90, SD=8.12) were selected from the National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center (NACC) Dataset. Participants were required to have no focal neurological deficits, or history of depression, stroke, or heart attack. Diagnoses and progression to MCI and/or dementia were determined at each visit through consensus conferences. Raw neuropsychological scores were standardized using NACC norms. Global baseline dispersion was defined as the intraindividual standard deviation (ISD) across the 10 scores in the NACC battery. Domain-specific dispersions were calculated by constructing composites and ISD was computed across tests sampling their respective domains (executive functioning/attention/processing speed [EFAS], language, and memory; see Table 1 for details on these tests). Higher values on each of these metrics reflect greater dispersion. Multinomial logistic regression model fit statistics and parameter estimates were compared across four different models (global, EFAS, Language, and Memory dispersion) covarying for age, years of education, sex, race, ethnicity, and ApoE4 status. Models were compared using the Likelihood Ratio Test (LRT) and the Akaike Information Criteria (AIC) of Models statistics.
Results:
Of the 308 participants, 70 (22.7%) progressed to MCI, and 82 (26.6%) progressed to dementia. Tables 1 and 2 show the results of the logistic regressions for the four models. All models fit the data well, with statistically significant predictions of conversion. Model 1 (global dispersion) showed a better fit than domain-specific models of dispersion per LRT and AIC values. Consistent with the results from mean differences between groups, parameter estimates showed that only global dispersion and EFAS dispersion significantly predicted conversion to dementia (when included with other covariates in models), with higher dispersion reflecting a greater risk of conversion.
Conclusions:
In this sample, baseline global and EFAS dispersion measures significantly predicted conversion to dementia. Although global dispersion was a stronger predictor of dementia progression, findings suggest that executive functioning performance may be driving this relationship. A single index of global variability, from the calculation of standard deviation across test scores, may be supplementary for clinicians when distinguishing individuals at risk for dementia progression. None of the models were predictive of conversion to MCI. Further research is required to examine cognitive variability differences among patients who progress to MCI and patient-specific factors that may relate to test score dispersion and its utility in predicting the progression of symptoms.
Recommend this
Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this to your organisation's collection.