We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
To save content items to your account,
please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies.
If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account.
Find out more about saving content to .
To save content items to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org
is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings
on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part
of your Kindle email address below.
Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations.
‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi.
‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
People with dementia commonly have impaired social functioning and may not recognise this. This lack of insight may result in worse outcomes for the person and their family carers. We aimed to characterise insight into social functioning in dementia, and describe its association with dementia severity.
Methods:
Observational cross-sectional study of people aged >65 years with clinically diagnosed dementia and their family informants recruited from three sites in Germany, Japan and the United Kingdom. We used the Social Functioning in Dementia scale (SF-DEM), which assesses three domains: “spending time with other people” (domain 1), “communicating with other people” (domain 2), and “sensitivity to other people” (domain 3). We calculated lack of insight into social functioning as the discrepancy between the ratings of the participants with dementia and their informant. We described this discrepancy and the proportion of people with dementia whose rating was overestimated, congruent or underestimated compared to their family informant. We calculated the association between SF-DEM discrepancy score and total mini-mental status examination (MMSE) score and recall and attention/concentration subdomains.
Results:
In 108 participants with dementia (50.9% women), mean age = 78.9 (standard deviation, SD 6.5) years, and mean MMSE score = 22.7 (SD 3.7). Ratings of patients and informants for domain 1 did not differ, but patient-rating was higher than carer-rating for domain 2 (patient-rated score 11.2 (2.5), carer-rated score 10.1 (3.4); p = 0.003) and domain 3 (patient-rated score 9.7 (2.4), carer-rated score 8.1 (2.8); p < 0.001). Sixty (55.6%) people with dementia overestimated their overall social functioning, 30 (27.8%) underestimated, and 18 (16.7%) gave ratings congruent with their family informant. Performance on the MMSE, and its sub-domains was not associated with SF-DEM discrepancy score.
Conclusions:
We found that insight varies according to subdomains of social functioning, with people with dementia rating their communication and sensitivity differently, and usually higher than their carers. Researchers and clinicians should consider insight into social functioning in dementia as a multidimensional, rather than a unified, concept. Clinicians should help family members understand and adapt by explaining their relative with dementia’s lack of insight about aspects of their social functioning.
Recently, artificial intelligence-powered devices have been put forward as potentially powerful tools for the improvement of mental healthcare. An important question is how these devices impact the physician-patient interaction.
Aims
Aifred is an artificial intelligence-powered clinical decision support system (CDSS) for the treatment of major depression. Here, we explore the use of a simulation centre environment in evaluating the usability of Aifred, particularly its impact on the physician–patient interaction.
Method
Twenty psychiatry and family medicine attending staff and residents were recruited to complete a 2.5-h study at a clinical interaction simulation centre with standardised patients. Each physician had the option of using the CDSS to inform their treatment choice in three 10-min clinical scenarios with standardised patients portraying mild, moderate and severe episodes of major depression. Feasibility and acceptability data were collected through self-report questionnaires, scenario observations, interviews and standardised patient feedback.
Results
All 20 participants completed the study. Initial results indicate that the tool was acceptable to clinicians and feasible for use during clinical encounters. Clinicians indicated a willingness to use the tool in real clinical practice, a significant degree of trust in the system's predictions to assist with treatment selection, and reported that the tool helped increase patient understanding of and trust in treatment. The simulation environment allowed for the evaluation of the tool's impact on the physician–patient interaction.
Conclusions
The simulation centre allowed for direct observations of clinician use and impact of the tool on the clinician–patient interaction before clinical studies. It may therefore offer a useful and important environment in the early testing of new technological tools. The present results will inform further tool development and clinician training materials.
Recommend this
Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this to your organisation's collection.