We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
To save content items to your account,
please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies.
If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account.
Find out more about saving content to .
To save content items to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org
is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings
on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part
of your Kindle email address below.
Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations.
‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi.
‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
There has been a surge in individuals seeking neuropsychological assessment for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) after watching social media created by people claiming to have ADHD. While online content may promote destigmatization of ADHD, self-diagnoses derived from social media use may contribute to the development of inaccurate illness beliefs. Individuals who feel strongly connected to social media that mentions personal anecdotes of ADHD might be more likely to believe they also have ADHD. We examined associations between social media search for ADHD and beliefs about everyday experiences being diagnostic of ADHD among adults concerned about having ADHD, as compared to a control group.
Participants and Methods:
A cross-sectional online study included 320 university students (Mage=19.56±2.92; 72% female; 81% White) without history of ADHD. Participants who reported concern about having ADHD, with (n=43) or without other psychological history (n=73) rated whether 100 experiences taken from social media were diagnostic of ADHD, and then rated the amount of time they spent on social media searching for ADHD content. They then rated how often they personally experienced the symptoms. Participants who reported no concern about having ADHD (n=184) only rated how often they personally experienced the symptoms.
Results:
Social media search for ADHD was related to total number of experiences believed to be diagnostic of ADHD among participants concerned about having ADHD without psychological history (r=.28, p=.03), but not for those with psychological history (r=.09, p=.57). For participants concerned about having ADHD (regardless of psychological history), social media search for ADHD was related to total number of symptoms personally experienced (rs=.48-.56, ps<.001) and to the number of symptoms endorsed at a clinical level (rs=.48, ps<.001). Total number of experiences believed to be diagnostic of ADHD was related to the number of symptoms personally experienced among participants concerned about having ADHD with psychological history (r=.53, p<.001; clinical level .47, p=.002), but not for those without psychological history (r=.14, p=.31; clinical level .19, p=.15). Of the 100 symptoms, 56 were believed to be diagnostic of ADHD by at least 50% of participants concerned about having ADHD. Of the 56, 43 were personally experienced at a clinical level by controls. For the 13 remaining symptoms not endorsed at a clinical level by controls, symptoms believed to be diagnostic of ADHD was related to symptoms personally experienced among participants concerned about having ADHD with psychological history (r=.53, p<.001; clinical level .52, p<.001), but not for those without psychological history (r=.14, p=.30; clinical level .19, p=.15).
Conclusions:
Greater social media search for ADHD is related to higher symptom report among individuals concerned about having ADHD regardless of psychological history. However, individuals concerned about having ADHD without psychological history who engage in greater social media use appear to be more likely to believe that general symptoms are specifically due to ADHD. These individuals may be more prone to misattribute symptoms to ADHD. Nearly 77% of symptoms rated as diagnostic of ADHD were frequently experienced by individuals without concern about having ADHD, which demonstrates the high base rate of ADHD-like symptoms in the general population.
Recommend this
Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this to your organisation's collection.