Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-cfpbc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-18T14:28:30.715Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The case for semantic relations: evidence from the verbal and nonverbal domains*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Roberta Michnick Golinkoff
Affiliation:
University of Delaware

Abstract

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Notes and Discussion
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1981

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Antinucci, F. & Parisi, D. (1973). Early language acquisition: a model and some data. In Ferguson, C. A. & Slobin, D. I. (eds), Studies of child language development. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Arlman-Rupp, A. J. L., Van Niekerk de Haan, D. & van de Sandt-Koenderman, M. (1976). Brown's early stages: some evidence from Dutch. JChLang 3. 267–74.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Camaioni, L. & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. MPQ 21. 205–66.Google Scholar
Benedict, H. (1979). Early lexical development: comprehension and production. JChLang 6. 183200.Google ScholarPubMed
Bever, T. G. (1970). The cognitive basis for linguistic structures. In Hayes, J. R. (ed.), Cognition and the development of language. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. (1970). Language development: form and function in emerging grammars. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T.Google Scholar
Bloom, L., Bitetti-Capatides, J. & Tackeff, J. (1981). Further remarks on interpretive analysis: in response to Christine Howe. JChLang 8. 403–11.Google Scholar
Bloom, L., Lightbown, P. & Hood, L. (1975). Structure and variation in child language. Monogr.Soc.Res.Ch.Devel. 40.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1974). Structure relationships in children's utterances: syntactic or semantic? In Schiefelbusch, R. L. & Lloyd, L. L. (eds), Language perspectives – acquisition, retardation and intervention. Maryland: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Braine, M. D. (1963). The ontogeny of English phrase structure: the first phase. Lg 39. 113.Google Scholar
Braine, M. D. & Wells, R. S. (1978). Case-like categories in children: the actor and some related categories. Cognition 10. 100–22.Google Scholar
Bransford, J. D. & McCarrell, N. S. (1974). A sketch of a cognitive approach to comprehension: some thoughts about understanding what it means to comprehend. In Weimer, W. B. & Palermo, D. S. (eds), Cognition and the symbolic process. New Jersey: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: the early stages. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1974). From communication to language: a psychological perspective. Cognition 3. 255–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bushnell, E. W. & Aslin, R. N. (1977). Inappropriate expansion: a demonstration of a methodology for child language research. JChLang 4. 115–22.Google Scholar
Chafe, W. L. (1970). Meaning and the structure of language. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Chapman, R. & Miller, J. (1975). Word order in early two- and three-word utterances: does production precede comprehension? JSHR 18. 355–71.Google Scholar
Clark, H. & Begun, J. S. (1971). The semantics of sentence subjects. Lg&Sp 14. 3446.Google ScholarPubMed
Cohen, L. B. & Gelber, E. R. (1975). Infant visual memory. In Cohen, L. B. & Salapatek, P. (eds), Infant perception: from sensation to cognition. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Dale, P. S. (1980). Is early pragmatic development measurable? JChLang 7. 112.Google ScholarPubMed
de Villiers, J. & de Villiers, P. (1973). Development of the use of word order in comprehension. JPsycholingRes. 2. 331–41.Google ScholarPubMed
Duchan, J. & Lund, N. J. (1979). Why not semantic relations? JChLang 6. 243–52.Google Scholar
Edwards, D. (1973). Sensory-motor intelligence and semantic relations in early child grammar. Cognition 2. 395434.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. (1974). An overview of theories of grammatical development. In Slobin, D. I. (ed.), The ontogenesis of grammar. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Fillmore, C. J. (1968). The case for case. In Bach, E. & Harms, R. T. (eds), Universals in linguistic theory. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Fritz, J. (1977). Semantic comprehension of the action–role relationship in early linguistic infants. Doctoral dissertation, Cornell University.Google Scholar
Gilmore, L. M. (1978). The responses of pre-verbal and early verbal infants to differences in the narration of visual events. Doctoral dissertation, Cornell University.Google Scholar
Gilmore, L. M., Suci, G. & Chan, S. (1974). Heart rate deceleration as a function of viewing complex visual events in 18-month-old infants. Paper presented at the American Psychological Association meetings, New Orleans.Google Scholar
Golinkoff, R. M. (1973). Semantic development in infants: the concept of agent. Doctoral dissertation, Cornell University.Google Scholar
Golinkoff, R. M. (1975). Semantic development in infants: the concept of agent and recipient. MPQ 21. 181–93.Google Scholar
Golinkoff, R. M. (1980 a). Processing visual events: the conceptual status of ‘agent’ and ‘recipient’. In preparation.Google Scholar
Golinkoff, R. M. (1980 b). The influence of Piagetian theory on the study of the development of communication. In Sigel, I., Brodzinski, D. & Golinkoff, R. M. (eds), Piagetian theory and research: new directions and applications. New Jersey: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Golinkoff, R. M. & Harding, C. G. (1978). Infants' perceptions of filmed events portraying case role concepts. Paper presented at the International Conference on Infant Studies, Rhode Island.Google Scholar
Golinkoff, R. M. (1980). The development of causality: the distinction between animates and inanimates. Paper presented at the International Conference on Infant Studies, Connecticut.Google Scholar
Golinkoff, R. M. & Kerr, L. J. (1978). Infants' perceptions of semantically defined action role changes in filmed events. MPQ 24. 5361.Google Scholar
Golinkoff, R. M. & Markessini, J. (1980). ‘Mommy sock’: the child's understanding of possession as expressed in two-noun phrases. JChLang 7. 119–36.Google ScholarPubMed
Gratch, G. (1975). Recent studies based on Piaget's view of object concept development. In Cohen, L. B. & Salapatek, P. (eds), Infant perception, Vol. 2. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. M. & Smith, J. H. (1976). The structure of communication in early language development. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. M. & Zukow, P. G. (1978). Why do children say what they say when they say it: an experimental approach to the psychogenesis of presupposition. In Nelson, K. (ed.), Children's Language, Vol. I. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Harding, C. G. & Golinkoff, R. M. (1979). The origins of intentional vocalizations in prelinguistic infants. ChDev 50. 3340.Google ScholarPubMed
Howe, C. J. (1976). The meanings of two-word utterances in the speech of young children. JChLang 3. 2948.Google Scholar
Johnston, J. R. & Slobin, D. I. (1979). The development of locative expressions in English, Italian, Serbo-Croatian and Turkish. JChLang 6. 529–46.Google ScholarPubMed
Lynn, R. (1966). Attention, arousal, and the orientation reaction. Oxford: Dawson & Goodall.Google Scholar
Macnamara, J. (1972). Cognitive basis of language learning in infants. PsychRev 79. 113.Google ScholarPubMed
Macrae, A. (1979). Combining meanings in early language. In Fletcher, P. & Garman, M. (eds), Language acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
McHale, C. (1973). The development of the semantic concept of action role in preverbal and early verbal infants. Doctoral dissertation, Cornell University.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1974). Concept, word, and sentence: interrelations in acquisition and development. PsychRev 81. 267–85.Google Scholar
Olson, D. R. & Filby, N. (1972). On the comprehension of active and passive sentences. CogPsychol 3. 361–81.Google Scholar
Osgood, C. E. (1971). Where do sentences come from? In Steinberg, D. D. & Jakobovits, L. A. (eds), Semantics. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. New York: Norton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robertson, S. (1975). The cognitive organization of action events: a developmental perspective. Paper presented to the American Psychological Association, Chicago.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1977). Thought and language in infancy: the perception of visual action events. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Cornell University.Google Scholar
Robertson, S. & Suci, G. J. (1980). Event perception by children in the early stages of language production. ChDev 51. 8996.Google ScholarPubMed
Rodgon, M. M. (1977). Situation and meaning in one- and two-word utterances: observations on Howe's ‘The meanings of two-word utterances in the speech of young children’. JChLang 4. 111–14.Google Scholar
Schlesinger, I. M. (1971). Production of utterances and language acquisition. In Slobin, D. I. (ed.), The ontogenesis of grammar. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Schlesinger, I. M. (1974). Relational concepts underlying language. In Schiefelbusch, R. L. & Lloyd, L. L. (eds), Language perspectives – acquisition, retardation and intervention. Maryland: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Schlesinger, I. M. (1977). The role of cognitive development and linguistic input in language acquisition. JChLang 4. 153–70.Google Scholar
Segalowitz, S. (1978). The agent priority in events in motion and still pictures. Paper presented at the Convention of the Canadian Psychological Association, Ottawa.Google Scholar
Shafto, M. (1973). The space for case. JVLVB 12. 551–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shatz, M. (1978). On the development of communicative understandings: an early strategy for interpreting and responding to messages. CogPsychol 10. 271301.Google Scholar
Sinclair-de-Zwart, H. (1973). Language acquisition and cognitive development. In Moore, T. E. (ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1969). Universals of grammatical development in children. Language Behavior Research Laboratory, Working paper 22.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1973). Cognitive prerequisites for the development of grammar. In Ferguson, C. & Slobin, D. (eds), Studies of child language development. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E. (1977). Mothers' speech research: from input to interaction. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds), Talking to children: language input and acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Snyder, L. S. (1978). Communicative and cognitive abilities in the sensorimotor period. MPQ 24. 161–80.Google Scholar
Spilton, D. (1977). Language and the integration of action events in infant perception. Doctoral dissertation, Cornell University.Google Scholar
Strohner, H. & Nelson, K. (1974). The young child's development of sentence comprehension: influences of event probability, nonverbal context, syntactic form, and strategies. ChDev 45. 567–76.Google Scholar
Suci, J. G. (1971). Approaches to the non-linguistic aspects of the comprehension process. Unpublished manuscript, Cornell University.Google Scholar
Suci, J. G. (1978). Cognitive bases for early language. Unpublished paper, Cornell University.Google Scholar
Suci, J. G. & Hamacher, J. (1972). Psychological dimensions of case in sentence processing: action role and animateness. Linguistics 89. 3448.Google Scholar
Weisenburger, J. L. (1976). A choice of words: two-year-old speech from a situational point of view. JChLang 3. 275–82.Google Scholar
Wells, G. (1974). Coding experience through language. JChLang 1. 243–69.Google Scholar
Wetstone, H. & Friedlander, B. (1973). The effects of word order on young children's responses to simple questions and commands. ChDev 44. 734–40.Google Scholar
Wieman, L. (1976). Stress patterns of early child language. JChLang 3. 283–6.Google Scholar