Background
The human gastrointestinal tract is home to trillions of microbial cells that make up an ecosystem that is increasingly recognized as a central component of human physiology, health, and wellbeing (Bordenstein & Theis, Reference Bordenstein and Theis2015; Human Microbiome Project Consortium, Reference Cowan, Dinan and Cryan2012; Peterson et al., Reference Petanjek, Judas, Kostović and Uylings2009). The early postnatal phase of the human life span is a period of remarkable plasticity, characterized by dynamic interplay between extrinsic and intrinsic ecology and systems. The first year of life is characterized by sensitive periods of development for both the brain and gut (Borre et al., Reference Borre, O'Keeffe, Clarke, Stanton, Dinan and Cryan2014; Jašarević, Morrison, & Bale, Reference Huttenlocher and Dabholkar2016; Stilling, Dinan, & Cryan, Reference Stifter and Spinrad2014), and brain maturation coincides with pioneer microbial colonization of the gastrointestinal tract. The early life window of sensitivity of these biological systems is followed by long-term stable phenotypes, thus rendering the first year of life a period during which critical developmental progress occurs (Charbonneau et al., Reference Charbonneau, Blanton, DiGiulio, Relman, Lebrilla, Mills and Gordon2016; Hensch, Reference Hensch2004). Overlapping sensitive periods of these biologically interconnected systems suggests that maturation of the gut microbiome may have lifelong consequences for neuropsychological function. Preclinical studies support this hypothesis and indicate that dysbiosis in the gut during sensitive periods of early life development may detrimentally impact neurodevelopment in ways that shape emotion regulation and affective disorder risk across the life course (Rogers et al., Reference Robertson, Manges, Finlay and Prendergast2016; Sampson & Mazmanian, Reference Sampson and Mazmanian2015). However, few studies have investigated associations between the microbiome and neurodevelopment of humans.
Overlapping sensitive periods of gut and brain development
The first year of life is a sensitive window for development, involving extraordinary plasticity for both the gut and the brain. Developmental plasticity describes the ability of an individual to produce a range of phenotypes depending on the conditions and exposures encountered during development (Gluckman, Cutfield, Hofman, & Hanson, Reference Gluckman, Cutfield, Hofman and Hanson2005). Humans are sensitive to a variety of environmental cues that may shape the trajectory of phenotypic specification (Gluckman, Hanson, Spencer, & Bateson, Reference Gluckman, Hanson, Spencer and Bateson2005). Plasticity is not unlimited and individuals tend to exhibit less plasticity with age due to irreversible canalization (Bateson, Reference Bateson2001). Infancy is a period of heightened neuroplasticity as it is characterized by rapid brain growth (Knickmeyer et al., Reference Klindworth, Pruesse, Schweer, Peplies, Quast, Horn and Glöckner2008; Tau & Peterson, Reference Sung, D'Amico, Cabana, Chau, Koren, Savino and Tancredi2010), massive outgrowth of dendrites and axons, and synaptogenesis alongside synaptic pruning (Huttenlocher & Dabholkar, 1997; Petanjek, Judas, Kostović, & Uylings, Reference Pantoja-Feliciano, Clemente, Costello, Perez, Blaser, Knight and Dominguez-Bello2008). Glial cells proliferate in the subventricular zone of the forebrain, migrate across brain regions, and differentiate into oligodendrocytes and astrocytes (Menn et al., Reference Martino, Morton, Marotz, Thompson, Tripathi, Knight and Zengler2006; Sharon, Sampson, Geschwind, & Mazmanian, Reference Sela and Mills2016), which facilitates synaptic pruning by complement activation and phagocytosis (Hong & Stevens, Reference Hong and Stevens2016).
Coincident with neurodevelopmental progress, human infants exhibit acquisition of cognitive abilities and emotion regulation capabilities during the first year. The development of temperament is a critical aspect of self-regulation that emerges in this period, and infant temperament development is a plastic process influenced by several biological factors (Fox, Henderson, Pérez-Edgar, & White, Reference Fox, Henderson, Pérez-Edgar, White and Fox & M. Luciana2008). However, the specific biological factors that shape inter-individual differences remain unclear. While modest changes in temperament may reflect maturational changes across development (Montroy, Bowles, Skibbe, McClelland, & Morrison, Reference Messaoudi, Violle, Bisson, Desor, Javelot and Rougeot2016), it is considered relatively stable after childhood and features of early temperament have been shown to predict personality and adverse mental health outcomes later in life (Laceulle, Ormel, Vollebergh, van Aken, & Nederhof, Reference Komsi, Räikkönen, Pesonen, Heinonen, Keskivaara, Järvenpää and Strandberg2014; Rothbart & Posner, Reference Rogers and Blissett2015; Sayal, Heron, Maughan, Rowe, & Ramchandani, Reference Savino, Quartieri, De Marco, Garro, Amaretti, Raimondi and Rossi2014). Negative affectivity and emotional reactivity, especially, are associated with risk of behavioral and emotional problems in childhood (Abulizi, Pryor, Michel, Melchior, & van der Waerden, Reference Abulizi, Pryor, Michel, Melchior and van der Waerden2017), as well as later depressive and anxiety symptoms (Compas, Connor-Smith, & Jaser, Reference Compas, Connor-Smith and Jaser2004; De Pauw & Mervielde, Reference De Pauw and Mervielde2010), attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (Sullivan et al., Reference Stinson2015), and autism (Clifford, Hudry, Elsabbagh, Charman, & Johnson, Reference Clifford, Hudry, Elsabbagh, Charman and Johnson2013).
The first year is also a sensitive window for gut microbiome development. From birth to age 1 year, the gut microbiome undergoes significant changes, resulting in a community structure that is much more stable after 1 year of age and virtually matured to adult status by 3 years of age (Yatsunenko et al., Reference Yassour, Vatanen, Siljander, Hämäläinen, Härkönen, Ryhänen and Xavier2012). The first few days of life set the developmental trajectory of the gut microbiome as pioneer species of Escherichia, Staphylococcus, and Streptococcus typically dominate and produce anaerobic environments (Pantoja-Feliciano et al., Reference Palmer, Bik, DiGiulio, Relman and Brown2013), inviting subsequent growth of anaerobic genera such as Bifidobacterium and Bacteroides (Cong, Henderson, Graf, & McGrath, Reference Cong, Henderson, Graf and McGrath2015). During the first few weeks of life, the gut typically comes to be dominated by Bacteroides, Bifidobacterium, Parabacteroides, Escherichia, and Shigella (Bäckhed et al., Reference Bäckhed, Roswall, Peng, Feng, Jia, Kovatcheva-Datchary and Wang2015). Specifically, newborn gut microbial colonization begins with microbial communities from the mother's birth canal ingested during parturition (Dominguez-Bello, Blaser, Ley, & Knight, Reference Dominguez-Bello, Blaser, Ley and Knight2011; Jašarević et al., Reference Huttenlocher and Dabholkar2016; Jašarević, Rodgers, & Bale, Reference Jašarević, Morrison and Bale2015; Mueller, Bakacs, Combellick, Grigoryan, & Dominguez-Bello, Reference Morais, Golubeva, Moloney, Moya-Pérez, Ventura-Silva, Arboleya and Cryan2015; Pantoja-Feliciano et al., Reference Palmer, Bik, DiGiulio, Relman and Brown2013; Song, Dominguez-Bello, & Knight, Reference Sommer and Bäckhed2013), followed by breast milk (Le Doare, Holder, Bassett, & Pannaraj, Reference Laceulle, Ormel, Vollebergh, van Aken and Nederhof2018), and subsequent exposure to extrinsic environmental exposures (Biasucci et al., Reference Biasucci, Rubini, Riboni, Morelli, Bessi and Retetangos2010; Dominguez-Bello et al., Reference Dominguez-Bello, Costello, Contreras, Magris, Hidalgo, Fierer and Knight2010; Palmer, Bik, DiGiulio, Relman, & Brown, Reference O'Mahony, Clarke, Dinan and Cryan2007). Within population-level trends, there is individual-level variation in timing and community structure (Bäckhed et al., Reference Bäckhed, Roswall, Peng, Feng, Jia, Kovatcheva-Datchary and Wang2015; Sharon et al., Reference Sela and Mills2016; Yassour et al., Reference Worobey and Blajda2016). The colonization of the gut microbiome also exhibits phenotypic variability as human infants present different microbial composition across environmental and cultural contexts (Sprockett et al., Reference Song, Dominguez-Bello and Knight2020).
Gut microbiota and neurodevelopment
Many of the phenotypes that affect health are connected to the gastrointestinal system (the gut controls energy availability and coordinates metabolic processes that fuel central and somatic functions) and the central nervous system. Furthermore, the gut and brain are physiologically connected in a bidirectional communication and control system. Multiple pathways link the gut microbiome and brain, including vagal nerve innervation, microbial production of neuromodulatory metabolites, and alterations to innate immunity (Jašarević et al., Reference Jašarević, Morrison and Bale2015). The vagus nerve is a two-way neural connection between the gut and brain, with a sensitive period of development of enteric axon terminals occurring early in postnatal life (Ratcliffe, Farrar, & Fox, Reference Quast, Pruesse, Yilmaz, Gerken, Schweer, Yarza and Glöckner2011) – a process that could plausibly be influenced by gut microbiota (Sommer & Bäckhed, Reference Sharon, Sampson, Geschwind and Mazmanian2013). In this way, the “microbiome–gut–brain axis” is fundamentally implicated in biological regulation of health.
While it has been speculated that dysbiosis in the gut during sensitive periods of early life development may detrimentally impact neurodevelopment (Diaz Heijtz, Reference Diaz Heijtz2016; O'Mahony, Clarke, Dinan, & Cryan, Reference Ogbonnaya, Clarke, Shanahan, Dinan, Cryan and O'Leary2017), few studies have examined the change in gut microbiome across infancy in humans. One prospective longitudinal study that included multiple assessments of microbiome across infancy found that alpha diversity at 12 months of age was negatively associated with neurodevelopment (cognitive and language scores) at age 2 years (Carlson et al., Reference Carlson, Xia, Azcarate-Peril, Goldman, Ahn, Styner and Knickmeyer2018). A longitudinal study of 201 children with fecal microbiome composition data at 1, 6, and 12 months of age found that an abundance of Prevotella was inversely associated with increased internalizing symptoms at age 2 years (Loughman, Ponsonby, et al., Reference Litvak and Bäumler2020).
To our knowledge, only two studies have investigated associations between composition of the gut microbiome and temperament in human infants, and one study in toddlers. Wang et al. (Reference Vuong, Pronovost, Williams, Coley, Siegler, Qiu and Hsiao2020) found that an abundance of the genus Bifidobacterium was positively associated with soothability and a relative abundance of Hungatella was negatively associated with cuddliness in 12-month-old infants. In a subcohort from the FinnBrain Birth Cohort Study, Aatsinki et al. (Reference Aatsinki, Lahti, Uusitupa, Munukka, Keskitalo, Nolvi and Karlsson2019) found that higher abundances of Bifidobacterium and Streptococcus and a lower abundance of Atopobium at age 2.5 months were associated with greater surgency/extraversion scores, measured using the Infant Behavior Questionnaire-Revised (IBQ-R), in 6-month-old infants. These results were sex-specific, with only boys showing associations between Bifidobacterium and surgency. The third study investigated associations between the gut microbiome and temperament in toddlers aged 18–27 months (Christian et al., Reference Christian, Galley, Hade, Schoppe-Sullivan, Kamp Dush and Bailey2015). It found that gut microbial phylogenetic diversity was positively associated with surgency/extraversion.
While these limited studies support associations between gut microbiome composition and neurodevelopment in infancy, more longitudinal studies are needed to examine the role of the microbiome in the development of infant temperament. It is also necessary to examine prospective effects at earlier ages than previously have been considered, given the dramatic changes that occur in gut microbial composition across the first 2 months of life.
This study
The aim of this exploratory study was to investigate associations between gut microbiome composition across the first year of life and infant temperament. We took a longitudinal approach and quantified associations between gut microbiome composition (diversity and genera) at ages 1–3 weeks, 2, 6, and 12 months and temperament at 12 months of age. This is the first study, to our knowledge, to investigate associations between composition of the gut microbiome as early as 1–3 weeks of age (or any timepoint earlier than 2.5 months of age) and temperament later in infancy. We conducted exploratory analyses with three validated dimensions of temperament. When a dimension exhibited a significant relationship with microbiome composition, we conducted a follow-up exploration of how each component subscale for that domain related to microbiome composition in order to check whether a particular subcomponent was driving the trend. Following the extant literature, we hypothesized that microbiome composition would be associated with variation in temperament in the dimensions of surgency/extraversion and negative affectivity. This study sheds light on the biological mechanisms that influence inter-individual differences in temperament.
Method
Cohort
This project utilized data from a larger, prospective, longitudinal cohort study of mother–child dyads in Southern California, the Pregnancy Experiences and Infant Development Study (PEIDS) (P50/MH096889). Women were offered voluntary participation in PEIDS, recruited through their clinicians’ offices, email, and print announcements. Written informed consent was obtained from mothers for their own and their infants’ participation after full study procedures were described. PEIDS and our microbiome substudy were approved by the institutional review boards of participating institutions. Our study adhered to the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki. We capitalized on PEIDS data collection in which visits occurred every few weeks and involved mother–child psychological, behavioral, cognitive, and biomedical assessments. This study included a subset of the cohort and involved four sessions: a home visit 1–3 weeks after birth and sessions at a clinical research site when the child was aged 2, 6, and 12 months. This subset comprised infants who produced stool during these sessions. Therefore, participants were not preselected or actively selected for the substudy because it was based on the random occurrence of when the infant produced stool.
Sample collection and processing
Visit protocols involved ~2.5 hr of assessments, both related and unrelated to the current project. When the infant produced stool, the diaper was collected by study staff, who covered the stool with film to seal the sample during transport. For home visits, the entire diaper was then sealed in a plastic bag and transported in a hard-sided cooler to the laboratory (maximum 45 min). Visits at age 2, 6, and 12 months occurred at a clinical site with a laboratory. The study staff transferred stool into OMNIgene gut collection kits (OMR-200, DNA Genotek), aliquoted the mixture into cryovials, and stored them at −80°C.
Infant temperament
To assess infant temperament, mothers completed the IBQ-R when the infant was 12 months old (Gartstein & Rothbart, Reference Gartstein and Rothbart2003). The IBQ-R includes 191 questions addressing concrete behaviors; for example, “During a peek-a-boo game, how often did the baby smile” and “How often during the last week did the baby startle to a sudden or loud noise.” The IBQ-R was developed to reduce the possibility of maternal reporting bias by asking about specific behaviors in defined situations, rather than asking for judgments about child temperament or behaviors. Responses on these scales range from 1 = never to 7 = always. The IBQ-R measures three broad dimensions of temperament: negative affectivity, surgency/extraversion, and orienting/regulation. The negative affectivity dimension is created by averaging scores across four subscales assessing sadness, fear, falling reactivity, and distress to limitations. The surgency/extraversion dimension consists of six subscales assessing approach, vocal reactivity, high-intensity pleasure, smiling/laughter, activity level, and perceptual sensitivity. The orienting/regulation dimension comprises the subscales cuddliness/affiliation, low-intensity pleasure, duration of orienting, and soothability. This widely used parental-report instrument exhibits good internal reliability and validity (Goldsmith & Campos, Reference Goldsmith and Campos1990; Worobey & Blajda, Reference Wasser, Bentley, Borja, Davis Goldman, Thompson, Slining and Adair1989), and correlates well with infant behavioral observations (Worobey & Blajda, Reference Wasser, Bentley, Borja, Davis Goldman, Thompson, Slining and Adair1989). In our cohort, IBQ-R dimensions had Cronbach's alphas as follows: negative affectivity, α = .77; surgency/extraversion, α = .95; orienting/regulation, α = .89; subscales mean α = .80, standard deviation (SD) = .04.
16S ribosomal RNA gene sequencing
Stool samples were submitted to DNA Genotek for DNA extraction and sequencing of the V3–V4 region of the 16S ribosomal RNA gene by Illumina MiSeq v3 according to a published protocol (Klindworth et al., Reference Kim, Sitarik, Woodcroft, Johnson and Zoratti2013). DADA2 was used to perform quality filtering, merge paired end reads, remove chimeras, and cluster sequences into exact amplicon sequence variants (Callahan et al., Reference Callahan, McMurdie, Rosen, Han, Johnson and Holmes2016). Forward reads were truncated to 280 base pairs and reverse reads to 220 base pairs. Reads were removed if the expected error rate exceeded two base pairs or if a single nucleotide had a Phred score of two or less. After these processing steps, the sequence depth ranged from 6,958 to 72,100 with a mean of 30,398. Taxonomy was assigned for amplicon sequence variants based on the SILVA database down to the level of family, genus, or species, depending on the depth of reliable classifier assignments (Quast et al., Reference Peterson, Garges, Giovanni, McInnes, Wang, Schloss and Guyer2013).
Bioinformatics and statistical methods
Significance in differential mean relative abundances of each phylum, family, and genus across timepoints was determined using Kruskal–Wallis test. Microbial alpha diversity was assessed on data sets rarefied to equal sequencing depth (6,958) using the Chao1 index (richness) and Shannon index (evenness and richness). Significance for differences in alpha diversity measures by age was determined using one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) adjusting for subject to control for effects of repeated sampling from the same individual. Beta diversity of the unrarefied genus-level data set after removing genera that were present in less than 10% of the samples was calculated using robust Aitchison distances implemented with the DEICODE plugin in QIIME 2, then visualized with principal coordinates analysis (Martino et al., Reference Martin, Makino, Cetinyurek Yavuz, Ben-Amor, Roelofs, Ishikawa and Knol2019). The significance of differences in beta diversity was assessed using permutational multivariate analysis of variance (PERMANOVA). Significance testing for changes in relative abundance at phylum level by age was performed by one-way ANOVA, adjusting for subject. The differential abundance of microbial genera present in at least 10% of the samples was determined using multivariate negative binomial mixed models in DESeq2 (Love, Huber, & Anders, Reference Loughman, Quinn, Nation, Reichelt, Moore, Van and Tang2014). The unrarefied genus counts were normalized by the size factor (median value of all ratios for a given sample). Results of differential abundance testing were adjusted for multiple hypotheses testing with a significance threshold of false discovery rate <0.1.
For the multivariate models, a two-step process was implemented to decide which covariates to include. First, a list of potential covariates was identified based on previous literature indicating the possibility of relationships with both infant temperament and microbiome composition, or otherwise justified by previous literature. To this end, infant sex (Gartstein & Rothbart, Reference Gartstein and Rothbart2003; Martin et al., Reference Mangelsdorf, Schoppe, Buur, Molfese and McCrae2000), breastfeeding duration (Ho et al., Reference Ho, Li, Lee-Sarwar, Tun, Brown, Pannaraj and Kuhn2018; Rogers & Blissett, Reference Rogers, Keating, Young, Wong, Licinio and Wesselingh2019; Wasser et al., Reference Wang, Chen, Yu, Liu, Zhang and Bai2011), and antibiotic use (Kim, Sitarik, Woodcroft, Johnson, & Zoratti, Reference Jašarević, Rodgers and Bale2019) were selected as covariates based on previous studies. Breastfeeding duration included both exclusive and mixed feeding, as long as breastmilk was being given. The mode of delivery was also included because all three previous studies of the relationship between early life temperament and gut microbiome utilized this control variable (Aatsinki et al., Reference Aatsinki, Lahti, Uusitupa, Munukka, Keskitalo, Nolvi and Karlsson2019; Carlson et al., Reference Carlson, Xia, Azcarate-Peril, Goldman, Ahn, Styner and Knickmeyer2018; Loughman, Ponsonby, et al., Reference Litvak and Bäumler2020). It should also be noted that mode of delivery appears to influence brain (Castillo-Ruiz, Mosley, Jacobs, Hoffiz, & Forger, Reference Castillo-Ruiz, Mosley, Jacobs, Hoffiz and Forger2018), gut microbial composition, and social behavior (Morais et al., Reference Montroy, Bowles, Skibbe, McClelland and Morrison2020) in animal models.
Second, each variable was tested in a univariable model predicting beta diversity outcomes using the age-based subsets or the entire data set after adjusting for age (Supplementary Material Table S1). Selection criteria for covariates included at least one significant (p < .05) association with microbial beta diversity in any subgroup. We did not adjust for variables that had no statistical relationship with microbiome composition at any timepoint. Consequently, we adjusted the multivariate models for infant sex and breastfeeding duration, and not mode of delivery or antibiotic use.
Our statistical methods involved the following analyses. (a) We assessed alpha diversity differences by age as measured by the Chao1 index (species richness) and the Shannon index (evenness and richness) by ANOVA, adjusting for subject. (b) We assessed beta diversity differences by age using PERMANOVA, adjusting for subject, and visualized the effect using principal coordinates analysis plots of the microbial beta diversity measured by DEICODE distances. (c) We compared the mean relative abundances in each age group of bacterial clades at phylum, family, and genus levels. (d) We assessed the association between alpha diversity at each timepoint and IBQ-R scores at 12 months of age using multivariate linear regression models, adjusting for infant sex and breastfeeding duration. (e) We assessed the association between beta diversity at each timepoint and IBQ-R scores at 12 months of age using PERMANOVA, adjusting for infant sex and breastfeeding duration. (f) For significant associations encountered in step (e), we conducted differential abundance testing to identify specific genera associated with the corresponding IBQ-R scores at 12 months of age.
Results
Gut microbial diversity and composition change across infant age groups
In total, 91 samples were collected at different ages (1–3 weeks, 2, 6, and 12 months) from 67 infant donors (Table 1). Similar to findings from other infant microbiome studies (Hill et al., Reference Hill, Lynch, Murphy, Ulaszewska, Jeffery, O'Shea and Stanton2017; Niu et al., Reference Neufeld, Kang, Bienenstock and Foster2020), we found a significant increase in alpha diversity with age according to the Chao1 (p < .001) and Shannon (p = .027) indices (Figures 1a and 1b). Beta diversity analysis demonstrated significant microbial community alterations by age after adjusting for participant to account for inter-individual differences (Figure 1c; p < .001). At the phylum level, the mean relative abundance of Firmicutes increased (p = .005) with age and the mean relative abundance of Proteobacteria decreased (p = .004) with age after adjusting for the participant (Figure 1d; Supplementary Material Figure S1). Changes in Firmicutes were largely driven by increases in genera Blautia and Faecalibacterium with age, which belong to families Lachnospiraceae and Ruminococcaceae, respectively. Changes in Proteobacteria were driven by decreases in genera Klebsiella, Escherichia/Shigella, and Serratia, all of which belong to the family Enterobacteriaceae. Genera Bacteroides (13%–22%) and Bifidobacterium (15%–32%), belonging to phyla Bacteroidetes and Actinobacteria, respectively, were present in abundance throughout the first year of life (Figures 1d–1f).
a Delivery types for non-Cesarean section include normal spontaneous vaginal delivery, vaginal birth after cesarean, outlet or low forceps, outlet or low vacuum, mid forceps, and unknown.
b White, European, North African, Middle Eastern.
c African American or Black.
d Pregnancy history counts before current pregnancy.
Gut microbiota composition associations with temperament
We investigated the relationship between gut microbiota at each age group and IBQ-R scores at 12-months of age (Table 1 and Supplementary Material Table S2) by alpha diversity (Supplementary Material Table S3) and beta diversity measures (Table 2). We found a trend toward the Shannon index of fecal microbiota at age 2 months being negatively associated with negative affectivity score at 12 months of age, though this was not statistically significant (β = −0.57, p = .06). No other IBQ-R domain or subscale at age 12 months demonstrated a significant association with the microbial alpha diversity measures at different ages. We found that gut microbial beta diversity at age 1–3 weeks was associated with surgency/extraversion (R 2 = 0.276, p = .012) as well as its subscales, including approach (R 2 = 0.285, p = .010), high-intensity pleasure (R 2 = 0.275, p = .013), and smiling/laughter (R 2 = 0.273, p = .013) (Figures 2a–2d). In addition, we found a trend toward an association between concurrent gut microbial beta diversity with negative affectivity (R 2 = 0.101, p = .094), although it did not reach statistical significance, and an association with its sadness subscale (R 2 = 0.126, p = .047) at 12 months of age (Figures 3a and 3b).
a * indicates a p value ≤ 0.05.
Individual taxa associated with temperament
Based on the relationships between microbiota beta diversity at age 1–3 weeks and IBQ-R scores at age 12 months, we then performed differential abundance testing to identify specific genera from the infant gut microbiota that are associated with the corresponding IBQ-R scores at age 12 months. Of note, genus Bifidobacterium, an unclassified Lachnospiraceae, and genus Collinsella were positively associated with the surgency/extraversion scale as well as two or more of its subscales (activity level, approach, smiling/laughter, or perceptual sensitivity) at age 12 months (Figure 2e). In addition, there was a negative association between genus Klebsiella in the microbiota at age 1–3 weeks and the surgency/extraversion scores at age 12 months (Figure 2e). Although there was a significant association between beta diversity at age 1–3 weeks and high-intensity pleasure scores at 12 months (Figure 2c; p = .013), no individual taxa were associated with this IBQ-R subscale.
Next, we investigated whether any individual taxa in the concurrent microbiome were associated with the negative affectivity domain score and its subscales at age 12 months. This analysis revealed genera Megamonas, Acidaminococcus and Ruminococcus-1 to be positively associated with negative affectivity and two or more of its subscales (sadness, distress to limitations, falling reactivity, and fear) at age 12 months (Figure 3c). In addition, negative affectivity and its subscales sadness and distress to limitations showed significant negative associations with genus Lactobacillus (Figure 3c).
Discussion
We found that intestinal microbial composition and diversity of infants at 1–3 weeks, 2 months, and 12 months of age were associated with two temperament domains – surgency/extraversion and negative affectivity – at age 12 months. This study adds to the growing literature demonstrating associations between gut microbiome composition and temperament in infancy. While exploratory, our results also suggest the potential existence of sensitive periods during which the coinciding maturation of the gut microbiome and brain may have an influence on infant neurodevelopmental outcomes.
Gut microbiome diversity and temperament
Previous studies have shown that the composition of the gut microbiome in early infancy is associated with individual differences in temperament in late infancy and early childhood that are predictive of risk for affective disorders in childhood and adulthood. We found that alpha diversity and beta diversity in early infancy were associated with IBQ-R scores at age 12 months. Alpha diversity at age 2 months was inversely associated with negative affectivity scores at age 12 months, although it did not reach the standard criteria for statistical significance (p = .06). This is consistent with another study that reported negative associations between gut microbiome alpha diversity at age 2.5 months and negative affectivity scores at age 6 months (Aatsinki et al., Reference Aatsinki, Lahti, Uusitupa, Munukka, Keskitalo, Nolvi and Karlsson2019). The significant association between beta diversity and surgency/extraversion observed in this study is also consistent with previous work. A study of gut microbiome composition and temperament of toddlers similarly detected a positive relationship between phylogenetic diversity (another alpha diversity measure) and surgency/extraversion (Christian et al., Reference Christian, Galley, Hade, Schoppe-Sullivan, Kamp Dush and Bailey2015).
Taken together, our results suggest that gut microbial diversity in early infancy predicts temperament traits related to negative affectivity and surgency/extraversion, which have been elsewhere related to lifetime risk for developing affective disorders. Negative affectivity during infancy is associated with risk for developing depressive and anxiety symptoms later in life (Abulizi et al., Reference Abulizi, Pryor, Michel, Melchior and van der Waerden2017; Compas et al., Reference Compas, Connor-Smith and Jaser2004; De Pauw & Mervielde, Reference De Pauw and Mervielde2010). Surgency/extraversion in infancy has shown mixed associations with mental health outcomes later in life. For example, it is associated with greater self-regulation in childhood and lower risk for depressive symptoms (Komsi et al., Reference Knickmeyer, Gouttard, Kang, Evans, Wilber, Smith and Gilmore2006; Rothbart & Posner, Reference Rogers and Blissett2015). However, surgency/extraversion scores in infancy have also been associated with adverse outcomes such as negative peer behaviors and externalizing problems in childhood and adolescence (Berdan, Keane, & Calkins, Reference Berdan, Keane and Calkins2008; Dollar & Stifter, Reference Dollar and Stifter2012; Honomichl & Donnellan, Reference Honomichl and Donnellan2012). We are aware of only one longitudinal study with multiple assessments of infant gut microbiome composition and child outcomes, which reported no associations between alpha or beta diversity at 1, 6, or 12 months of age with behavioral problems, measured using the Childhood Behavioral Checklist at age 2 (Loughman, Ponsonby, et al., Reference Litvak and Bäumler2020). This suggests that the association between gut microbial alpha and beta diversity in early infancy and behavioral outcomes may be attenuated by 2 years of age. More longitudinal studies are needed in this area as questions about the role of microbial diversity in shaping neurodevelopmental outcomes, such as temperament, remain unanswered.
Microbial taxa and temperament
We replicated several associations between microbial composition and temperament reported in previous studies. We found a positive association between Bifidobacterium at 1–3 weeks and surgency/extraversion scores. Bifidobacterium is an essential group of bacteria that digest human milk oligosaccharides that are otherwise indigestible (Gnoth, Kunz, Kinne-Saffran, & Rudloff, Reference Gnoth, Kunz, Kinne-Saffran and Rudloff2000; Sela & Mills, Reference Sayal, Heron, Maughan, Rowe and Ramchandani2010). Human milk oligosaccharides are believed to regulate development of the gut microbiome by promoting the growth of beneficial bacteria and preventing pathogenic bacteria from colonizing the infant gut (Liévin et al., Reference Le Doare, Holder, Bassett and Pannaraj2000). Bifidobacterium appears to be an important predictor of infant temperament, as a similar study also detected a significant positive association between Bifidobacterium at age 2.5 months and surgency/extraversion scores at age 6 months (Aatsinki et al., Reference Aatsinki, Lahti, Uusitupa, Munukka, Keskitalo, Nolvi and Karlsson2019). Our results suggest that the presence of Bifidobacterium may be important as early as 1–3 weeks after birth. Such results point to the potential significance of this genus in the development of extraversion, given the relationship between surgency/extraversion scores in infancy and surgency later in childhood (Komsi et al., Reference Knickmeyer, Gouttard, Kang, Evans, Wilber, Smith and Gilmore2006). A study in toddlers reported that surgency scores were associated, for boys only, with different genera from those observed in this study (Parabacteroides, Dialister, and Rikenellaceae) (Christian et al., Reference Christian, Galley, Hade, Schoppe-Sullivan, Kamp Dush and Bailey2015).
We also found associations between several genera at age 1–3 weeks and 12 months with surgency/extraversion. We found a positive association with an unclassified Lachnospiraceae and a negative association with Klebsiella. The associations with Lachnospiraceae are generally consistent with results from a longitudinal study that reported positive associations between this bacteria at age 12 months and internalizing problems in 2-year-old children (Loughman, Ponsonby, et al., Reference Litvak and Bäumler2020). The negative associations between Klebsiella and surgency/extraversion may not be surprising as Klebsiella is pathogenic and thought to be a gas-producing bacteria that may cause intestinal discomfort (Savino et al., Reference Sampson and Mazmanian2011). Klebsiella levels have been implicated in infant colic and infants with colic have low emotional regulation (Loughman, Quinn, et al., Reference Loughman, Ponsonby, O'Hely, Symeonides, Collier, Tang and Vuillermin2020; Savino et al., Reference Savino, Cordisco, Tarasco, Locatelli, Di Gioia, Oggero and Matteuzzi2017; Stifter & Spinrad, Reference Stiemsma and Michels2002).
We found associations between several concurrent microbial taxa and negative affectivity scores at age 12 months. This included a positive association with Ruminococcus-1, consistent with previously observed associations of the Ruminococcaceae family with depressive symptoms in adults (Valles-Colomer et al., Reference Tau and Peterson2019). We also found a negative association between Lactobacillus and negative affectivity, which is consistent with the extant literature. Supplementation with probiotics containing Lactobacillus strains has been shown to reduce crying behaviors of infants with colic (Sung et al., Reference Sullivan, Holton, Nousen, Barling, Sullivan, Propper and Nigg2018) and symptoms of anxiety and depression in adults (Messaoudi et al., Reference Menn, Garcia-Verdugo, Yaschine, Gonzalez-Perez, Rowitch and Alvarez-Buylla2011).
The mechanisms by which specific microbial taxa may influence behavior remain unclear. However, mouse models offer potential mechanisms: gut microbial composition was demonstrated to influence neurological biomechanisms such as the expression of neurotransmitters and their hormones, including dopamine and serotonin (Clarke et al., Reference Clarke, Grenham, Scully, Fitzgerald, Moloney, Shanahan and Cryan2013; Diaz Heijtz et al., Reference Diaz Heijtz, Wang, Anuar, Qian, Björkholm, Samuelsson and Pettersson2011; Neufeld, Kang, Bienenstock, & Foster, Reference Mueller, Bakacs, Combellick, Grigoryan and Dominguez-Bello2011). Another potential mechanism is differences in neuronal survival and neurogenesis in regions such as the striatum, amygdala, and hippocampus (Diaz Heijtz et al., Reference Diaz Heijtz, Wang, Anuar, Qian, Björkholm, Samuelsson and Pettersson2011; Ogbonnaya et al., Reference Niu, Xu, Qian, Sun, Yu, Huang and Gao2015; Stilling et al., Reference Stilling, Dinan and Cryan2015). The gut microbiome may also influence brain function by regulating microglial activity (Erny et al., Reference Erny, Hrabě de Angelis, Jaitin, Wieghofer, Staszewski, David and Prinz2015).
Sensitive periods in gut microbiome and neurodevelopment
The sensitive periods during which the gut microbiome may influence neurodevelopment and behavior are unknown. A growing body of work suggests that infancy may be a sensitive period in which the microbiome may have far-reaching influence on later neuropsychological health and behavior (Borre et al., Reference Borre, O'Keeffe, Clarke, Stanton, Dinan and Cryan2014; Jašarević et al., Reference Huttenlocher and Dabholkar2016; Stilling et al., Reference Stifter and Spinrad2014), although some scholars have argued that it may extend into the first 1,000 days of life or even into adolescence (Cowan, Dinan, & Cryan, Reference Cowan, Dinan and Cryan2020; Robertson, Manges, Finlay, & Prendergast, Reference Ratcliffe, Farrar and Fox2019). Early life may be a particularly important period due to founder species that may influence the long-term composition of the gut microbiome (Litvak & Bäumler, Reference Liévin, Peiffer, Hudault, Rochat, Brassart, Neeser and Servin2019). The first year of life is a dynamic period of brain development involving the formation of functional networks (Gao, Lin, Grewen, & Gilmore, Reference Gao, Lin, Grewen and Gilmore2017; Knickmeyer et al., Reference Klindworth, Pruesse, Schweer, Peplies, Quast, Horn and Glöckner2008). Infants exhibit rapid social and emotional development, including the emergence of temperament. The early postnatal phase is one of heightened plasticity, and perturbations to the gut and brain may have far-reaching consequences for mental health risk over the life course. Our results, and those of previous studies, provide preliminary evidence to support infancy as a sensitive period during which gut microbiome composition may impact neurodevelopmental outcomes (Aatsinki et al., Reference Aatsinki, Lahti, Uusitupa, Munukka, Keskitalo, Nolvi and Karlsson2019; Carlson et al., Reference Carlson, Xia, Azcarate-Peril, Goldman, Ahn, Styner and Knickmeyer2018; Christian et al., Reference Christian, Galley, Hade, Schoppe-Sullivan, Kamp Dush and Bailey2015). The importance of these overlapping windows of development are increasingly recognized as scholars have hypothesized that the microbiome may be a mechanism in the developmental origins of health and disease (Stiemsma & Michels, Reference Sprockett, Martin, Costello, Burns, Holmes, Gurven and Relman2018; Stinson, Reference Stilling, Ryan, Hoban, Shanahan, Clarke, Claesson and Cryan2020). Mouse models suggest that early life stages are sensitive periods in which the microbiome influences neurodevelopment. The maternal gut microbiome may regulate fetal brain development in utero through the production of microbial metabolites that promote axonogenesis (Vuong et al., Reference Valles-Colomer, Falony, Darzi, Tigchelaar, Wang, Tito and Raes2020). Another pathway is through transmission of dysbiosis from mothers to pups, which disrupts social and behavioral developmental processes (Buffington et al., Reference Buffington, Di Prisco, Auchtung, Ajami, Petrosino and Costa-Mattioli2016; Champagne-Jorgensen et al., Reference Champagne-Jorgensen, Mian, Kay, Hanani, Ziv, McVey Neufeld and Bienenstock2020). Evidence suggests that weaning and adolescence are transitions marked by microbial instability and these are thus other likely sensitive periods for the development of the microbiome–gut–brain axis (Cowan et al., Reference Cowan, Dinan and Cryan2020). Additional studies are needed to elucidate the number and duration of overlapping sensitive periods of gut microbiome and neurodevelopment.
Limitations
The strengths of this study include a longitudinal design of stool sampling at multiple timepoints across infancy, including two timepoints earlier than any previous study. However, the results should be considered in the light of several limitations.
First, a relatively small sample size was used in this study. While such sample sizes are common in human microbiome research, they limit statistical power to detect associations with small, but meaningful, effect sizes. Future studies should be conducted with larger samples and consideration of more potential covariates. Second, we assessed infant temperament by maternal report. While parents are able to best observe infants across a wide variety of environments (Gartstein & Rothbart, Reference Gartstein and Rothbart2003), parental reports do not always correlate strongly with observer reports in standardized conditions (Mangelsdorf, Schoppe, & Buur, Reference Love, Huber and Anders2000). Third, microbiome analysis was performed using 16S ribosomal RNA gene sequencing, which provides information on microbial composition but not function (e.g., bacterial gene content or metabolite levels). Fourth, we conducted a large number of comparisons and, importantly, acknowledge the possibility that this may have led to spurious findings. However, some concern over the number of comparisons is ameliorated by the fact that many of the associations we detected are consistent with previous studies. Fifth, with the small sample size we were not able to assess other variables that may influence infant gut microbiome composition and we had to be discerning in the selection of covariates in order to preserve statistical power. Sixth, we were unable to prove the existence of sensitive periods because we could not compare influences at various timepoints on phenotypes later in life than 12 months of age. Additional longitudinal studies are needed to identify specific critical windows for contributions of the gut microbiome to inter-individual differences in behavioral and mental health outcomes.
Conclusions
Our study shows that composition of the gut microbiome at 1–3 weeks, 2 months, and 12 months of age is associated with infant temperament at age 12 months. This suggests that early infancy may be a sensitive period for gut microbiome and brain crosstalk. Our results may inform early life interventions, such as probiotics that target the gut microbiome to promote optimal infant development. While this study supports the hypothesis that the gut microbiome in early life has far-reaching consequences for neurodevelopment, further studies are needed to understand the mechanisms involved in this process.
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579421000456
Acknowledgments
The authors greatly thank the study participants and their families. The authors thank the staff, students, and volunteers of the UCI–Chapman Early Human Lifespan and Development Program, Christina Canino Brown, Mariann Howland, Sheena Ram, Amanda Appel, Jessie Van Fleet, the UCLA Biological Anthropology of Motherhood Lab, Micaela Maciel, Itzel Garcia, Elizabeth Flores, Valeria Calvillo, Sarah Meskal, Olivia Schulist, Kristine Chua, Kotrina Kajokaite, DNA Genotek, Evgueni Doukhanine, Lisa Gamwell, Ian Barrett, and Laura Cunningham. J. P. J. and L. M. G. contributed equally to the manuscript.
Funding Statement
This work was funded by the National Institutes of Health (K01DK105110 and R03DK125524 to M. F., F32MD015201 to K. S. W., P50MH096889 to C. A. S. and L. M. G., and T32DA024635 to S. M. L). This work was also funded by the U.S. Department of Veterans Affairs (IK2CX001717 to J. P. J.) and UCLA California Center for Population Research (seed grant to M. F., supported by NIH 2P2CHD04102216).
Conflicts of Interest
None.