Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-ndmmz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-16T06:46:01.367Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Mutual synergies between reactive and active inhibitory systems of temperament in the development of children's disruptive behavior: Two longitudinal studies

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 December 2020

Danming An*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA, USA
Grazyna Kochanska
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA, USA
*
Author for Correspondence: Danming An, Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Iowa, 340 Iowa Ave, Iowa City, Iowa 52242-1407; E-mail: danming-an@uiowa.edu.

Abstract

Individual differences in two inhibitory temperament systems have been implicated as key in the development of early disruptive behaviors. The reactive inhibition system, behavioral inhibition (BI) entails fearfulness, shyness, timidity, and caution. The active inhibition system, or effortful control (EC) entails a capacity to deliberately suppress, modify, or regulate a predominant behavior. Lower scores in each system have been associated with more disruptive behaviors. We examined how the two systems interact, and whether one can alleviate or exacerbate risks due to the other. In two community samples (Study 1, N = 112, ages 2.5 to 4, and Study 2, N = 102, ages 2 to 6.5), we assessed early BI and EC, and future disruptive behaviors (observed disregard for rules in Study 1 and parent-rated externalizing problems in Study 2). Robustly replicated interactions revealed that for children with low BI (relatively fearless), better EC was associated with less disruptive behavior; for children with low EC, more BI was associated with less disruptive behavior. This research extends the investigation of Temperament × Temperament interactions in developmental psychology and psychopathology, and it suggests that reactive and active inhibition systems may play mutually compensatory roles. Those effects emerged after age 2.

Type
Regular Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Biederman, J., Hirshfeld-Becker, D. R., Rosenbaum, J. F., Hérot, C., Friedman, D., Snidman, N., … Faraone, S. V. (2001). Further evidence of association between behavioral inhibition and social anxiety in children. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 16731679. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.158.10.1673CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Block, J. H., & Block, J. (1969). The California child Q-set. Berkeley: Institute of Human Development, University of California.Google Scholar
Block, J. H., & Block, J. (1980). The role of ego control and ego resiliency in the organization of behavior. In Collins, W. A. (Ed.), Development of cognition, affect, and social relations: The Minnesota symposia on child psychology (Vol. 13, pp. 49112). New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Boyce, W. T., Essex, M. J., Woodward, H. R., Measelle, J. R., Ablow, J. C., Kupfer, D. J., & MacArthur Assessment Battery Working Group. (2002). The confluence of mental, physical, social, and academic difficulties in middle childhood. I: Exploring the “headwaters” of early life morbidities. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 580587. doi:10.1097/00004583-200205000-00016CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brock, L. L., Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., Nathanson, L., & Grimm, K. J. (2009). The contributions of “hot” and “cool” executive function to children's academic achievement, learning-related behaviors, and engagement in kindergarten. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 24, 337349. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2009.06.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buss, K. A., Kiel, E. J., Morales, S., & Robinson, E. (2014). Toddler inhibitory control, bold response to novelty, and positive affect predict externalizing symptoms in kindergarten. Social Development, 23, 232249. doi:10.1111/sode.12058CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buzzell, G. A., Troller-Renfree, S. V., Morales, S., & Fox, N. A. (2018). Relations between behavioral inhibition, cognitive control, and anxiety: Novel insights provided by parsing subdomains of cognitive control. In Pérez-Edgar, K. & Fox, N. A. (Eds.), Behavioral inhibition: Integrating theory, research, and clinical perspectives (pp. 213235). Cham, Switzerland: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-98077-5_10CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Calkins, S. D., Blandon, A. Y., Williford, A. P., & Keane, S. P. (2007). Biological, behavioral, and relational levels of resilience in the context of risk for early childhood behavior problems. Development and Psychopathology, 19, 675700. doi:10.1017/S095457940700034XPCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, X., & Schmidt, L. (2015). Temperament and personality. In Lerner, R. & Lamb, M. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science. Socioemotional processes (7th ed., Vol. 3, pp. 152200). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Colder, C. R., & Stice, E. (1998). A longitudinal study of the interactive effects of impulsivity and anger on adolescent problem behavior. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 27, 255274. doi:10.1023/A:1022889202419CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Degnan, K. A., Almas, A. N., & Fox, N. A. (2010). Temperament and the environment in the etiology of childhood anxiety. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 497517. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02228.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Degnan, K. A., & Fox, N. A. (2007). Behavioral inhibition and anxiety disorders: Multiple levels of a resilience process. Development and Psychopathology, 19, 729746. doi:10.1017/s0954579407000363CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Degnan, K. A., Hane, A. A., Henderson, H. A., Moas, O. L., Reeb-Sutherland, B. C., & Fox, N. A. (2011). Longitudinal stability of temperamental exuberance and social–emotional outcomes in early childhood. Developmental Psychology, 47, 765780. doi:10.1037/a0021316CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dollar, J. M., Stifter, C. A., & Buss, K. A. (2017). Exuberant and inhibited children: Person-centered profiles and links to social adjustment. Developmental Psychology, 53, 12221229. doi:10.1037/dev0000323CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duckworth, A. L., & Kern, M. L. (2011). A Meta-analysis of the convergent validity of self-control. Journal of Research in Personality, 45, 259268. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2011.02.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (1992). Emotion, regulation, and the development of social competence. In Clark, M. S. (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology: Vol. 14. Emotion and social behavior (pp. 119150). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Guthrie, I. K., Fabes, R. A., Shepard, S., Losoya, S., Murphy, B., … Reiser, M. (2000). Prediction of elementary school children's externalizing problem behaviors from attentional and behavioral regulation and negative emotionality. Child Development, 71, 13671382. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00233CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, N., Smith, C. L., Sadovsky, A., & Spinrad, T. L. (2004). Effortful control: Relations with emotion regulation, adjustment, and socialization in childhood. In Baumeister, R. F. & Vohs, K. D. (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (pp. 259282). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Valiente, C., Spinrad, T. L., Cumberland, A., Liew, J., & Reiser, M. (2009). Longitudinal relations of children's effortful control, impulsivity, and negative emotionality to their externalizing, internalizing, and co-occurring behavior problems. Developmental Psychology, 45, 9881008. doi:10.1037/a0016213CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Essex, M. J., Boyce, W. T., Goldstein, L. H., Armstrong, J. M., Kraemer, H. C., Kupfer, D. J., & MacArthur Assessment Battery Working Group. (2002). The confluence of mental, physical, social, and academic difficulties in middle childhood. II: Developing the MacArthur Health and Behavior Questionnaire. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 588603. doi:10.1097/00004583-200205000-00017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fowles, D. C., & Dindo, L. (2009). Temperament and psychopathy: A dual-pathway model. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 179183. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01632.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, N. A., Henderson, H. A., Marshall, P. J., Nichols, K. E., & Ghera, M. M. (2005). Behavioral inhibition: Linking biology and behavior within a developmental framework. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 235262. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141532CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, N. A., Henderson, H. A., Rubin, K. H., Calkins, S. D., & Schmidt, L. A. (2001). Continuity and discontinuity of behavioral inhibition and exuberance: Psychophysiological and behavioral influences across the first four years of life. Child Development, 72, 121. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00262CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frick, P. J., & Morris, A. S. (2004). Temperament and developmental pathways to conduct problems. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33, 5468. doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp3301_6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gadow, K. D., & Sprafkin, J. N. (2002). Child Symptom Inventory 4: Screening and norms manual. Stony Brook, NY: Checkmate Plus.Google Scholar
Gao, Y., Raine, A., Venables, P. H., Dawson, M. E., & Mednick, S. A. (2010). Association of poor childhood fear conditioning and adult crime. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 5660. DOI: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09040499CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hassan, R., Poole, K. L., & Schmidt, L. A. (2020). Revisiting the double-edged sword of self-regulation: Linking shyness, attentional shifting, and social behavior in preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 196, 104842. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2020.104842CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Honomichl, R. D., & Donnellan, M. B. (2012). Dimensions of temperament in preschoolers predict risk taking and externalizing behaviors in adolescents. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3, 1422. doi:10.1177/1948550611407344CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, P. O., & Fay, L. C. (1950). The Johnson–Neyman technique, its theory and application. Psychometrika, 15, 349367. doi:10.1007/BF02288864CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kagan, J., & Fox, N. (2006). Biology, culture, and temperamental biases. In Damon, W., Lerner, R. & Eisenberg, N. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 99166). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., Clarke, C., Snidman, N., & Garcia Coll, C. (1984). Behavioral inhibition to the unfamiliar. Child Development, 55, 22122225. doi:10.2307/1129793.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., & Gibbons, J. (1989). Inhibited and uninhibited types of children. Child Development, 60, 838845. doi:10.2307/1131025.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kagan, J., & Snidman, N. (2004). The long shadow of temperament. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press/Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Kim, S., & Kochanska, G. (2020). Family sociodemographic resources moderate the path from toddlers’ hard-to-manage temperament to parental control to disruptive behavior in middle childhood. Development and Psychopathology. Advance publication online, doi:10.1017/s0954579419001664Google Scholar
Kim, S., Nordling, J. K., Yoon, J. E., Boldt, L. J., & Kochanska, G. (2013). Effortful control in “hot” and “cool” tasks differentially predicts children's behavior problems and academic performance. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41, 4356. doi:10.1007/s10802-012-9661-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, D. N., Dyson, M. W., Kujawa, A. J., & Kotov, R. (2012). Temperament and internalizing disorders. In Zentner, M. & Shiner, R. (Eds.), Handbook of temperament (pp. 541561). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Klein, D. N., & Mumper, E. E. (2018). Behavioral Inhibition as a Precursor to Psychopathology. In Perez-Edgar, K. & Fox, N. (Eds.), Behavioral inhibition: Integrating theory, research, and clinical perspectives (pp. 283307). Cham, Switzerland: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-98077-5_13CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kochanska, G. (1993). Toward a synthesis of parental socialization and child temperament in early development of conscience. Child Development, 64, 325347. doi:10.2307/1131254CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kochanska, G., Aksan, N., & Joy, M. E. (2007). Children's fearfulness as a moderator of parenting in early socialization: Two longitudinal studies. Developmental Psychology, 43, 222237. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.1.222CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kochanska, G., Aksan, N., Penney, S. J., & Doobay, A. F. (2007). Early positive emotionality as a heterogeneous trait: Implications for children's self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 10541066. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.93.6.1054CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kochanska, G., Barry, R. A., Jimenez, N. B., Hollatz, A. L., & Woodard, J. (2009). Guilt and effortful control: Two mechanisms that prevent disruptive developmental trajectories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 322333. doi:10.1037/a0015471CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kochanska, G., Brock, R. L., & Boldt, L. J. (2017). A cascade from disregard for rules of conduct at preschool age to parental power assertion at early school age to antisocial behavior in early preadolescence: Interplay with the child's skin conductance level. Development and Psychopathology, 29, 875885. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0954579416000547CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kochanska, G., Coy, K. C., & Murray, K. T. (2001). The development of self-regulation in the first four years of life. Child Development, 72, 10911111. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00336CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kochanska, G., & Knaack, A. (2003). Effortful control as a personality characteristic of young children: Antecedents, correlates, and consequences. Journal of Personality, 71, 10871112. doi:10.1111/1467-6494.7106008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kochanska, G., Koenig, J. L., Barry, R. A., Kim, S., & Yoon, J. E. (2010). Children's conscience during toddler and preschool years, moral self, and a competent, adaptive developmental trajectory. Developmental Psychology, 46, 13201332. DOI:10.1037/a0020381CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kochanska, G., Murray, K. T., & Harlan, E. T. (2000). Effortful control in early childhood: Continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development. Developmental Psychology, 36, 220232. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.36.2.220CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, P., & Pérez-Edgar, K. E. (2019). Developmental pathways from early behavioral inhibition to later anxiety: An integrative review of developmental psychopathology research and translational implications. Adolescent Research Review, 4, 4558. doi:10.1007/s40894-018-0092-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lonigan, C. J., & Vasey, M. W. (2009). Negative affectivity, effortful control, and attention to threat-relevant stimuli. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 387399. doi:10.1007/s10802-008-9284-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lykken, D. T. (1995). The antisocial personalities. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Moffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Belsky, D., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J., Harrington, H., … Caspi, A. (2011). A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 26932698. doi:10.1073/pnas.1010076108CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morales, S., Miller, N. V., Troller-Renfree, S. V., White, L. K., Degnan, K. A., Henderson, H. A., & Fox, N. A. (2019). Attention bias to reward predicts behavioral problems and moderates early risk to externalizing and attention problems. Development and Psychopathology, 32, 397409. doi:10.1017/s0954579419000166CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morales, S., Pérez-Edgar, K., & Buss, K. (2016). Longitudinal relations among exuberance, externalizing behaviors, and attentional bias to reward: The mediating role of effortful control. Developmental Science, 19, 853862. doi:10.1111/desc.12320CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muris, P., & Ollendick, T. H. (2005). The role of temperament in the etiology of child psychopathology. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 8, 271289. doi:10.1007/s10567-005-8809-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998–2020). Mplus user's guide. Los Angeles, CA: Author.Google Scholar
Nielsen, J. D., Olino, T. M., Dyson, M. W., & Klein, D. N. (2019). Reactive and regulatory temperament: Longitudinal associations with internalizing and externalizing symptoms through childhood. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 47, 17711784. doi:10.1007/s10802-019-00555-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nigg, J. T. (2000). On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: Views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 220246. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.220CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nigg, J. T. (2006). Temperament and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 395422. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01612.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nigg, J. T. (2017). Annual Research Review: On the relations among self-regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58, 361383. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12675CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nystrom, B., & Bengtsson, H. (2016). Temperamental influences on children's risk-taking in decision making: A dual process, multi-level analysis. Personality and Individual Differences, 89, 177181. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2015.10.020CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oldehinkel, A. J., Hartman, C. A., Ferdinand, R. F., Verhulst, F. C., & Ormel, J. (2007). Effortful control as modifier of the association between negative emotionality and adolescents’ mental health problems. Development and Psychopathology, 19, 523539. doi:10.1017/s0954579407070253CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olson, S. L., Sameroff, A. J., Kerr, D. C., & Lopez, N. L. (2005). Developmental foundations of externalizing problems in young children: The role of effortful control. Development and Psychopathology, 17, 2545. DOI: 10.1017/S0954579405050029CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patrick, C. J., Kramer, M. D., Vaidyanathan, U., Benning, S. D., Hicks, B. M., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2019). Formulation of a measurement model for the boldness construct of psychopathy. Psychological Assessment, 31, 643659. doi:10.1037/pas0000690CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pérez-Edgar, K., Taber-Thomas, B., Auday, E., & Morales, S. (2014). Temperament and attention as core mechanisms in the early emergence of anxiety. In Lagattuta, K. (Ed.), Children and emotion: New insights into developmental affective science (Vol. 26, pp. 4256). Basel, Switzerland: Karger Publishing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2000). Developing mechanisms of self-regulation. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 427441. doi:10.1017/s0954579400003096CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putnam, S. P., & Stifter, C. A. (2005). Behavioral approach-inhibition in toddlers: Prediction from infancy, positive and negative affective components, and relations with behavior problems. Child Development, 76, 212226. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00840.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raine, A., Reynolds, C., Venables, P. H., Mednick, S. A., & Farrington, D. P. (1998). Fearlessness, stimulation-seeking, and large body size at age 3 years as early predispositions to childhood aggression at age 11 years. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 745751. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.55.8.745CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (2006). Temperament. In Damon, W., & Lerner, R. M. (Series Eds.) & Eisenberg, N. (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (pp. 99166). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Rueda, M. R. (2012). Effortful control. In Zentner, M. & Shiner, R. L. (Eds.), Handbook of temperament (pp. 145167). New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Sandstrom, A., Uher, R., & Pavlova, B. (2020). Prospective association between childhood behavioral inhibition and anxiety: A meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 48, 5766. doi:10.1007/s10802-019-00588-5Google ScholarPubMed
Shaw, D. S., Gilliom, M., Ingoldsby, E. M., & Nagin, D. S. (2003). Trajectories leading to school-age conduct problems. Developmental Psychology, 39, 189200. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.39.2.189CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sportel, B. E., Nauta, M. H., de Hullu, E., de Jong, P. J., & Hartman, C. A. (2011). Behavioral inhibition and attentional control in adolescents: Robust relationships with anxiety and depression. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 20, 149156. doi:10.31988/scitrends.42068CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stifter, C. A., Putnam, S., & Jahromi, L. (2008). Exuberant and inhibited toddlers: Stability of temperament and risk for problem behavior. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 401421. doi:10.1017/s0954579408000199CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tackett, J. L., Martel, M. M., & Kushner, S. C. (2012). Temperament, externalizing disorders, and attention deficit/ hyperactivity disorder. In Zentner, M. & Shiner, R. (Eds.), Handbook of temperament (pp. 562580). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Thomas, A., & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and development. Oxford, UK: Brunner/Mazel.Google Scholar
Thorell, L. B., Bohlin, G., & Rydell, A. M. (2004). Two types of inhibitory control: Predictive relations to social functioning. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 28, 193203. doi:10.1080/01650250344000389CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Troller-Renfree, S. V., Buzzell, G. A., Bowers, M. E., Salo, V. C., Forman-Alberti, A., Smith, E., … Fox, N. A. (2019). Development of inhibitory control during childhood and its relations to early temperament and later social anxiety: Unique insights provided by latent growth modeling and signal detection theory. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 60, 622629. doi:10.1111/jcpp.13025CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Troller-Renfree, S. V., Buzzell, G. A., Pine, D. S., Henderson, H. A., & Fox, N. A. (2019). Consequences of not planning ahead: Reduced proactive control moderates longitudinal relations between behavioral inhibition and anxiety. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 58, 768775. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2018.06.040CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, L. K., McDermott, J. M., Degnan, K. A., Henderson, H. A., & Fox, N. A. (2011). Behavioral inhibition and anxiety: The moderating roles of inhibitory control and attention shifting. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39, 735747. doi:10.1007/s10802-012-9688-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willoughby, M., Kupersmidt, J., Voegler-Lee, M., & Bryant, D. (2011). Contributions of hot and cool self-regulation to preschool disruptive behavior and academic achievement. Developmental Neuropsychology, 36, 162180. doi:10.1080/87565641.2010.549980CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xu, Y., Farver, J. A. M., & Zhang, Z. (2009). Temperament, harsh and indulgent parenting, and Chinese children's proactive and reactive aggression. Child Development, 80, 244258. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01257.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zelazo, P. D., & Carlson, S. M. (2012). Hot and cool executive function in childhood and adolescence: Development and plasticity. Child Development Perspectives, 6, 354360. doi:10.1111/j.1750-8606.2012.00246.xGoogle Scholar