Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-ndmmz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-09T06:58:49.790Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effects of age on the relationship between sleep quality and cognitive performance: Findings from the Human Connectome Project-Aging cohort

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 December 2023

Daniel E. Cohen
Affiliation:
Division of Geriatric Psychiatry, New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York, NY, USA
Hyun Kim*
Affiliation:
Division of Geriatric Psychiatry, New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York, NY, USA Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University Irving Medical Center, New York, NY, USA
Alina Levine
Affiliation:
Division of Mental Health Data Science, New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York, NY, USA
Davangere P. Devanand
Affiliation:
Division of Geriatric Psychiatry, New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York, NY, USA Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University Irving Medical Center, New York, NY, USA Department of Neurology, Columbia University Irving Medical Center, New York, NY, USA
Seonjoo Lee
Affiliation:
Division of Mental Health Data Science, New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York, NY, USA Department of Biostatistics, Mailman School of Public Health, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
Terry E. Goldberg
Affiliation:
Division of Geriatric Psychiatry, New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York, NY, USA Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University Irving Medical Center, New York, NY, USA Department of Anesthesiology, Columbia University Irving Medical Center, New York, NY, USA
*
Correspondence should be addressed to: Hyun Kim, Division of Geriatric Psychiatry, New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York, NY, USA. Email: monhykim@bu.edu.

Abstract

Background:

The association between sleep quality and cognition is widely established, but the role of aging in this relationship is largely unknown.

Objective:

To examine how age impacts the sleep–cognition relationship and determine whether there are sensitive ranges when the relationship between sleep and cognition is modified. This investigation could help identify individuals at risk for sleep-related cognitive impairment.

Subjects:

Sample included 711 individuals (ages 36.00–89.83, 59.66 ± 14.91, 55.7 % female) from the Human Connectome Project-Aging (HCP-A).

Methods:

The association between sleep quality (Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index, PSQI) and cognition (Crystallized Cognition Composite and Fluid Cognition Composite from the NIH Toolbox, the Trail Making Test, TMT, and the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, RAVLT) was measured using linear regression models, with sex, race, use of sleep medication, hypertension, and years of education as covariates. The interaction between sleep and age on cognition was tested using the moderation analysis, with age as both continuous linear and nonlinear (quadratic) terms.

Results:

There was a significant interaction term between the PSQI and nonlinear age term (age2) on TMT-B (p = 0.02) and NIH Toolbox crystallized cognition (p = 0.02), indicating that poor sleep quality was associated with worse performance on these measures (sensitive age ranges 50–75 years for TMT-B and 66–70 years for crystallized cognition).

Conclusions:

The sleep–cognition relationship may be modified by age. Individuals in the middle age to early older adulthood age band may be most vulnerable to sleep-related cognitive impairment.

Type
Original Research Article
Copyright
© International Psychogeriatric Association 2023

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

Data used in preparation of this article were obtained for the Human Connectome Project Aging (HCP-A) database (https://www.humanconnectome.org/). As such, the investigators within the HCP-A contributed to the design and implementation of HCP-A and/or provided data but did not participate in the writing of this report. A complete listing of HCP-A investigators can be found at: https://www.humanconnectome.org/study/hcp-lifespan-aging

Daniel E. Cohen and Hyun Kim have equal contribution to authorship.

This article was originally published with errors in the abstract which led to information being misconstrued. The errors have been rectified and the article updated. A correction notice has been published.

References

Akshoomoff, N. et al. (2013). VIII. NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (CB): composite scores of crystallized, fluid, and overall cognition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 78, 119132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alexander, G. C., Emerson, S. and Kesselheim, A. S. (2021). Evaluation of Aducanumab for Alzheimer disease: scientific evidence and regulatory review involving efficacy, safety, and futility. JAMA, 325, 17171718.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Altman, D. G. and Royston, P. (2006). The cost of dichotomising continuous variables. BMJ, 332, 1080.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Army Individual Test Battery, I. (1944). Manual of Directions and Scoring. Washington, DC: War Department, Adjunct General’s Office.Google Scholar
Barnes, D. E. and Yaffe, K. (2011). The projected effect of risk factor reduction on Alzheimer’s disease prevalence. The Lancet Neurology, 10, 819828.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartolacci, C., Scarpelli, S., D'Atri, A., Gorgoni, M., Annarumma, L., Cloos, C., Giannini, A. M. and De Gennaro, L. (2020). The influence of sleep quality, vigilance, and sleepiness on driving-related cognitive abilities: a comparison between young and older adults. Brain Sciences, 10(6), 327.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bhatti, G. K., Reddy, A. P., Reddy, P. H. and Bhatti, J. S. (2019). Lifestyle modifications and nutritional interventions in aging-associated cognitive decline and Alzheimer’s disease. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 11, 369.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bliese, P. D., Wesensten, N. J. and Balkin, T. J. (2006). Age and individual variability in performance during sleep restriction. Journal of Sleep Research, 15, 376385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonnet, M. H. (2005). Acute Sleep Deprivation. In: Kryger, M. H. (Eds.), Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine (pp 5166). Philadelphia, PA: W.B. Saunders Company.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bookheimer, S. Y. et al. (2019). The lifespan human connectome project in aging: an overview. Neuroimage, 185, 335348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buckner, R. L. (2004). Memory and executive function in aging and AD: multiple factors that cause decline and reserve factors that compensate. Neuron, 44, 195208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buysse, D. J., Reynolds, C. F., Monk, T. H., Berman, S. R. and Kupfer, D. J. (1989). The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Research, 28, 193213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Couyoumdjian, A. et al. (2010). The effects of sleep and sleep deprivation on task-switching performance. Journal of Sleep Research, 19, 6470.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craik, F. I. and Bialystok, E. (2006). Planning and task management in older adults: cooking breakfast. Memory & Cognition, 34, 12361249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Jager, C. A., Budge, M. M. and Clarke, R. (2003). Utility of TICS-M for the assessment of cognitive function in older adults. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 18, 318324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Sousa Magalhães, S., Mallow-Diniz, L. F. and Hamdan, A. C. (2012). Validity convergent and reliability test-tetest of the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test. Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 9, 129137.Google Scholar
Doody, R. S. et al. (2014). Phase 3 trials of solanezumab for mild-to-moderate Alzheimer’s disease. New England Journal of Medicine, 370, 311321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dzierzewski, J. M., Dautovich, N. and Ravyts, S. (2018). Sleep and cognition in older adults. Sleep Medicine Clinics, 13, 93106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harrison, Y. and Horne, J. A. (1998). Sleep loss impairs short and novel language tasks having a prefrontal focus. Journal of Sleep Research, 7, 95100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harrison, Y. and Horne, J. A. (1999). One night of sleep loss impairs innovative thinking and flexible decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 78, 128145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hedden, T. and Gabrieli, J. D. (2004). Insights into the ageing mind: a view from cognitive neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 8796.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hirshkowitz, M. et al. (2015). National sleep foundation’s sleep time duration recommendations: methodology and results summary. Sleep Health, 1, 4043.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holanda, F. W. N. J. and de Almondes, K. M. (2016). Sleep and executive functions in older adults: a systematic review. Dementia & Neuropsychologia, 10, 185197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hukkelhoven, C. W. et al. (2003). Patient age and outcome following severe traumatic brain injury: an analysis of 5600 patients. Journal of Neurosurgery, 99, 666673.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ivnick, R. J., Malec, J. F., Tangalos, E. G., Petersen, R. C., Kokmen, E. and Kurkland, L. T. (1990). The Auditory-Verbal Learning Test (AVLT): norms for ages 55 years and older. Psychological Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and Clnical Psychology, 2, 304312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, J. N. (2006). Age-related neuropathology, cognitive decline, and Alzheimer’s disease. Ageing Research Reviews, 5, 113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Killgore, W. D. (2010). Effects of sleep deprivation on cognition. Progress in Brain Research, 185, 105129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, H., Suh, S., Cho, E. R., Yang, H. C., Yun, C. H., Thomas, R. J., Lee, S. K. and Shin, C. (2013). Longitudinal course of insomnia: age-related differences in subjective sleepiness and vigilance performance in a population-based sample. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 75(6), 532538.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knutson, K. L. et al. (2009). Association between sleep and blood pressure in midlife: the CARDIA sleep study. Archives of Internal Medicine, 169, 10551061.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kurdziel, L. B. F., Mantua, J. and Spencer, R. M. C. (2017). Novel word learning in older adults: a role for sleep? Brain and Language, 167, 106113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, S., Vigoureux, T. F., Hyer, K. and Small, B. J. (2022). Prevalent insomnia concerns and perceived need for sleep intervention among direct-care workers in long-term care. Journal of Applied Gerontology, 41, 274284.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, S. C., Huxhold, O. and Schmiedek, F. (2004). Aging and attenuated processing robustness. Evidence from cognitive and sensorimotor functioning. Gerontology, 50, 2834.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, M. A., Wright, H., Ji, C. and Cappuccio, F. P. (2014). Cross-sectional study of sleep quantity and quality and amnestic and non-amnestic cognitive function in an ageing population: the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA). PLoS One, 9, e100991.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morin, C. M., Colecchi, C., Stone, J., Sood, R. and Brink, D. (1999). Behavioral and pharmacological therapies for late-life insomnia: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA, 281, 991999.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muzur, A., Pace-Schott, E. F. and Hobson, J. A. (2002). The prefrontal cortex in sleep. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 475481.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nadel, L., Hupbach, A., Gomez, R. and Newman-Smith, K. (2012). Memory formation, consolidation and transformation. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36, 16401645.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nasreddine, Z. S. et al. (2005). The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 53, 695699.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishtala, A. et al. (2014). Midlife cardiovascular risk impacts executive function: Framingham offspring study. Alzheimer Disease & Associated Disorders, 28, 1622.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olivera-Souza, R. D. et al. (2000). Trail making and cognitive set-shifting. Arquivos de Neuro-Psiquiatria, 58, 826829.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ott, L. R. et al. (2022). Construct validity of the NIH toolbox cognitive domains: a comparison with conventional neuropsychological assessments. Neuropsychology, 36, 468481.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pace, V., Treloar, A. and Scott, S. (2011). Dementia: From Advanced Disease to Bereavement. Oxford Specialist Handbooks.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pilz, L. K., Keller, L. K., Lenssen, D. and Roenneberg, T. (2018). Time to rethink sleep quality: PSQI scores reflect sleep quality on workdays. Sleep, 41(5).Google Scholar
Plihal, W. and Born, J. (1997). Effects of early and late nocturnal sleep on declarative and procedural memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 534547.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rana, B. K. et al. (2018). Association of sleep quality on memory-related executive functions in middle age. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 24, 6776.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reitan, R. M. and Wolfson, D. (1985). The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery: Theory and Clinical Interpretation. Tucson, Ariz: Neuropsychology Press.Google Scholar
Rey, A. (1964). L' examen clinique en psychologie (The Clinical Psychological Examination). Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.Google Scholar
Sanchez-Cubillo, I. et al. (2009). Construct validity of the Trail Making Test: role of task-switching, working memory, inhibition/interference control, and visuomotor abilities. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15, 438450.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schaedler, T., Santos, J. S., Vincenzi, R. A., Pereira, S. I. R. and Louzada, F. M. (2018). Executive functioning is preserved in healthy young adults under acute sleep restriction. Sleep Science, 11, 152159.Google ScholarPubMed
Schoenberg, M. R., Dawson, K. A., Duff, K., Patton, D., Scott, J. G. and Adams, R. L. (2006). Test performance and classification statistics for the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test in selected clinical samples. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 21, 693703.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schroeck, J. L. et al. (2016). Review of safety and efficacy of sleep medicines in older adults. Clinical Therapeutics, 38, 23402372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, E. P., Sorrell, A. and Benitez, A. (2019). Psychometric properties of the NIH toolbox cognition battery in healthy older adults: reliability, validity, and agreement with standard neuropsychological tests. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 25, 857867.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scullin, M. K. and Bliwise, D. L. (2015). Sleep, cognition, and normal aging: integrating a half century of multidisciplinary research. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10, 97137.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shatenstein, B., Barberger-Gateau, P. and Mecocci, P. (2015). Prevention of age-related cognitive decline: which strategies, when, and for whom? Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 48, 3553.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spencer, R. M., Gouw, A. M. and Ivry, R. B. (2007). Age-related decline of sleep-dependent consolidation. Learning & Memory, 14, 480484.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stenuit, P. and Kerkhofs, M. (2005). Age modulates the effects of sleep restriction in women. Sleep, 28, 12831288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stickgold, R. (2005). Sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Nature, 437, 12721278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M. S. and Spreen, O. (2006). A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests : Administration, Norms, and Commentary. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Taylor, D. J. and Pruiksma, K. E. (2014). Cognitive and behavioural therapy for insomnia (CBT-I) in psychiatric populations: a systematic review. International Review of Psychiatry, 26, 205213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tombaugh, T. N. (2004). Trail Making Test A and B: normative data stratified by age and education. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19, 203214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waters, F. and Bucks, R. S. (2011). Neuropsychological effects of sleep loss: implication for neuropsychologists. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17, 571586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Webb, W. B. (1985). A further analysis of age and sleep deprivation effects. Psychophysiology, 22, 156161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weintraub, S. et al. (2013). Cognition assessment using the NIH Toolbox. Neurology, 80, S5464.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilckens, K. A., Woo, S. G., Kirk, A. R., Erickson, K. I. and Wheeler, M. E. (2014). Role of sleep continuity and total sleep time in executive function across the adult lifespan. Psychology and Aging, 29, 658665.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yagi, A., Nouchi, R., Murayama, K., Sakaki, M. and Kawashima, R. (2020). The role of cognitive control in age-related changes in well-being. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 12, 198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhao, Q., Lv, Y., Zhou, Y., Hong, Z. and Guo, Q. (2012). Short-term delayed recall of auditory verbal learning test is equivalent to long-term delayed recall for identifying amnestic mild cognitive impairment. PLoS One, 7, e51157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Supplementary material: File

Cohen et al. supplementary material

Cohen et al. supplementary material
Download Cohen et al. supplementary material(File)
File 29.5 KB