Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-wg55d Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-31T04:27:52.883Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A population-wide analysis of the familial risk of suicide in Utah, USA

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 August 2021

Amanda V. Bakian*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Huntsman Mental Health Institute, University of Utah School of Medicine, Salt Lake City, UT, USA Department of Family and Preventive Medicine, University of Utah School of Medicine, Salt Lake City, UT, USA
Danli Chen
Affiliation:
Study Design & Biostatics Center, Utah Clinical & Translational Science Institute, Salt Lake City, Utah, USA
Chong Zhang
Affiliation:
Study Design & Biostatics Center, Utah Clinical & Translational Science Institute, Salt Lake City, Utah, USA
Heidi A. Hanson
Affiliation:
Department of Surgery, University of Utah School of Medicine, Salt Lake City, UT, USA Population Sciences, Huntsman Cancer Institute, Salt Lake City, UT, USA Scientific Computing Institute, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, USA
Anna R. Docherty
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Huntsman Mental Health Institute, University of Utah School of Medicine, Salt Lake City, UT, USA
Brooks Keeshin
Affiliation:
Department of Pediatrics, University of Utah School of Medicine, Salt Lake City, UT, USA
Douglas Gray
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Huntsman Mental Health Institute, University of Utah School of Medicine, Salt Lake City, UT, USA
Ken R. Smith
Affiliation:
Population Sciences, Huntsman Cancer Institute, Salt Lake City, UT, USA Scientific Computing Institute, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, USA Department of Family and Consumer Studies, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, USA
James A. VanDerslice
Affiliation:
Department of Family and Preventive Medicine, University of Utah School of Medicine, Salt Lake City, UT, USA
David Z. Yu
Affiliation:
Population Sciences, Huntsman Cancer Institute, Salt Lake City, UT, USA
Yue Zhang
Affiliation:
Department of Internal Medicine, University of Utah School of Medicine, Salt Lake City, UT, USA
Hilary Coon
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Huntsman Mental Health Institute, University of Utah School of Medicine, Salt Lake City, UT, USA
*
Author for correspondence: Amanda V. Bakian, E-mail: amanda.bakian@hsc.utah.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

The degree to which suicide risk aggregates in US families is unknown. The authors aimed to determine the familial risk of suicide in Utah, and tested whether familial risk varies based on the characteristics of the suicides and their relatives.

Methods

A population-based sample of 12 160 suicides from 1904 to 2014 were identified from the Utah Population Database and matched 1:5 to controls based on sex and age using at-risk sampling. All first through third- and fifth-degree relatives of suicide probands and controls were identified (N = 13 480 122). The familial risk of suicide was estimated based on hazard ratios (HR) from an unsupervised Cox regression model in a unified framework. Moderation by sex of the proband or relative and age of the proband at time of suicide (<25 v. ⩾25 years) was examined.

Results

Significantly elevated HRs were observed in first- (HR 3.45; 95% CI 3.12–3.82) through fifth-degree relatives (HR 1.07; 95% CI 1.02–1.12) of suicide probands. Among first-degree relatives of female suicide probands, the HR of suicide was 6.99 (95% CI 3.99–12.25) in mothers, 6.39 in sisters (95% CI 3.78–10.82), and 5.65 (95% CI 3.38–9.44) in daughters. The HR in first-degree relatives of suicide probands under 25 years at death was 4.29 (95% CI 3.49–5.26).

Conclusions

Elevated familial suicide risk in relatives of female and younger suicide probands suggests that there are unique risk groups to which prevention efforts should be directed – namely suicidal young adults and women with a strong family history of suicide.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Suicide is the 10th leading cause of death in the USA (Hedegaard, Curtin, & Warner, Reference Hedegaard, Curtin and Warner2018). While the risk of other top causes of mortality has declined recently in the USA, suicide rates increased by 30% between 1999 and 2016 (Stone et al., Reference Stone, Simon, Fowler, Kegler, Yuan, Holland and Crosby2018). Suicide's etiology is complex with predisposing, mediating, and short-term risk factors from genetic and environmental sources implicated in its causal pathway (Turecki, Reference Turecki2014). Low predictive performance of existing prediction tools (Belsher, Smolenski, & Pruitt, Reference Belsher, Smolenski and Pruitt2019) and a lack of effective evidence-based interventions for suicide mortality (Nelson et al., Reference Nelson, Denneson, Low, Bauer, O'Neil, Kansagara and Teo2017; Riblet, Shiner, Young-Xu, & Watts, Reference Riblet, Shiner, Young-Xu and Watts2017) indicate the ongoing need for improved understanding of suicide's risk factors.

The aggregation of suicide mortality (herein referred to as ‘suicide’) in families is a strong risk factor for suicide with evidence originating largely from twin (Juel-Nielsen & Videbech, Reference Juel-Nielsen and Videbech1970; Pedersen & Fiske, Reference Pedersen and Fiske2010; Roy, Segal, Centerwall, & Robinette, Reference Roy, Segal, Centerwall and Robinette1991), adoption (Kendler, Ohlsson, Sundguist, Sundguist, & Edwards, Reference Kendler, Ohlsson, Sundguist, Sundguist and Edwards2020; Petersen, Sørensen, Andersen, Mortensen, & Hawton, Reference Petersen, Sørensen, Andersen, Mortensen and Hawton2013; Schulsinger, Kety, Rosenthal, & Wender, Reference Schulsinger, Kety, Rosenthal, Wender, Schou and Stromgren1979; von Borczyskowski, Lindblad, Vinnerljung, Reintjes, & Hjern, Reference von Borczyskowski, Lindblad, Vinnerljung, Reintjes and Hjern2011), and population-based family studies (Agerbo, Reference Agerbo2005; Agerbo, Nordentoft, & Mortensen, Reference Agerbo, Nordentoft and Mortensen2002; Cheng et al., Reference Cheng, Yen, Chang, Wu, Ko and Li2014; Egeland & Sussex, Reference Egeland and Sussex1985; Garssen, Deerenberg, Mackenbach, Kerkhof, & Kunst, Reference Garssen, Deerenberg, Mackenbach, Kerkhof and Kunst2011; Kim et al., Reference Kim, Sequin, Therrien, Riopel, Chawky, Lesage and Turecki2005; Qin & Mortensen, Reference Qin and Mortensen2003; Qin, Agerbo, & Mortensen, Reference Qin, Agerbo and Mortensen2002, Reference Qin, Agerbo and Mortensen2003; Runeson & Åsberg, Reference Runeson and Åsberg2003; Tidemalm, Runeson, & Waern, Reference Tidemalm, Runeson and Waern2011). While twin and adoption studies offer robust designs for the investigation of familial suicide risk among close relatives, population-based family studies often include large sample sizes, which allow for the calculation of familial suicide risks in specific kinships across a broad range of relatives. The preferred family study is prospective and uses random sampling thereby minimizing the risk of selection bias (Hopper, Bishop, & Easton, Reference Hopper, Bishop and Easton2005). To date, research into the familial risk of suicide using population-based family study designs has largely been limited to work done in Northwestern European countries using data linked across health and multi-generational registries (but see Cheng et al., Reference Cheng, Yen, Chang, Wu, Ko and Li2014; Kim et al., Reference Kim, Sequin, Therrien, Riopel, Chawky, Lesage and Turecki2005). These studies have primarily measured the familial aggregation of suicide in first-degree relatives (Agerbo, Reference Agerbo2005; Agerbo et al., Reference Agerbo, Nordentoft and Mortensen2002; Garssen et al., Reference Garssen, Deerenberg, Mackenbach, Kerkhof and Kunst2011; Qin & Mortensen, Reference Qin and Mortensen2003; Qin et al., Reference Qin, Agerbo and Mortensen2002, Reference Qin, Agerbo and Mortensen2003; Runeson & Åsberg, Reference Runeson and Åsberg2003) with a single study (Tidemalm et al., Reference Tidemalm, Runeson and Waern2011) examining familial risk in second- and third-degree relatives (e.g. cousins). In combination, population-based family studies report a two (Agerbo, Reference Agerbo2005; Agerbo et al., Reference Agerbo, Nordentoft and Mortensen2002; Garssen et al., Reference Garssen, Deerenberg, Mackenbach, Kerkhof and Kunst2011; Qin & Mortensen, Reference Qin and Mortensen2003; Qin et al., Reference Qin, Agerbo and Mortensen2002, Reference Qin, Agerbo and Mortensen2003; Runeson & Åsberg, Reference Runeson and Åsberg2003; Tidemalm et al., Reference Tidemalm, Runeson and Waern2011) to 15-fold [in monozygotic twins (Tidemalm et al., Reference Tidemalm, Runeson and Waern2011)] increase in the risk of suicide among first-degree relatives of suicide probands. Some of this work suggests that suicide's familial liability may be higher in female relatives compared to males (Cheng et al., Reference Cheng, Yen, Chang, Wu, Ko and Li2014; Qin et al., Reference Qin, Agerbo and Mortensen2003; Qin & Mortensen, Reference Qin and Mortensen2003) especially if the suicide proband is also female or young at time-of-death (Cheng et al., Reference Cheng, Yen, Chang, Wu, Ko and Li2014; Garssen et al., Reference Garssen, Deerenberg, Mackenbach, Kerkhof and Kunst2011; Qin & Mortensen, Reference Qin and Mortensen2003). Little has been reported on the familial liability of suicide across specific kinships in more distantly related relatives.

In the USA, data resources comparable to the health registries in Northwestern Europe are not widely available for examining the patterns of familial suicide transmission using a population-based family study design. The exception being the pioneering work done in the Old Order Amish in which all suicides in a 100-year period were identified (n = 26 deaths) and linked to their extended pedigrees (Egeland & Sussex, Reference Egeland and Sussex1985). This study found that 73% of suicides clustered in four multigenerational families, which encompassed only 16% of the total population. The study's focus on the isolated Old Order Amish community, however, greatly limits the generalizability of its findings.

The current urgency to reduce suicide rates and develop well-performing predictive models of suicide in US populations (Gordon, Avenevoli, & Pearson, Reference Gordon, Avenevoli and Pearson2020) necessitates the estimation of specific measures of how suicide aggregates in American families. The US state of Utah is unique in the availability of data resources that make feasible a population-based analysis of the familial aggregation of suicide. The Utah Population Database (UPDB) is a repository for multiple population-wide sources of biomedical-related information and includes information on multi-generational pedigrees (Smith & Fraser, Reference Smith and Fraser2018). Using multigenerational data from the UPDB linked to mortality information, the current study aimed to determine the familial aggregation of suicide in Utah in first- through fifth-degree relatives. To expand upon prior work, we also examined if and how the familial risk of suicide varies in first- through fifth-degree relatives based on sex and age of the suicide proband and sex of the proband's relative. Analyses were conducted using a unified modeling approach (Lee, Rebora, Valsecchi, Czene, & Reilly, Reference Lee, Rebora, Valsecchi, Czene and Reilly2013), which minimizes the total number of models needed to test kinship and interaction effects while simultaneously maximizing statistical efficiency. Finally, the attributable risk and the population attributable fraction of the familial risk of suicide were calculated to measure its contribution on individual and population levels to suicide death in Utah.

Methods

Study population and sampling design

The current study used a prospective cohort design to determine the familial aggregation of suicide in relatives of suicide probands v. relatives of non-suicide controls. The study population was identified in the UPDB, a unique, multi-source comprehensive data resource containing genealogical, demographic, and vital records on over 11 million current and previous Utah residents (Smith & Fraser, Reference Smith and Fraser2018). The suicide group included all Utah suicides from 1904 to 2014 who were 10 years or older at time of death (six suicides were excluded for age <10 at time of death) based on a suicide manner of death indication on a death certificate. For deaths from 1957 to 2014, International Classification of Diseases (ICD) cause-of-death coding was used to identify additional suicides including ICD-6 codes E970-E979, ICD-7 codes E963 and E970-E979, ICD-8 and 9 codes E950-D959, and ICD-10 codes X60-X84 and U03. For deaths from 1904 to 1956, identification of suicide was based on UPDB translation of death certificate causes-of-death text into corresponding ICD-10 codes using the 2000 Mortality Medical Data System developed by the US Department of Health and Human Services (Lu, Reference Lu2003; MMDS, 2015). Covariate information obtained for suicide probands included sex, birth year, and death year. Each proband was matched to five non-suicide controls based on sex and birth year using at-risk sampling whereby a potential control had to be alive at the time of the suicide proband's death but could have died by suicide at a later time.

The UPDB maintains extensive, multi-generational genealogies with founding family members belonging to Utah's original European settlers who migrated to present-day Utah beginning in the mid-19th century. Data from Utah state vital records including birth, death, and divorce certificates are used to construct de novo genealogies, and follow-up information is acquired through regular linkage to state driver license information, records from the Centers of Medicaid and Medicare, and to the Social Security Death Index. The largest families in UPDB include up to 18 generations. First (i.e. siblings, parents, children), second (i.e. grandparents, grandchildren, aunts, uncles, nieces, nephews), third (i.e. great-grandparents, great-grandchild, first cousins, great-uncles, great-aunts, great-nieces, great-nephews), and fifth (i.e. second cousins) degree relatives of suicide probands and matched controls were identified. Additional information obtained for relatives included their sex, birth year, last known year residing in Utah or death year, and manner of death.

Suicide probands were excluded from the analysis if they did not link to known relatives or all relatives were missing critical information (e.g. sex, no follow-up or death information, N = 3335), all of their identified relatives were missing birth year information (N = 898), or if appropriate controls were not available (N = 347) (Fig. 1). Family clusters were identified to take into account the non-independent, correlated structure of the data. A single family cluster included the combined set of all first through fifth-degree relatives of each proband and their matched controls (online Supplementary Fig. S1).

Fig. 1. Flow diagram describing the Utah suicide proband, control, and relative cohorts used in the study.

Statistical methods

Familial risk of suicide

The analysis was conducted in the relatives of the suicide probands and controls whereby the exposure was the suicide proband or the matched control. The familial aggregation of suicide was quantified in a unified modeling framework (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Rebora, Valsecchi, Czene and Reilly2013) by hazard ratios (HR) from an unsupervised Cox regression model, which measured the incidence of suicide in relatives of suicide probands v. controls. Age was used as the underlying time-scale. To account for left censoring due to missing manner of death information, relatives of suicide cases and controls born before 1904 entered the model starting with their age in 1904. All relatives were followed until suicide, death by non-suicide, loss to UPDB follow-up (e.g. migration out of Utah) or 31 December 2014, whichever came first. Individuals populating the suicide proband/control and relative (analysis) cohorts could have contributed to the analysis in multiple ways. First, a small proportion of the control group died of suicide at a later date thereby serving as both a suicide proband and control (N = 245). Second, individual relatives may have belonged to more than one family cluster. A robust sandwich variance estimator (Wei, Lin, & Weissfeld, Reference Wei, Lin and Weissfeld1989) was used to account for dependence among individual family members. Additional covariates included in the model were characteristics of the proband such as sex and characteristics of the relatives such as birth year, sex, and type of relationship to the proband (e.g. sibling, aunt, first cousin).

The hazard of suicide in specific kinships (e.g. children–parents, grandchildren–grandparents) were estimated using two-way interactions between exposure (i.e. relative was exposed to a suicide or control proband) and relative's relationship to the proband. Similarly, four-way interactions that included exposure, proband's sex, relative's sex, and relative's relationship to the proband were used to test for moderation of the familial suicide HR based on sex of the suicide proband or relative. To investigate age effects, the unified Cox regression model was stratified by suicide probands <25 years or ⩾25 years at time of death. This age was selected as prior Utah-based work examining clinical correlates of youth suicide examined suicide through age 25 (Keeshin, Gray, Zhang, Presson, & Coon, Reference Keeshin, Gray, Zhang, Presson and Coon2018). Sensitivity analyses were conducted to examine the robustness of study findings. First, to investigate the potential influence of families with extremely high aggregation of suicide among first-degree relatives, families with (1) three or (2) four or more siblings who died by suicide were removed from the analysis. Next, to investigate the influence of alternative age cut-offs, additional Cox regression models were stratified by suicide probands (1) ⩽18 years or >18 years, and (2) <41 years or ⩾41 years, which is mean age of suicide in Utah (Table 1). Finally, to test the potential influence of using at-risk sampling whereby a matched control could have died by suicide at a later date, additional age-stratified (<25 v. ⩾25 years) Cox regression models were formulated with the 245 matched controls who later died by suicide removed from the analysis.

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of Utah suicides, matched controls and relatives of suicides and matched controls

N, number; s.d., standard deviation.

Attributable risk and population attributable fraction

The attributable risk of suicide is a calculation of the percent of suicides among relatives of suicide probands that is attributable to familial risk. The population attributable fraction is a measure of the percent of Utah suicides that is attributed to familial risk. Age-adjusted attributable and population attributable risk fractions were estimated using the indirect standardization approach according to the STDRATE procedure in SAS (Yuan, Reference Yuan2013). The calculation of attributable risk and the population attributable fraction included all 12 160 suicide deaths used in the main analysis.

Analyses were conducted in SAS version 9.4 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA) and a Type I error was set at 0.05. Institutional Review Board approval was obtained from the University of Utah and the Utah Resource for Genetic and Epidemiologic Research. STROBE reporting guidelines were followed in preparing this study.

Results

Sample characteristics

The final sample included 12 160 Utah suicide probands identified in UPDB born before 2005 with death dates from 1904 to 2014; suicide probands were matched to 53 787 non-suicide probands (Fig. 1). Relatives of suicide and non-suicide probands included 2 112 462 and 11 367 660 first through fifth-degree relatives (includes duplicate individuals that belong to more than one suicide or non-suicide family cluster), respectively. Table 1 summarizes the demographic characteristics of the suicide, matched control, and relatives groups.

Familial risk of suicide in first through fifth-degree relatives

A significantly heightened familial risk of suicide was measured in all first through fifth-degree relatives (Table 2). The overall hazard of suicide was 1.26 [95% confidence intervals (CI) 1.22–1.30] among all first through fifth-degree relatives. First-degree relatives of suicide probands were at more than three times the risk of suicide compared with first-degree relatives of non-suicide probands (HR 3.45; 95% CI 3.12–3.82). The HR of suicide was 3.20 (95% CI 2.71–3.77) in parents, 3.46 (95% CI 2.98–4.03) in children, and 3.67 (95% CI 3.18–4.24) in siblings of suicide probands v. non-suicide probands. The familial risk of suicide in second- through fifth-degree relatives displayed a decreasing dose–response relationship with the HRs ranging from 1.85 (95% CI 1.67–2.06) in nieces/nephews to 1.07 (95% CI 1.02–1.12) in second cousins of suicide probands. A closer examination of parent–child dyads in which both a parent and a child died by suicide showed that in 75% of these cases, the parent died first. However, the mean number of years between parent–child suicides is considerably shorter when the child dies first (9.9 v. 19.2 years; online Supplementary Table S1).

Table 2. Familial risk of suicide in first-, second-, third-, and fifth-degree relatives of suicides and matched controls in Utaha

N, number.

a Model was adjusted for relative's birth year and sex and proband's sex.

b All fifth-degree relatives included in this study were second cousins.

Differences by sex

The familial risks of suicide based on sex of the proband and relative in specific kinships are shown in Fig. 2. Among first-degree relatives, the HR of suicide was 6.99 (95% CI 3.99–12.25) in mothers of female suicide probands compared with mothers of non-suicide female probands. Female probands were also associated with high suicide risks in sisters (HR 6.39; 95% CI 3.78–10.82), daughters (HR 5.65; 95% CI 3.38–9.44), sons (HR 4.48; 95% CI 3.26–6.14), grandmothers (HR 3.84; 95% CI 1.95–7.58), and granddaughters (HR 3.27; 95% CI 1.68–6.35).

Fig. 2. Suicide hazard ratios (HR) ± 95% confidence intervals (y-axis) in relatives of suicide probands v. controls in first- through fifth-degree relatives stratified by suicide proband's sex. Relative of suicide proband is listed on the x-axis. The models were adjusted for relative's sex and birth year and proband's sex.

Differences in the familial risk of suicide according to the proband or relative's sex persisted out to third-degree relatives in some specific kinships. Among great-aunts, the HR associated with the suicide of a great-niece was 2.06 (95% CI 1.37–3.09) compared with an HR of 1.35 (95% CI 1.06–1.73) associated with the suicide of a great-nephew. In contrast, in many kinships where the proband was male, the familial risk of suicide did not differ between male and female relatives. For example, the HR of suicide was similar for brothers (HR 3.40; 95% CI 2.85–4.07) and sisters (HR 3.83; 95% CI 2.92–5.03) of male suicide probands.

Differences by age and age sensitivity analysis

Figure 3 displays the hazard ratios in first-degree relatives of suicide probands v. matched controls stratified by age (<25 v. ⩾25 years). The suicide HR was 4.29 (95% CI 3.49–5.26) for all first-degree relatives of suicide probands who were <25 years at death compared with 3.33 (CI 2.98–3.72) for all first-degree relatives of suicide probands who were ⩾25 years at death. Higher HR estimates for relatives of younger suicide probands v. relatives of older suicide probands persisted across all specific first-degree kinships as well as for most second through fifth-degree kinships, although the risk differences for relatives of younger v. older suicide probands narrows as the relationship becomes more distant (online Supplementary Fig. S2). The highest familial risk of suicide reported in this study was measured among daughters of a parent who died by suicide before age 25 (HR 16.36; 95% CI 4.36–61.44). Using 18 years as the age cut-off for youth suicide did not impact study findings concerning age effects (online Supplementary Table S2). However, data sparseness when using age 18 years as the cut-off prevented HR estimation in some first-degree kinships. The effects of age on suicide risk appeared to disappear when stratifying suicide deaths by the mean age of suicide (41 years) in Utah (online Supplementary Table S3). Study results for the <25 v. ⩾25 years age-stratified models were robust to the removal of matched controls who later died by suicide (online Supplementary Table S4).

Fig. 3. Suicide hazard ratios (HR) ± 95% confidence intervals (y-axis) in first-degree relatives of suicide probands v. controls stratified by <25 v. ⩾25 years of age at time of death. Relative of suicide proband is listed on the x-axis. The models were adjusted for relative's sex and birth year and proband's sex.

Sensitivity analysis, attributable risk, and population attributable risk fraction

Forty-three families with three or more siblings who died by suicide were removed from the analysis and four families with four or more siblings who died by suicide were removed from the analysis. The removal of families with high aggregation of suicide among siblings did not substantively alter the general study findings (online Supplementary Table S5). Finally, the attributable risk of familial suicide was 0.20 (95% CI 0.19–0.22) and the population attributable risk fraction of familial suicide was 0.04 (95% CI 0.035–0.042).

Discussion

The current study represents the first total population-wide investigation of the familial risk of suicide in the USA. Our study included all suicides occurring in a 110-year period in Utah as well as 13.5 million relatives of suicide probands and control. The identification of first- through fifth-degree relatives of suicide probands and controls, born as far back as the 1800s, allowed us to thoroughly examine the familial risk of suicide by degree of genetic relationship.

We found that significant familial liabilities of suicide extend out to fifth-degree relatives and that the sex and age of a suicide proband impacts the suicide risk in his/her relatives. Study findings highlight the importance of acquiring information on an individual's family history of suicide, including the age and sex of the suicide decedent, to prevent suicide in potentially high-risk individuals.

Overall, the risk of suicide was 26% higher among all combined first- through fifth-degree relatives of suicide cases compared with controls. While prior studies have not considered up to fifth-degree relationships, our Utah-based estimates for some first through third-degree kinships are consistent with familial liability estimates reported in similarly designed studies conducted in Northwestern European populations. The current study and a Danish-based study (Qin et al., Reference Qin, Agerbo and Mortensen2003) both report an overall 3.5-fold increase in the familial risk of suicide in first-degree relatives of suicide probands. A Swedish-based study (Tidemalm et al., Reference Tidemalm, Runeson and Waern2011) identified relative risks of 3.1 in siblings (v. 3.7 in Utah), 1.6 in nieces/nephews (v. 1.9 in Utah), and 1.5 in first cousins (v. 1.3 in Utah) of suicide probands. In contrast, two population-based studies conducted in Denmark (Qin et al., Reference Qin, Agerbo and Mortensen2002) and Sweden (Runeson & Åsberg, Reference Runeson and Åsberg2003) identified a more modest two-fold increase in the familial risk of suicide in first-degree relatives.

The opportunity to compare Utah-specific estimates with Northwestern European-based estimates may be particularly relevant given the ancestry of Utah's suicides. While the majority of Utah suicide cases self-identify as White, non-Hispanic (see Limitations section), a recent molecular analysis, allowing for a more refined look at ancestry, determined that 80% of a population-based sample of Utah suicide decedents from 1996 to 2017 (N = 4379) were of majority Northwestern European ancestry (Docherty et al., Reference Docherty, Shabalin, DiBlasi, Monson, Mullins, Adkins, Bacanu and Coon2020). Interestingly, at 21.2 per 100 000 persons (AFSP, Reference :2019), Utah's age-adjusted suicide rate is considerably higher than those observed in Northwestern Europe [e.g. the age-adjusted suicide rate is 11.7 per 100 000 persons in Sweden (World Health Organization, 2019)]. While prior work demonstrates consistency in relative suicide rates among European countries over time (Hansen & Pritchard, Reference Hansen and Pritchard2008), stark differences in age-adjusted suicide rates between Utah and Northwestern Europe suggest that a unique set of factors, beyond ancestral similarities, influence suicide risk differences between locations.

Suicide aggregates in families due to a combination of genetic, shared environmental or gene–environment interactions and the current study was not designed to tease apart the unique contribution of each of these factors. In consideration of genetic contributors, our discovery of significantly elevated familial suicide risks in second- through fifth-degree kinships is an important and novel contribution. The detection of significant familial clustering of suicide well beyond the nuclear family may be evidence of genetic sharing within Utah's extended families, as more distantly related relatives are less likely to share environmental exposures. A considerable genetic contribution to suicide supports findings from prior adoption (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Ohlsson, Sundguist, Sundguist and Edwards2020; Petersen et al., Reference Petersen, Sørensen, Andersen, Mortensen and Hawton2013; Schulsinger et al., Reference Schulsinger, Kety, Rosenthal, Wender, Schou and Stromgren1979; von Borczyskowski et al., Reference von Borczyskowski, Lindblad, Vinnerljung, Reintjes and Hjern2011) and twin (Juel-Nielsen & Videbech, Reference Juel-Nielsen and Videbech1970; Pedersen & Fiske, Reference Pedersen and Fiske2010; Roy et al., Reference Roy, Segal, Centerwall and Robinette1991) studies. The specific genes implicated in suicide, however, remain unclear. While over 200 genes are associated with suicidal behaviors (Lutz, Mechawar, & Turecki, Reference Lutz, Mechawar and Turecki2017), many of these gene findings require replication. Further, the majority of gene-focused suicide studies have been conducted in individuals exhibiting suicidal behavior and not in suicide decedents (Coon et al., Reference Coon, Darlington, DiBlasi, Callor, Ferris, Fraser, ]Yu and Gray2020) (but see Coon et al., Reference Coon, Darlington, DiBlasi, Callor, Ferris, Fraser, ]Yu and Gray2020; Darlington et al., Reference Darlington, Pimentel, Smith, Bakian, Jerominski, Cardon, Camp and Coon2014; Docherty et al., Reference Docherty, Shabalin, DiBlasi, Monson, Mullins, Adkins, Bacanu and Coon2020; Otsuka et al., Reference Otsuka, Akiyama, Shirakawa, Momozawa, Kamatani, Izumi, Numata and Hishimoto2019; Tombáca et al., Reference Tombáca, Maróti, Kalmár, Csabai, Balázs, Takahashi, Palkovits and Boldogkói2017). To better understand suicide's genetic architecture, additional work is needed in suicide death cohorts.

Several environmental contributors to suicide are known to be transmitted through families and may impact study findings, especially among close kinships. Increased suicide risk during youth and adolescence has been linked to exposure to early-life adversity from parental neglect, substance abuse, criminality (Björkenstam, Hjern, Björkenstam, & Kosidou, Reference Björkenstam, Hjern, Björkenstam and Kosidou2018; Björkenstam, Kosidou, & Björkenstam, Reference Björkenstam, Kosidou and Björkenstam2017; Brent et al., Reference Brent, Perper, Moritz, Liotus, Schweers, Balach and Roth1994), and interpersonal violence (Rajalin, Hirvikoski, & Jokinen, Reference Rajalin, Hirvikoski and Jokinen2013). Epigenetic modification following exposure to early-life adversity of genes involved in pathways underlying suicide including neural plasticity, neuroprotection, stress, and cognition provides evidence of potential biological effects from such exposures (Turecki & Brent, Reference Turecki and Brent2016). Beyond youth and adolescent suicide, a family history of psychiatric conditions and hospitalization for psychiatric conditions among first-degree relatives has been shown to be linked to an increased risk of suicide across the lifespan (Agerbo et al., Reference Agerbo, Nordentoft and Mortensen2002; Qin et al., Reference Qin, Agerbo and Mortensen2002, Reference Qin, Agerbo and Mortensen2003). In contrast, little is currently known about how a family history of early-life adversity and psychopathology influences the risk of suicide in more distantly related relatives (e.g. beyond first-degree relationships).

Our findings of heightened familial risks of suicide in extended family members may be evidence of gene–environment interactions. While distant relatives are less likely than close relatives to share environmental exposures, the environmental factors they do share are more likely to be those that are widespread across a population (Amundadottir et al., Reference Amundadottir, Thorvaldsson, Gudbjartsson, Sulem, Kristjansson, Arnason, Gulcher and Stefansson2004). Suicide population attributable fraction estimates attest to the important contribution of widespread, population-level environmental factors to risk. For example, the attributable risks of suicide associated with being single range from 10.3% (Qin et al., Reference Qin, Agerbo and Mortensen2003) to 25.6% (Mortensen, Agerbo, Erikson, Qin, & Westergaard-Nielsen, Reference Mortensen, Agerbo, Erikson, Qin and Westergaard-Nielsen2000).

We found that sex and age influence the familial risk of suicide. Although suicide is more common in males, our work supports prior research demonstrating especially heightened liabilities of suicide in first-degree relatives of female (Cheng et al., Reference Cheng, Yen, Chang, Wu, Ko and Li2014; Garssen et al., Reference Garssen, Deerenberg, Mackenbach, Kerkhof and Kunst2011; Petersen et al., Reference Petersen, Sørensen, Andersen, Mortensen and Hawton2013; Qin et al., Reference Qin, Agerbo and Mortensen2003; Qin & Mortensen, Reference Qin and Mortensen2003) and younger (Garssen et al., Reference Garssen, Deerenberg, Mackenbach, Kerkhof and Kunst2011; Qin & Mortensen, Reference Qin and Mortensen2003) suicide decedents. Our findings also indicate that female and youth transmission of risk extends beyond the nuclear family. The heritability of suicide mortality has been hypothesized to be higher in women than men (Pedersen & Fiske, Reference Pedersen and Fiske2010), which might in-part explain the high risks of suicide in female-specific kinships (e.g. mother-daughters, sisters). Alternatively, the loss of a mother may represent the disappearance of a critical source of support and caregiving. Such loss may be especially heightened among daughters as parental attachment and the transmission of learned behaviors to children appears to be more strongly linked to their same-sex parent (Diener, Isabella, Behunin, & Wong, Reference Diener, Isabella, Behunin and Wong2008). In addition, psychopathology is a strong risk factor for suicide (Arsenault-Lapierre, Kim, & Turecki, Reference Arsenault-Lapierre, Kim and Turecki2004; Qin et al., Reference Qin, Agerbo and Mortensen2002) with some forms of psychopathology (i.e. major depressive disorder) occurring more frequently in women (Salk, Hyde, & Abramson, Reference Salk, Hyde and Abramson2017; Oquendo et al., Reference Oquendo, Ellis, Greenwald, Malone, Weissman and Mann2001). Although suicide and psychopathology risk both cluster in families, their transmission, despite some overlap, has been shown to be distinct (Brent, Bridge, Johnson, & Connolly, Reference Brent, Bridge, Johnson and Connolly1996; Egeland & Sussex, Reference Egeland and Sussex1985; Qin et al., Reference Qin, Agerbo and Mortensen2003). It is not known, however, if the familial transmission of suicide and psychopathology differs by sex and is more likely to occur among women. In contrast, it is important to note that the low absolute suicide rate in females in Utah could result in relatively high hazard ratios. In terms of age effects, an inverse relationship has been observed between early age-of-onset and familial risk for a variety of medical and psychiatric conditions (Gillespie, Gale, & Bingley, Reference Gillespie, Gale and Bingley2002; Kendler, Gatz, Gardner, & Pedersen, Reference Kendler, Gatz, Gardner and Pedersen2005; Kharazmi, Fallah, Sundquist, & Memminki, Reference Kharazmi, Fallah, Sundquist and Memminki2012; Nestadt et al., Reference Nestadt, Samuels, Riddle, Bienvenu, Liang, LaBuda, Walkup and Hoehn-Saric2000).

Limitations of the current study are important to recognize. First, although the models were adjusted for relative's sex and birth year and proband's sex, they were not adjusted for additional confounders including socioeconomic status, urbanicity, and history of mental illness and trauma. Prior work indicates that familial suicide risk estimates are robust to adjustment by additional covariates, especially among first-degree relatives who likely share potential confounders, however their inclusion has been shown to slightly attenuate effect sizes (Qin et al., Reference Qin, Agerbo and Mortensen2002, Reference Qin, Agerbo and Mortensen2003). The generalizability of study findings to the broader USA is unknown and may, in particular, be influenced by the racial and ethnic composition of Utah's suicide population. Information in the UPDB indicates that 95.2% of the suicide deaths since 1904 self-identified as White, non-Hispanic. Of note, the 3335 suicide deaths excluded from the study due to a lack of relatives in UPDB, who might be expected to be more recent in-migrants to Utah and therefore more racially and ethnically diverse, were of similar race and ethnicity (92.3% White, non-Hispanic). Although racially and ethnically homogeneous, Utah's age-adjusted suicide rates are among the highest in the USA (AFSP, Reference :2019) suggesting that Utah-specific discoveries may apply to other high suicide risk populations in the USA with similar demographic compositions. Finally, the current study relied on death certificate determination of suicide, which is susceptible to possible misclassification of suicide deaths as undetermined or accidental (Mohler & Earls, Reference Mohler and Earls2001; Ohberg & Lonnqvist, Reference Ohberg and Lonnqvist1998; Rosenberg et al., Reference Rosenberg, Davidson, Smith, Berman, Buzbee, Gantner, Gay and Jobes1988). In particular, the majority of Utah's population are members of the Church of Jesus Christ of Latter Day Saints (LDS), and evidence suggests that suicide misclassification may be more likely in areas with strong single religion identities (Prichard & Hansen, Reference Prichard and Hansen2015). Utah-based work, however, reports that highly active male members of the LDS church are at a reduced risk of suicide relative to their less active and non-LDS peers (Hilton, Fellingham, & Lyon, Reference Hilton, Fellingham and Lyon2002). Consistency in the method of death determination in Utah is further assisted by the use of a single, centralized Office of the Medical Examiner. Any death misclassification is likely to result in a conservative determination of suicide with the associated bias attenuating effect estimates toward the null.

Despite these limitations, our study has a number of strengths. The UPDB provides access to unprecedented resources for conducting population-wide analyses; a similar study of this magnitude is not currently feasible elsewhere in the USA. The depth of genealogical and death certificate data maintained in UPDB is more extensive than what is available in similar, primarily European health registries. We used a unified modeling approach, which minimized the formulation of multiple models thereby improving effect estimate precision. Further, our modeling approach accounted for participant clustering within multiple families reducing the risk of selection bias (Bai, Sherman, Khoury, & Flanders, Reference Bai, Sherman, Khoury and Flanders2000).

In conclusion, we found that the familial liability of suicide extends from first- through fifth-degree relatives in Utah with the magnitude of risk in relatives varying based on their sex and the sex and age of the suicide proband. The attributable risk of familial suicide was estimated to be 20% suggesting that our findings have important implications for suicide prevention on an individual-level and within a family. In particular, our study indicates an increased intensity of interventions for suicidal young adults and women with a strong family history of suicide. Further attention to this approach to suicide prevention may be especially warranted in the USA given recent increases in suicide rates among young adults (Miron, Yu, Wilf-Miron, & Kohane, Reference Miron, Yu, Wilf-Miron and Kohane2019), especially females (Ruch et al., Reference Ruch, Sheftall, Schlagbaum, Rausch, Campo and Bridge2019).

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721003020.

Acknowledgements

We thank the Pedigree and Population Resource of the Huntsman Cancer Institute, University of Utah (funded in part by the Huntsman Cancer Foundation) for its role in the ongoing collection, maintenance, and support of the Utah Population Database. We would also like to acknowledge Dr Marie Reilly and her colleagues for sharing their SAS code for formulating the unified models used in this study and for approving the use of a figure from their paper in the online materials.

Financial support

This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health (HC, grant number R01MH099134; KS, grant number R01AG022095; ARD, grant number K01MH093731; AVB, grant number R01ES032028), the American Foundation for Suicide Prevention (AVB & ARD), the Brain & Behavior Research Foundation (ARD), the Clark Tanner Foundation (HC & AVB), and the University of Utah EDGE Scholar Program (ARD). We also acknowledge partial support for the Utah Population Database from the National Cancer Institute (grant P30CA2014), the University of Utah, the University of Utah's Program in Personalized Health, and the Utah Clinical and Translational Science Institute.

Conflict of interest

None.

References

American Foundation for Suicide Prevention (AFSP) (2019) Suicide facts & figures :, Utah: American Foundation for Suicide Prevention (AFSP). Retrieved from https://afsp.org/about-suicide/state-fact-sheets/#Utah.Google Scholar
Agerbo, E. (2005). Midlife suicide risk, partner's psychiatric illness, spouse and child bereavement by suicide or other modes of death: A gender specific study. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 59, 407412.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Agerbo, E., Nordentoft, M., & Mortensen, P. (2002). Familial, psychiatric, and socioeconomic risk factors for suicide in young people: Nested case-control study. British Medical Journal, 325, 74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amundadottir, L. T., Thorvaldsson, S., Gudbjartsson, D. F., Sulem, P., Kristjansson, K., Arnason, S., Gulcher, J. R., … Stefansson, K. (2004). Cancer as a complex phenotype: Pattern of cancer distribution within and beyond the nuclear family. PLoS Medicine, 1, e65. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.0010065CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arsenault-Lapierre, G., Kim, C., & Turecki, G. (2004). Psychiatric diagnoses in 3275 suicide: A meta-analysis. BMC Psychiatry, 4, 37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bai, Y., Sherman, S., Khoury, M., & Flanders, D. (2000). Bias associated with study protocols in epidemiologic studies of disease familial aggregation. American Journal of Epidemiology, 151, 927937.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belsher, B. E., Smolenski, D. J., & Pruitt, L. D. (2019). Prediction models for suicide attempts and deaths: A systematic review and simulation. JAMA Psychiatry, 76, 642651.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Björkenstam, E., Hjern, A., Björkenstam, C., & Kosidou, K. (2018). Association of cumulative childhood adversity and adolescent violent offending with suicide in early adulthood. JAMA Psychiatry, 75, 185193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Björkenstam, C., Kosidou, K., & Björkenstam, E. (2017). Childhood adversity and risk of suicide: Cohort study of 548 721 adolescents and young adults in Sweden. The BMJ, 357, j1334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brent, D. A., Bridge, J., Johnson, B. A., & Connolly, J. (1996). Suicidal behavior runs in families: A controlled family study of adolescent suicide victims. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53, 11451152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brent, D. A., Perper, J. A., Moritz, G., Liotus, L., Schweers, J., Balach, L., & Roth, C. (1994). Familial risk factors for adolescent suicide: A case-control study. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 89, 5258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheng, C. C. J., Yen, W. J., Chang, W. T., Wu, K. C. C., Ko, M. C., & Li, C. Y. (2014). Risk of adolescent offspring's completed suicide increases with prior history of their same-sex parents’ death by suicide. Psychological Medicine, 44, 18451854.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coon, H., Darlington, T. M., DiBlasi, E., Callor, W. B., Ferris, E., Fraser, A., ]Yu, Z., … Gray, D. (2020). Genome-wide significant regions in 43 Utah high-risk families implicate multiple genes involved in risk for completed suicide. Molecular Psychiatry, 25, 30773090. Doi: 10.1038/s41380-018-0282-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Darlington, T. M., Pimentel, R., Smith, K., Bakian, A. V., Jerominski, L., Cardon, J., Camp, N. J., … Coon, H. (2014). Identifying rare variants for genetic risk through a combined pedigree and phenotype approach: Application to suicide and asthma. Translational Psychiatry, 4, e471.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diener, M. L., Isabella, R. A., Behunin, M. G., & Wong, M. S. (2008). Attachment to mothers and fathers during middle childhood: Associations with child gender, grade, and competence. Social Development, 17, 84101.Google Scholar
Docherty, A. R., Shabalin, A. A., DiBlasi, E., Monson, E., Mullins, N., Adkins, D. E., Bacanu, S., … Coon, H. (2020). Genome-wide association study of suicide death and polygenic prediction of clinical antecedents. American Journal of Psychiatry, 117, 917927.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Egeland, J. A., & Sussex, J. N. (1985). Suicide and family loading for affective disorder. JAMA, 254, 915918.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garssen, J., Deerenberg, I., Mackenbach, J. P., Kerkhof, A., & Kunst, A. E. (2011). Familial risk of early suicide: Variations by age and sex of children and parents. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 41, 585593.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gillespie, K. M., Gale, E. A. M., & Bingley, P. J. (2002). High familial risk and genetic susceptibility in early onset childhood diabetes. Diabetes, 51, 210214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gordon, J. A., Avenevoli, S., & Pearson, J. L. (2020). Suicide prevention research priorities in health care. JAMA Psychiatry, 77, 885886.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hansen, L., & Pritchard, C. (2008). Consistency in suicide rates in twenty-two developed countries by gender over time 1874–78, 1974–76, and 1998–2000. Archives of Suicide Research, 12, 251262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hedegaard, H., Curtin, S. C., & Warner, M. (2018). Suicide mortality in the United States, 1999–2017. NCHS data brief, no 330. Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health Statistics.Google Scholar
Hilton, S. C., Fellingham, G. W., & Lyon, J. L. (2002). Suicide rates and religious commitment in young adult males in Utah. American Journal of Epidemiology, 155, 413419.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hopper, J. L., Bishop, D. T., & Easton, D. F. (2005). Population-based family studies in genetic epidemiology. Lancet (London, England), 366, 1521.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Juel-Nielsen, N., & Videbech, T. (1970). A twin study of suicide. Acta Geneticae Medicae et Gemellologiae, 19, 307310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keeshin, B. R., Gray, D., Zhang, C., Presson, A. P., & Coon, H. (2018). Youth suicide deaths: Investigation of clinical predictors in a statewide sample. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 48, 601612.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendler, K. S., Gatz, M., Gardner, C. O., & Pedersen, N. L. (2005). Age at onset and familial risk for major depression in a Swedish national twin sample. Psychological Medicine, 35, 15731579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendler, K. S., Ohlsson, H., Sundguist, J., Sundguist, K., & Edwards, A. C. (2020). The sources of parent-child transmission of risk for suicide attempt and deaths by suicide in Swedish national samples. American Journal of Psychiatry, 177, 928935. Doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2020.20010017.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kharazmi, E., Fallah, M., Sundquist, K., & Memminki, K. (2012). Familial risk of early and late onset cancer: Nationwide prospective cohort study. British Medical Journal, 345, e8076.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, C. D., Sequin, M., Therrien, N., Riopel, G., Chawky, N., Lesage, A. D., & Turecki, G. (2005). Familial aggregation of suicidal behavior: A family study of male suicide completers from the general population. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 10171019.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, M., Rebora, P., Valsecchi, M. G., Czene, K., & Reilly, M. (2013). A unified model for estimating and testing familial aggregation. Statistics in Medicine, 32, 53535365.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lu, T. H. (2003). Using ACME (automatic classification of medical entry) software to monitor and improve the quality of cause of death statistics. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 57, 470471.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lutz, P. E., Mechawar, N., & Turecki, G. (2017). Neuropathology of suicide: Recent findings and future directions. Molecular Psychiatry, 22, 13951412.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miron, O., Yu, K., Wilf-Miron, R., & Kohane, I. S. (2019). Suicide rates among adolescents and young adults in the United States, 2000–2017. JAMA, 321, 23622364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mohler, B., & Earls, F. (2001). Trends in adolescent suicide: Misclassification bias? American Journal of Public Health, 91, 150153.Google ScholarPubMed
Mortality Medical Data System (MMDS) (2015) U.S. CDC National Vital Statistics System. Retrieved from https://www.cdc.gov/nchs/nvss/mmds.htm.Google Scholar
Mortensen, P. B., Agerbo, E., Erikson, T., Qin, P., & Westergaard-Nielsen, N. (2000). Psychiatric illness and risk factors for suicide in Denmark. Lancet (London, England), 355, 912.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, H. D., Denneson, L. M., Low, A. R., Bauer, B. W., O'Neil, M., Kansagara, D., & Teo, A. R. (2017). Suicide risk assessment and prevention: A systematic review focusing on veterans. Psychiatric Services, 68, 10031015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nestadt, G., Samuels, J., Riddle, M., Bienvenu, J., Liang, K., LaBuda, M., Walkup, J., … Hoehn-Saric, R. (2000). A family study of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 358363.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ohberg, A., & Lonnqvist, J. (1998). Suicides hidden among undetermined deaths. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 98, 214218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oquendo, M. A., Ellis, S. P., Greenwald, S., Malone, K. M., Weissman, M. M., & Mann, J. J. (2001). Ethnic and sex differences in suicide rates relative to major depression in the United States. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 16521658.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Otsuka, I., Akiyama, M., Shirakawa, O., Momozawa, Y., Kamatani, Y., Izumi, T., Numata, S., … Hishimoto, A. (2019). Genome-wide association studies identify polygenic effects for completed suicide in the Japanese population. Neuropsychopharmacology, 44, 21192124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pedersen, N. L., & Fiske, A. (2010). Genetic influences on suicide and nonfatal suicidal behavior: Twin study findings. European Psychiatry, 25, 264267.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, L., Sørensen, T. I. A., Andersen, P. K., Mortensen, P. B., & Hawton, K. (2013). Genetic and familial environmental effects on suicide-an adoption study of siblings. PLoS ONE, 8, e77973.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prichard, C., & Hansen, L. (2015). Examining undetermined and accidental deaths as a source of ‘under-reported-suicide’ by age and sex in twenty western countries. Journal of Community Mental Health, 51, 365376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Qin, P., Agerbo, E., & Mortensen, P. B. (2002). Suicide risk in relation to family history of completed suicide and psychiatric disorders: A nested case-control study based on longitudinal registers. Lancet (London, England), 360, 11261130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Qin, P., Agerbo, E., & Mortensen, P. B. (2003). Suicide risk in relation to socioeconomic, demographic, psychiatric, and familial factors: A national register-based study of all suicides in Denmark, 1981–1997. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 765772.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Qin, P., & Mortensen, P. B. (2003). The impact of parental status on the risk of completed suicide. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 797802.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rajalin, M., Hirvikoski, T., & Jokinen, J. (2013). Family history of suicide and exposure to interpersonal violence in childhood predict suicide in male suicide attempters. Journal of Affective Disorders, 148, 9292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riblet, N., Shiner, B., Young-Xu, Y., & Watts, B. (2017). Strategies to prevent death by suicide: Meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 210, 396402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenberg, M. L., Davidson, L. E., Smith, J. C., Berman, A. L., Buzbee, H., Gantner, G., Gay, G. A., … Jobes, D. (1988). Operational criteria for the determination of suicide. Journal of Forensic Sciences, 33, 14451456.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roy, A., Segal, N. L., Centerwall, B. S., & Robinette, C. D. (1991). Suicide in twins. Archives of General Psychiatry, 48, 2932.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruch, D. A., Sheftall, A. H., Schlagbaum, P., Rausch, J., Campo, J. V., & Bridge, J. A. (2019). Trends in suicide among youth aged 10 to 19 years in the United States, 1975 to 2016. JAMA Network Open, 2, e193886.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runeson, B., & Åsberg, M. (2003). Family history of suicide among suicide victims. American Journal of Psychiatry, 168, 15251526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salk, R. H., Hyde, J. S., & Abramson, L. Y. (2017). Gender differences in depression in representative national samples: Meta-analyses of diagnoses and symptoms. Psychological Bulletin, 143, 783822.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schulsinger, F., Kety, S. S., Rosenthal, D., & Wender, P. H. (1979). A family study of suicide. In Schou, M. & Stromgren, E. (Eds.), Origin, prevention and treatment of affective disorders (pp. 277287). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Smith, K., & Fraser, A. (2018). If you link it they will come, if they like it they will stay: The Utah population database as a model for creating a confidential linked population health care registry. International Journal of Population Data Science, 8, 430.Google Scholar
Stone, D. M., Simon, T. R., Fowler, K. A., Kegler, S. R., Yuan, K., Holland, K. M., … Crosby, A. E. (2018). Vital signs: Trends in state suicide rates – United States, 1999–2016 and circumstances contributing to suicide – 27 states, 2015. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 67, 617624.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tidemalm, D., Runeson, B., & Waern, M. (2011). Familial clustering of suicide risk: A total population study of 11.4 million individuals. Psychological Medicine, 41, 25272534.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tombáca, D., Maróti, Z., Kalmár, T., Csabai, Z., Balázs, Z., Takahashi, S., Palkovits, M., … Boldogkói, Z. (2017). High-coverage whole-exome sequencing identifies candidate genes for suicide in victims with major depressive disorder. Scientific Reports, 7, 7106. Doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-06522-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turecki, G. (2014). The molecular bases of the suicidal brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 15, 803816.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turecki, G., & Brent, D. A. (2016). Suicide and suicidal behaviour. The Lancet, 387, 12271239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
von Borczyskowski, A., Lindblad, F., Vinnerljung, B., Reintjes, R., & Hjern, A. (2011). Familial factors and suicide: An adoption study in a Swedish national cohort. Psychological Medicine, 41, 749758.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wei, L. J., Lin, D. Y., & Weissfeld, L. (1989). Regression analysis of multivariate incomplete failure time data by modeling marginal distribution. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 84, 10651073.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
World Health Organization (2019). Suicide in the world, global health estimates. Retrieved from https://www.who.int/publications/i/item/suicide-in-the-world.Licence: CC BY-NC-SA 3.0 IGO.Google Scholar
Yuan, Y. (2013). Computing direct and indirect standardized rates and risks with the STDRATE procedure. SAS Global Forum 2013. Paper 423–2013.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Flow diagram describing the Utah suicide proband, control, and relative cohorts used in the study.

Figure 1

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of Utah suicides, matched controls and relatives of suicides and matched controls

Figure 2

Table 2. Familial risk of suicide in first-, second-, third-, and fifth-degree relatives of suicides and matched controls in Utaha

Figure 3

Fig. 2. Suicide hazard ratios (HR) ± 95% confidence intervals (y-axis) in relatives of suicide probands v. controls in first- through fifth-degree relatives stratified by suicide proband's sex. Relative of suicide proband is listed on the x-axis. The models were adjusted for relative's sex and birth year and proband's sex.

Figure 4

Fig. 3. Suicide hazard ratios (HR) ± 95% confidence intervals (y-axis) in first-degree relatives of suicide probands v. controls stratified by <25 v. ⩾25 years of age at time of death. Relative of suicide proband is listed on the x-axis. The models were adjusted for relative's sex and birth year and proband's sex.

Supplementary material: File

Bakian et al. supplementary material

Bakian et al. supplementary material

Download Bakian et al. supplementary material(File)
File 434.8 KB