Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-pftt2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-02T12:30:36.331Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Who Believes in Fraud in the 2006 Mexican Presidential Election? Election Denialism, Partisan Motivated Reasoning, and Affective Polarization

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 May 2024

Rodrigo Castro Cornejo*
Affiliation:
University of Massachusetts–Lowell, Lowell, Massachusetts, US
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Why do some voters believe that there is electoral fraud when this belief contradicts the best available evidence? While the literature on public opinion has explored misperceptions’ pervasiveness and the factors that contribute to them in advanced industrial democracies, the present study analyzes motivated reasoning among voters in a young democracy, Mexico. This study highlights the important role that partisanship plays in voters’ likelihood to believe the allegations of electoral fraud in the 2006 presidential election in Mexico, which continues polarizing both political elites and the electorate even today. To understand the mechanisms at work, this research finds that it is not voters lacking information but rather voters with high levels of affective polarization and conspiratorial thinking who are more likely to believe that there was electoral fraud. The study also includes a survey experiment that fact-checked the belief in the alleged electoral fraud. Consistent with motivated reasoning theory, MORENA partisans resisted efforts to reduce their misperception. The findings of this study contribute to our understanding of the conditions that make some voters hold misperceptions in young democracies.

Resumen

Resumen

¿Por qué algunos votantes creen que hubo fraude electoral cuando esta creencia contradice la mejor evidencia disponible? Si bien la literatura sobre la opinión pública ha estudiado las percepciones erróneas y los factores que contribuyen a ellas en las democracias industriales avanzadas, el presente estudio analiza el razonamiento motivado entre los votantes en una democracia joven, México. Este estudio destaca el importante papel que juega la identificación partidista en la probabilidad que los votantes crean en las acusaciones de fraude electoral en las elecciones presidenciales de 2006 en México, que continúan polarizando tanto a las élites políticas como al electorado incluso hoy. Para comprender los mecanismos existentes, esta investigación encuentra que no es la falta de información, sino que los votantes con altos niveles de polarización afectiva y pensamiento conspirativo son más propensos a creer que hubo un fraude electoral. Este estudio también incluye un experimento de encuesta que verificó la creencia en el supuesto fraude electoral. Consistente con la teoría del razonamiento motivado, los partidarios de MORENA se resistieron a la intervención para reducir su percepción errónea. Los hallazgos de este estudio contribuyen a nuestra comprensión de las condiciones que hacen que algunos votantes tengan percepciones erróneas en las democracias jóvenes.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Latin American Studies Association

Election denialism is an increasingly important issue in elections around the world. In the United States, Mexico, and Brazil, political elites and the public are increasingly questioning the validity of election results. In the United States, 60 percent of Republicans strongly or somewhat agreed that the 2020 election was stolen from Donald Trump (Ipsos 2021). In the 2022 Brazilian presidential election, Jair Bolsonaro delegitimized the election results before election day. As a result, thousands of his supporters took part in demonstrations refusing to recognize the new president-elect—even calling for a military intervention (Ellsworth and Viga Reference Ellsworth and Viga2022). In Mexico, according to the 2021 Mexican Election Study, after more than fifteen years, at least one-fourth of the public claims that Andrés Manuel López Obrador won the 2006 presidential election (Beltrán, Castro Cornejo, and Ley Reference Beltrán, Castro Cornejo and Ley2023), even with no indication of significant irregularities that affected the results of the election (Aparicio Reference Aparicio2009; Bruhn Reference Bruhn and Mudde2012).

Why do some voters believe that there is electoral fraud when this belief contradicts the best available evidence? While the existing literature has focused on how “winners” and “losers” systematically differ with respect to postelection attitudes (Anderson et al. Reference Anderson, Blais, Bowler, Donovan and Listhaug2005), this study seeks to understand how these perceptions are formed. In line with previous studies on public opinion formation, this research argues that voters can perceive the same fact—in this particular case, the results of an election—and still make different judgments about its meaning (Gaines et al. Reference Gaines, Kuklinski, Quirk, Peyton and Verkuilen2007). Prior studies have explored misperceptions’ pervasiveness and the factors that contribute to them in advanced industrial democracies (Nyhan and Refiler Reference Nyhan and Reifler2010; Miller, Saunders, and Farhart Reference Miller, Saunders and Farhart2016). This study analyzes election denialism among voters in a young democracy: Mexico.

While some studies have previously argued that partisanship in Mexico is weak (Greene Reference Greene2011), recent studies in Mexico (Castro Cornejo Reference Castro Cornejo2021) and across Latin America (Lupu Reference Lupu2015; Calvo and Ventura Reference Calvo and Ventura2021; Haime and Cantú Reference Haime and Cantú2022) have found that, similar to old democracies, partisans are more informed and engaged than nonpartisans and that their partisan attachments moderate their attitudes and voting behavior. In that sense, this research argues that citizens in young democracies like Mexico also engage in partisan-motivated reasoning (Taber and Lodge Reference Taber and Lodge2006)—that is, the tendency to accept information with the goal of arriving at predetermined conclusions. Advancing the literature on motivated reasoning, this study seeks to understand which individual-level factors, in addition to partisanship, make voters more likely to engage in election denialism. Affective polarization—fear and loathing between out-partisans (Iyengar and Westwood, Reference Iyengar and Westwood2015)—is likely to drive belief in electoral fraud because voters have the tendency to form in and out-groups to protect their identities and self-esteem (Sherif Reference Sherif1961). This study also considers alternative hypotheses such as voters’ tendency to conspiratorial thinking, as false claims in election results fit the definition of a conspiracy theory: “unsubstantiated accusatory beliefs positing small groups working in secret and against the common good” (Edelson et al. Reference Edelson, Alduncin, Krewson, Sieja and Uscinski2017, 1). This makes underlying conspiratorial thinking relevant for some voters to understand political events. Finally, levels of political information can also moderate such beliefs since more informed voters are expected to be less likely to believe in rumors without solid evidence (Bolsen, Druckman, and Lomax Cook Reference Bolsen, Druckman and Lomax Cook2014).

This research focuses on perceptions of electoral fraud in the 2006 Mexican presidential election, which continues to polarize political elites and the electorate today. In the 2006 presidential election, the candidate of the Party of the Democratic Revolution (PRD), Andrés Manuel López Obrador, claimed that a corrupt elite—a “political mafia” (mafia del poder in Spanish)—made up of the two main parties in Mexico, the Institutional Revolutionary Party (PRI) and the National Action Party (PAN), as well as the business sector and electoral institutions, had stolen the presidency from him (Bruhn Reference Bruhn and Mudde2012). Although the Federal Electoral Institute (Instituto Federal Electoral, IFE), international observers, and most parties in Mexico besides López Obrador’s PRD stated that the electoral process had been free and fair (Eisenstadt Reference Eisenstadt2007; Ugues Reference Ugues2010), López Obrador organized massive protests and refused to accept the election’s outcome (Aparicio Reference Aparicio2009; Bruhn Reference Bruhn and Mudde2012). In the 2012 and 2018 presidential campaigns, López Obrador relied on a populist rhetoric similar to that of his 2006 campaign, and he again denounced the same political establishment for being part of a corrupt elite that had robbed him of the presidency in 2006 and impoverished Mexico with neoliberal policies and widespread corruption. This misperception—a belief that is false or contradicts the best evidence available in the public domain (Berinsky Reference Berinsky2015; Flynn, Nyhan, and Reifler Reference Flynn, Nyhan and Reifler2017)—has remained pervasive within the Mexican electorate for more than a decade. During the 2018 presidential election, 24 percent of the electorate believed that it was “very true” that an electoral fraud took place in the 2006 presidential election (and 24 percent “somewhat true”).

This study is based on two original surveys conducted during the 2018 presidential campaign as well as the 2021 Mexican Election Study (Beltrán, Castro Cornejo, and Ley Reference Beltrán, Castro Cornejo and Ley2023)—part of the Comparative Study of Electoral Systems—conducted after the midterm election. Based on observational data, this research highlights the important role that partisanship plays in voters’ likelihood to engage in election denialism: voters who identify with López Obrador’s party, MORENA (National Regeneration Movement), are more likely to believe that the 2006 presidential election was fraudulent than any other partisan group. Moreover, this research identifies that affective polarization and high levels of conspiratorial thinking, and not voters’ levels of information, drive their belief in the allegations of electoral fraud in 2006. These results hold in both surveys conducted in 2018 and 2021, even when controlling for the opinion of Andrés Manuel López Obrador and voters’ general distrust in Mexican elections. Finally, to understand the persistence of this misperception, a survey experiment confirmed the belief in the alleged 2006 electoral fraud. Consistent with motivated reasoning theory, MORENA partisans resisted efforts to change their views. Instead of updating their beliefs, they continued to believe in electoral fraud.

The findings of this study contribute to our understanding of the conditions that make some voters hold misperceptions in young democracies, which can undermine the legitimacy of elections and erode faith in democratic norms (Norris, Garnett, and Gromping Reference Norris, Garnett and Gromping2020). While election denialism is not new in the region or elsewhere (Hernández-Huerta Reference Hernández-Huerta2020), candidates and parties increasingly refuse to accept the results of elections, particularly in countries that have recently experienced democratic erosion (e.g. USA and Brazil). The results also speak to the literature on populism and party systems, particularly the studies on the 2018 presidential election in Mexico. As recent studies have found (Castro Cornejo, Ley, and Beltrán Reference Castro Cornejo and Ley2020), the belief that a corrupt elite stole the 2006 election evolved into a powerful grievance for a significant part of the Mexican electorate and became an important predictor of the electorate’s populist mobilization. Just like in Latin American party systems that suffered systemic decay—such as Venezuela or Perú (Morgan Reference Morgan2007)—voters’ grievances created anger and moral outrage against the political establishment (Coppedge Reference Coppedge1997; Seawright Reference Seawright2012), increasing voters’ incentives to punish the incumbent government.

Perceptions of electoral fraud in the 2006 presidential election in Mexico

The Mexican party system can be considered fairly institutionalized compared to the rest of the party systems in the region (Mainwaring Reference Mainwaring2018). Although Mexico has only been considered a democracy since 2000, its political parties have existed for decades. The authoritarian successor party (PRI), the center-right PAN, and the center-left PRD opposition parties were key actors during Mexico’s democratic transition—negotiating electoral reforms and channeling social demands—from 1988 to 2000. The 2006 presidential election constituted the first major political crisis in Mexico’s young democracy. In that election, the PAN candidate, Felipe Calderón, narrowly defeated Andrés Manuel López Obrador, supported by the PRD and a coalition of leftist parties, in a highly contested race (Klesner Reference Klesner2007). Calderón’s margin of victory was only about 0.58%. Although the IFE, international observers, and most parties in Mexico—except for López Obrador’s PRD—confirmed that the electoral process had been free and fair, López Obrador denounced the election as fraudulent and contested the results (Ugues Reference Ugues2010). The Mexican Electoral Tribunal ordered that the IFE undertake a partial ballot recount but did not find any significant or systematic bias against López Obrador. Because there was never a full recount of the votes as demanded by López Obrador’s team—and some administrative controversies emerged during the campaign (Schedler Reference Schedler2007)—his campaign delegitimized the election. Footnote 1 While it is unclear exactly how any fraud supposedly took place, since López Obrador and his campaign team changed their accusations multiple times (e.g., from “cybernetic” fraud to allegations that there were thousands of hidden votes or that votes were incorrectly counted; see Aparicio, Reference Aparicio2009, for a review), the accusations triggered a significant postelection crisis.

The accusations about electoral fraud became one of the most polarizing issues in post-2006 Mexico politics. Footnote 2 In fact, as many political observers argue, election denialism became a founding myth of MORENA and the lopezobradorista movement, eventually playing a central role in Lopez Obrador’s rhetoric to mobilize his voters (Bruhn Reference Bruhn and Mudde2012; Castro Cornejo, Beltrán, and Ley Reference Castro Cornejo and Ley2020; see also Espino Reference Espino2021). While the country experienced a long history of systematic electoral fraud during the PRI’s hegemonic era (1929–2000), the IFE was a product of the 1996 electoral reform in which the PRI and opposition parties—the center-right PAN and the center-left PRD—created an independent electoral institution to organize national elections. Since then, Mexican elections are widely recognized as free and fair (Eisenstadt Reference Eisenstadt2007). Until 2006, the IFE was widely trusted by both winners and losers of the elections (Moreno Reference Moreno2022). It was not until the 2006 postelection crisis that a partisan bias emerged. After that election, on average, PAN and PRI partisans displayed high levels of trust while AMLO voters (PRD partisans between 2006–2015 and MORENA partisans beginning in 2015) reported low levels of trust. This partisan gap began to fade after 2018, when AMLO finally won the presidency on his third attempt. By 2021, the gap between losers and winners’ trust of the IFE shrank considerably: between 60 percent and 70 percent of the Mexican public now trust the National Electoral Institute (Instituto Nacional Electoral, the successor of the IFE).

As Bruhn (Reference Bruhn and Mudde2012) argues, the 2006 presidential election exposed that Mexico remained an unconsolidated democracy. López Obrador denounced the results as fraudulent without proof of irregularities, organized massive protests, refused to accept the outcome of the election, and declared himself the “legitimate President of Mexico.” As Bruhn (Reference Bruhn and Mudde2012) argues, the rules of democratic competition were not universally accepted, adhered to, and defended by all actors. Since then, López Obrador has consistently claimed that a political mafia in Mexico “stole” the presidency from him and “kidnapped” the country, particularly during his 2012 and 2018 presidential campaigns. While other studies have focused on the perceptions of a corrupt elite in Mexico and their implications on voters’ electoral behavior (Castro Cornejo, Ley, and Beltrán Reference Castro Cornejo and Ley2020), the following sections explore why some voters believe that there was fraud in 2006 even when that belief contradicts the best available evidence and even though more than fifteen years have passed since the election.

This perception, in fact, remains a strong grievance for a significant proportion of Mexican voters. According to an original survey conducted during the 2018 presidential campaign, 24 percent of the electorate believe that it is “very true” and 24 percent of voters consider it “somewhat true” that election fraud took place (question wording: “How true is it that there was fraud in the 2006 presidential election when Felipe Calderón won against López Obrador? Absolutely true, somewhat true, somewhat false, absolutely false”). In response to an open-ended question about how the election was stolen in the 2006 presidential election, 18 percent of voters reported that the votes were incorrectly counted or that the electoral counting system was manipulated, 15 percent reported that ballots were manipulated, another 15 percent that ballots were stolen, 14 percent cited vote buying, and 5 percent declared that the IFE was “bribed” (29 percent did not know). These open-ended answers mirror the accusations that López Obrador’s team raised against the IFE back in 2006 (Aparicio Reference Aparicio2009; Ugues Reference Ugues2010). Like other political events that have been studied in the literature—such as birtherism (Jardina and Traugott Reference Jardina and Traugott2019) or the existence of weapons of mass destruction in Iraq (Nyhan and Reifler Reference Nyhan and Reifler2010)—this perception of a fraudulent election has proven resilient and remained a polarized topic of public debate, even after the 2018 presidential election, when López Obrador finally won the presidency.

Why do some voters believe in events that contradict the best available evidence?

Past research on electoral integrity has studied the general public’s attitudes toward electoral fraud by focusing on institutional and individual variables. For example, administrative performance (Bowler et al. Reference Bowler, Brunell, Donovan and Gronke2015) and the independence of the electoral management body, as well as contextual factors such as countries’ levels of democracy and corruption (Birch Reference Birch2010), explain variations in perceptions of electoral integrity. Moreover, a level playing field for all contestants increases citizens’ confidence and supports the idea that institutions follow the rules (Birch Reference Birch2008). A second body of work considers perceptions of electoral fraud as a product of individual-level variables such as race (Wilson and King-Meadows Reference Wilson and King-Meadows2016), sociodemographic characteristics (Norris Reference Norris1999), political sophistication (Karp, Nai, and Norris Reference Karp, Nai and Norris2018), citizens’ direct experience with elections (Atkeson and Saunders Reference Atkeson and Saunders2007), their interaction with poll workers (Cantú and García Ponce Reference Cantú and García-Ponce2015), and underlying political predispositions Edelson et al. Reference Edelson, Alduncin, Krewson, Sieja and Uscinski2017) such as partisanship (Kernell and Mullinix Reference Kernell and Mullinix2019)—which plays a prominent role in the literature. For instance, depending on the elections’ results, partisanship adjusts voters’ confidence in the electoral procedures. Partisan identification has an amplifying effect for winners and losers (Anderson et al. Reference Anderson, Blais, Bowler, Donovan and Listhaug2005), whereby an electoral defeat diminishes the legitimacy of the electoral process among party supporters (e.g., on Mexico, see Cantú and Ponce Reference Cantú and García-Ponce2015; Ugues and Medina Vidal Reference Ugues and Vidal2015; Monsiváis Reference Monsiváis-Carrillo2023).

The literature on the winner-loser gap, or the “sore loser” hypothesis, argues that voters who support a losing party or candidate differ from those who support a winning party or candidate: electoral losers hold more negative opinions about the elections’ integrity (Anderson et al. Reference Anderson, Blais, Bowler, Donovan and Listhaug2005; Hernandez Huerta and Cantú Reference Hernández-Huerta and Cantú2022) and think there has been manipulation of the results (Sances and Stewart Reference Sances and Stewart III2015). However, while we know that the winner-loser gap shapes postelection attitudes in both long-standing democracies and young democracies (Carreras and İrepoğlu Reference Carreras and İrepoğlu2013; Maldonado and Seligson Reference Maldonado, Seligson, Norris, Frank and Martinez i Coma2014; Cantú and Ponce, Reference Cantú and García-Ponce2015; Ugues Reference Ugues2010), comparative studies on election fraud, as Kernell and Mullinix (Reference Kernell and Mullinix2019) argue, rarely integrate theories of preference formation. It is important to analyze how winners and losers systematically differ in their postelection attitudes. It is also necessary to understand the factors that shape those perceptions, particularly in a context—young democracies—where it has not been systematically studied. While there is evidence that motivated reasoning (Kernell and Mullinix Reference Kernell and Mullinix2019) shapes voters’ interpretations of election integrity and vote counting in long-standing democracies like the United States, we have less evidence of similar behaviors in Latin America and Mexico in particular.

Motivated reasoning theory argues that citizens are often determined to defend their prior beliefs and seek out and accept information consistent with them (Kunda Reference Kunda1990). In politics, voters are motivated to interpret information through a partisan lens (Bartels Reference Bartels2002; Zaller Reference Zaller1992), which makes them reject information that goes against their long-standing beliefs (Taber and Lodge Reference Taber and Lodge2006). While it is possible for voters to update their beliefs in a Bayesian way and carefully revise their opinions as they consume more information (Gerber and Green Reference Gerber and Green1999), this research argues that voters are particularly likely to see political events through a partisan lens in polarized settings after highly contested elections (Bartels Reference Bartels2002). This means that voters do update their beliefs, but the process is biased by partisan motivations: voters protect their in-group by choosing interpretations that rationalize their partisan beliefs, failing to consider that their partisan team might have lost the elections. Moreover, partisan-motivated reasoning affects voters’ interpretations of factual information (Gaines et al. Reference Gaines, Kuklinski, Quirk, Peyton and Verkuilen2007), altering voters’ perceptions of election integrity and, consequently, making them more prone to view the elections as fraudulent. This behavior has no instrumental benefits; rather, it is based on expressive benefits rooted in partisanship.

The literature on opinion formation has analyzed different episodes of political controversy viewed through a partisan lens. For example, Gaines and colleagues (Reference Gaines, Kuklinski, Quirk, Peyton and Verkuilen2007) study the Iraq War and show that both Democrats and Republicans updated their factual beliefs as conditions changed but interpreted the same facts quite differently. The literature has also analyzed perceptions that have persisted among the American public throughout the years, such as those about climate change (Hart and Nisbet Reference Hart and Nisbet2012) and birtherism (Hartman and Newmark Reference Hartman and Newmark2012) among others. For instance, survey researchers, beginning in 2008 and as recently as 2017, have provided evidence that a significant proportion of the American public believes that former president Barack Obama was born outside the United States and was raised as a Muslim (Jardina and Traugott Reference Jardina and Traugott2019). As Jardina and Traugott (Reference Jardina and Traugott2019) highlight, birtherism perceptions have persisted despite news media outlets’ efforts to correct the record and even after the release of two versions of Obama’s birth certificate. Like other pieces of misinformation (e.g., on healthcare reform, see Berinsky Reference Berinsky2015; on weapons of mass destruction, see Nyhan and Reifler Reference Nyhan and Reifler2010), such perceptions remain in the public debate for years and persist despite a lack of evidence.

Regardless of the specific issue, these studies show that facts or events do not speak for themselves; people must still interpret them (Gaines et al. Reference Gaines, Kuklinski, Quirk, Peyton and Verkuilen2007; Castro Cornejo Reference Castro Cornejo2023a), leading voters to interpret those facts and their implications differently. When directional motivations are more salient (e.g., accusations of election fraud in a polarized party system like Mexico), respondents are likely to process information in a manner that is consistent with their partisan preference rather than maximizing accuracy (Flynn, Nyhan and Reifler Reference Flynn, Nyhan and Reifler2017; Taber and Lodge Reference Taber and Lodge2006). Consistent with motivated reasoning theory, these voters do not judge the integrity of the election based on available evidence but choose partisan interpretations to explain the outcome of the election. Thus, the main hypothesis of this study, on partisan motivated reasoning, is the following:

Hypothesis 1: Voters are more likely to believe there is electoral fraud if such perception is consistent with their party identification.

To understand why voters believe in misperceptions, this research argues that some individual-level factors—affective polarization, information levels, and conspiratorial thinking—can exacerbate or diminish voters’ tendency to engage in motivated reasoning, for example, making them more likely to believe in electoral fraud even if it contradicts the best available evidence. If partisanship is important, affective polarization would be even more consequential given the in-groups (MORENA) and out-groups (the PAN and the PRI) that have recently formed in Mexico (e.g., lopezobradorismo or MORENA versus the “PRIAN” as López Obrador commonly refers to both parties) to explain political events (Castro Cornejo Reference Castro Cornejo2023b). Affective polarization is the tendency to view copartisans positively and opposing partisans negatively (Druckman and Levendusky Reference Druckman and Levendusky2019; Iyengar, Sood, and Lelkes Reference Iyengar, Sood and Lelkes2012), which makes ordinary partisans dislike and distrust one another and treat each other with prejudice (Iyengar and Westwood Reference Iyengar and Westwood2015). This means that polarization is not primarily rooted in ideology but identity (Iyengar, Sood, and Lelkes Reference Iyengar, Sood and Lelkes2012; Mason Reference Mason2018). Given polarized partisans’ tendency to develop in-group and out-group identity (Tajfel Reference Tajfel1981), they resort to motivated reasoning to view their party as acting with good intentions ( Edelson et al. Reference Edelson, Alduncin, Krewson, Sieja and Uscinski2017; Lodge and Taber Reference Lodge and Taber2013) and others as acting immorally and engaging in fraudulent behavior (Miller, Saunders, and Farhart Reference Miller, Saunders and Farhart2016). In other words, the more polarized partisans’ affective responses toward opposing parties are, the more influence these negative feelings are likely to have on their perceptions, which can promote the endorsement of inaccurate beliefs, causing them to ignore or misinterpret relevant evidence.

Affective polarization increasingly influences voters’ behavior and attitude formation in old and new democracies. For instance, the 2016 US presidential election represents an example of strong affective polarization rooted on identity (Mutz Reference Mutz2018). Donald Trump activated latent grievances in the electorate, particularly based on sexism (Valentino, Wayne, and Oceno Reference Valentino2018), racial resentment (Sides, Tesler, and Vavreck Reference Sides, Tesler and Vavreck2019), and white identity (Jardina Reference Jardina and Traugott2019). In fact, animus toward the Democratic-leaning social groups strongly predicted support for Trump’s candidacy (Mason, Wronski, and Kane Reference Mason, Wronski and Kane2021). In Brazil, Jair Bolsonaro’s victory was rooted in strong sentiment against the Workers’ Party (i.e., anti-petismo; Samuels and Zucco Reference Samuels and Zucco2018) as well as disenchantment with political institutions amid corruption scandals involving major parties. Bolsonaro was able to mobilize anti–Workers’ Party voters (Amaral Reference Amaral2020) by relying on a radical conservative rhetoric (Renno Reference Renno2020). In Mexico, Andrés Manuel López Obrador’s historic victory in 2018 was an outcome of a strong rejection of the country’s major parties, the PAN and the PRI, which were increasingly perceived as the same political alternative (Castro Cornejo Reference Castro Cornejo2023b).

Conspiratorial thinking can lead voters to engage in motivated reasoning, as some voters not only reject relevant evidence but also believe that pieces of evidence are concealed. Conspiratorial thinking refers to the belief that powerful people’s secretive and malevolent actions explain certain events (Vermeule and Sunstein Reference Vermeule and Sunstein2009). Voters who engage in conspiratorial thinking are more likely to believe unsubstantiated accusations that small groups are working in secret for their own benefit and against the common good (Uscinski and Parent Reference Uscinski and Parent2014). These voters are particularly resistant to information that challenges these misleading narratives (Berinsky Reference Berinsky2015; Nyhan and Reifler Reference Nyhan and Reifler2010; Uscinski and Parent Reference Uscinski and Parent2014). As Edelson et al. (Reference Edelson, Alduncin, Krewson, Sieja and Uscinski2017) argue, perceptions of election fraud are a good example of a belief that can be exacerbated by conspiratorial thinking. In the end, a conspiracy theory is not necessarily “wrong.” It is still a theory and requires evidence to either support or reject. However, conspiracy theories are troubling because, more often than not, they interpret the lack of confirming evidence as support. Since evidence of electoral fraud is normally hidden and fraud is typically accomplished by the machinations of powerful groups, the lack of evidence demonstrates the fraud—it shows just how widespread and concealed it really is (Edelson et al. Reference Edelson, Alduncin, Krewson, Sieja and Uscinski2017). This means that voters with strong predispositions to conspiratorial thinking are more likely to view elections with skepticism, expect fraud, and develop explanations for the results they are unhappy with.

This is particularly important when it comes to the 2006 presidential election in Mexico. The accusation of fraud in that election, in fact, fits the definition of a conspiracy theory. For example, after Lopez Obrador challenged the election results, the Mexican Electoral Tribunal ordered a partial recount of the votes (in precincts more likely to report mistakes) instead of a complete recount across the entire country—as López Obrador’s team demanded. While the partial recount did not find any systematic bias against López Obrador—in fact, Calderon’s margin of victory slightly increased—this decision prompted some accusations from López Obrador’s team that the tribunal was hiding the evidence of the fraud. In this scenario, consistent with the literature on conspiracy theories (Enders et al. Reference Enders, Uscinski, Seelig, Klofstad, Wuchty, Funchion and Stoler2021), losers argued that the corrupt elites and electoral institutions worked together in secret to violate electoral procedures for their own benefit.

Levels of information are also expected to exacerbate motivated reasoning. From a normative perspective, we expect that voters with higher levels of information will be less likely to believe in rumors without solid evidence (Bolsen, Druckman, and Lomax Cook Reference Bolsen, Druckman and Lomax Cook2014). For instance, some studies have found that trust in elections depends on the degree to which voters have political knowledge (Norris, Garnett, and Gromping Reference Norris, Garnett and Gromping2020). Nevertheless, studies on misinformation challenge the idea of higher levels of knowledge as a panacea. In fact, they have found that higher levels of information exacerbate motivated reasoning (Miller, Saunders, and Farhart Reference Miller, Saunders and Farhart2016), because highly informed voters tend to be more partisan and are highly motivated to protect their beliefs, which makes them more likely to engage in motivated reasoning (Lodge and Taber Reference Lodge and Taber2013; Miller, Saunders, and Farhart Reference Miller, Saunders and Farhart2016). As a result, this study proposes an additional set of hypotheses:

Hypothesis 2: Highly polarized voters are more likely to believe in electoral fraud.

Hypothesis 2b: Voters with high levels of conspiratorial thinking are more likely to believe in electoral fraud.

Hypothesis 2c: More informed voters are less likely to believe in electoral fraud.

Finally, the last hypothesis studies the persistence of perceptions, which is particularly relevant when studying election denialism. If motivated reasoning affects voters’ updating of their beliefs, voters are likely to believe in events that contradict the best available evidence and resist efforts to reduce voters’ belief inaccuracy, such as fact-checking, even if this event happened a long time ago. While some literature has found that voters’ can update their beliefs as they engage with new information (Wood and Porter, Reference Wood and Porter2018), most studies suggest that fact-checking fails to make respondents’ beliefs more accurate (Garrett, Nisbet, and Lynch Reference Garrett, Nisbet and Lynch2013; Nyhan, Reifler, and Ubel Reference Nyhan, Reifler and Ubel2013) and, in some cases, even strengthens misperceptions (“backfire effect”; Nyhan and Reifler Reference Nyhan and Reifler2010). For example, Nyhan and Reifler’s corrections on the belief that Iraq had weapons of mass destruction failed to reduce misperceptions and even strengthened such views among congenial ideological groups. Based on this discussion, the last hypothesis is the following:

Hypothesis 3: MORENA partisans will resist fact-checking about election fraud

The next section applies motivated-reasoning theory to the results of the 2006 presidential election in Mexico, a political event that continues to activate important grievances among voters and political elites to this day and one that played a key role in López Obrador’s rhetorical strategy during the 2018 election.

Perceptions of fraud in 2006 and the “corrupt elite” during the 2018 presidential election

As previously mentioned, the literature on public opinion highlights that attitudes are not formed in a vacuum: they reflect a marriage between political predispositions and political elites’ communication (Zaller Reference Zaller1992; Druckman Reference Druckman2014). In Mexico’s case, López Obrador’s rhetoric has consistently divided Mexican society between the “people” and a corrupt elite that stole the presidency from him and impoverished Mexico with neoliberal policies and rampant corruption (Bruhn Reference Bruhn and Mudde2012). This accusation began during the postelection crisis in 2006 and remained part of his political rhetoric for the next electoral cycles. In 2015, relations between López Obrador and his party deteriorated markedly after the PRD decided to join the Pact for Mexico (Pacto por México)—with the PAN and the PRI—which sought to create an agreement between political forces to approve various structural reforms in Congress. López Obrador denounced the PRD for betraying its members by joining the same “political mafia” as the “PRIAN,” the term he uses colloquially to conflate the PRI and the PAN. López Obrador resigned from the PRD and cofounded a new party—the National Regeneration Movement, or MORENA. Accusations of electoral fraud in 2006 served as a foundational myth of the new party. In other words, López Obrador has been communicating a clear and consistent message for many years: a corrupt elite stole the presidency from him in 2006.

In addition to clear elite cues, partisanship is also likely to bias voters’ perceptions. Recent literature highlights that partisanship is fairly strong among the Mexican electorate (Castro Cornejo Reference Castro Cornejo2019)—about two-thirds of the electorate self-identify with a political party since the country’s transition to democracy, according to the Mexican Election Study (Beltrán, Ley, and Castro Cornejo Reference Beltrán, Ley and Castro Cornejo2020). The emergence of a new major party, however, transformed the partisan attachments of an important part of the electorate. While partisan loyalties were fairly stable between 2000 and 2015, when priístas formed the largest partisan group, and panistas and perredistas followed in their wake, in 2018, morenistas became the largest partisan group in the country. According to the 2018 Mexican Election Study, 22 percent of the electorate self-identified with MORENA, 17 percent with the PAN, 14 percent with the PRI, 4 percent with the PRD (López Obrador’s former party)—which lost a significant part of its base after MORENA’s appearance in 2015—and 6 percent with smaller parties. In addition, 33 percent self-identified as “independent”—a proportion of voters that has remained fairly consistent since previous elections. In other words, the decay of the Mexican party system has not led to the demise of partisanship—as was the case in other Latin American countries such as Venezuela and Perú (Morgan Reference Morgan2011; Seawright Reference Seawright2012)—but to the development of new partisan loyalties, particularly to MORENA. While identification with MORENA as a party is still evolving (Aruguete et al. Reference Aruguete, Calvo, Cantú, Ley, Scartascini and Ventura2021), recent studies have found that MORENA partisanship moderates voters’ attitudes and behavior, such as their retrospective evaluations of the economy, security, corruption, belief in a corrupt elite, and turnout (Castro Cornejo, Ley, and Beltrán Reference Castro Cornejo and Ley2020). This partisan bias effect is independent of voters’ opinion of López Obrador (Castro Cornejo Reference Castro Cornejo2023a).

While the Mexican electorate has experienced some shifts in party identification, the same data suggests that affective polarization has increased in Mexico. Following survey strategies in American politics (Lelkes and Westwood Reference Lelkes and Westwood2017), Figure 1 reports the feeling thermometer that rates respondents’ opinion towards major parties in Mexico. Affective polarization—the difference between the in-group’s average rating and the out-group’s average rating (on a 0–10 scale)—has increased. Between 2000 and 2015, the average rating of in-groups and out-groups were quite stable. However, after 2015, there is a very important increase in out-party animus. The average rating of the opposing parties was 4.1 in 2000 and had decreased to 2.4 by 2018, almost 2 points on the 0–10 scale (Figure 1). This context makes partisans who self-identify with López Obrador’s MORENA more likely to engage in motivated reasoning, particularly on a highly polarized issue such as the outcome of the 2006 presidential election.

Figure 1. Affective Polarization in Mexico (2000–2018).

Note: Average ratings on the feeling thermometer (0 = very bad, and 10 = very good) among voters who self-identify with a political party.

Source: Mexican Election Study 2000–2018 (Beltrán, Ley, and Castro Cornejo Reference Beltrán, Ley and Castro Cornejo2020).

Observational data

This section relies on an original telephone survey and the 2021 Mexican Election Study. The telephone survey is nationally representative and was conducted by the polling firm BGC Beltrán y Asocs between April 3 and 11, 2018, the first month of the 2018 presidential campaign in Mexico (N = 1,000). The 2021 Mexican Election Study is a nationally representative survey and part of the Comparative Study of Electoral Systems (CSES). It was conducted as a face-to-face survey two weeks after the midterm election (N = 1,800).

To identify the proportion of voters who believe that the 2006 presidential election was fraudulent, both studies rely on the following survey question: “How true is it that there was fraud in the 2006 presidential election when Felipe Calderón won against López Obrador? Absolutely true, somewhat true, somewhat false, absolutely false.” To facilitate interpretation, the variable is recoded from 0 to 1, and the average answer was 0.59 in the telephone survey (13 percent didn’t know or didn’t answer) and 0.63 in the 2021 Mexican Election Study (20 percent didn’t know or didn’t answer). It is important to highlight that in the 2021 survey, this question was included for only half the sample. Table A2 in the appendix (see supplementary material) reports descriptive statistics of each variable included in the models.

To measure partisanship, the study relies on the following question: “Regardless of the party you normally vote for, do you consider yourself panista, priísta, perredista, a supportor of MORENA, or some other party?” To estimate affective polarization, both studies follow survey strategies in American politics (Druckman and Levendusky 2019) and rely on the feeling thermometers: “On a 0–10 scale, where 0 means having a very bad opinion and 10 means having a very good opinion, what is your opinion of the PRI/PAN/MORENA?” From these questions, a new variable estimates the difference between respondents’ favorability of MORENA (in-group attitudes) and the mean favorability of the PAN and the PRI (out-group attitudes). Footnote 3

$${\rm{AFFECTIVE}}\;{\rm{POLARIZATION}} = {\rm{MORENA\!{\hbox '}s}}\;{\rm{FT}} - ({\rm{PAN{\hbox '}s}}\;{\rm{FT}} + {\rm{PRI{\hbox '}s}}\;{\rm{FT}}){\rm{/2}}.$$

This new variable ranges from –10 to 10. To facilitate interpretation the variable has been recoded from 0 to 1. Both surveys measure voters’ levels of political information by looking at their ability to correctly answer questions about factual information. It uses an additive index based on three general knowledge questions about the Mexican political system: “What are the chambers of Mexico’s Congress? What is the term length of a member of Congress? What is the name of the current governor of your state?” This question was also recoded from 0 to 1 to facilitate interpretation.

As previously discussed, conspiratorial thinking increases the chances that a voter will believe in election fraud. To measure respondents’ susceptibility to conspiracy theories, the telephone survey (2018) relies on an additional additive index based on the following two questions: “In your opinion, how true is it that the Mexican government knew that the 2017 earthquakes were going to happen and refused to warn the citizens? How true is it that worldwide secret conspiracy groups control events such as world wars? Absolutely true, somewhat true, somewhat false, absolutely false.” In terms of the 2021 Mexican Election Study (Beltrán, Castro Cornejo, and Ley Reference Beltrán, Castro Cornejo and Ley2023), the analysis relies on the following question: “As far as you know, is it true or false that the coronavirus exists and it has caused the thousands of deaths that have been registered in Mexico during the present pandemic?” These questions are not related to respondents’ political behavior in order to avoid close conceptual or empirical connection to partisanship and voting behavior or to the dependent variable of this study. Footnote 4

The upcoming models also include control variables to make sure that third variables are not driving the results. The models control for respondents’ opinion about Andrés Manuel López Obrador to ensure that such attitudes are not driving their belief in electoral fraud in 2006. While we can expect voters’ opinion of López Obrador to be correlated with their belief in the alleged fraud, including the feeling thermometers makes it possible to identify whether partisanship and affective polarization have an independent effect on this study’s dependent variable: “On a 0–10 scale, where 0 means having a very bad opinion and 10 means having a very good opinion, what is your opinion of Andrés Manuel López Obrador.”

The following models also control for voters’ distrust of Mexican elections. As previously discussed, given the history of electoral fraud during the PRI hegemonic regime before the country’s democratization, it is expected that an important proportion of voters are skeptical about elections—even though elections are recognized to have been free and fair when organized by the IFE. For these reasons, the following questions seek to measure such beliefs. The telephone survey (2018) relies on the question “Talking about the presidential elections in Mexico since the year 2000, on a scale from 0 to 10, where 0 means that electoral fraud is not committed and 10 that electoral fraud is committed in Mexico, where would you place the presidential elections in Mexico?” In turn, the Mexican Election Study (2021) relies on the following statement: “In some countries, people think their elections are fair. In other countries, people think that their elections are not fair. Using the following 1–5 scale, where 1 means that the elections were not fair and 5 means that the elections were fair, where would you place the last elections?” These two variables were also recoded to range between 0 and 1 to ease interpretation.

The models also control for left-right ideology (“In politics, people sometimes talk of left and right. Using a scale from 0 to 10 where 0 means the left and 10 means the right, where would you place yourself?”) to ensure that ideology is not driving the results instead of partisanship. Finally, the models also control for voters’ sociodemographic characteristics, such as age, gender, and levels of education.

Table 1 displays the results of the ordinary-least-squares models. Footnote 5 Their dependent variable is the respondents’ probability to engage in election denialism. In both the 2018 survey and the 2021 Mexican Election Study, consistent with Hypothesis 1, election denialism varies across partisan groups (Table 1). MORENA partisans are the most likely to believe in electoral fraud in 2006 (p < 0.01, models 1 and 3). To better understand the substantive impact of partisanship, Figure 2 presents the probabilities of each partisan group. For example, in 2018, while PAN partisans had a 0.37 chance of believing that there was election fraud, MORENA partisans had a probability of 0.82, a 45-percentage-point difference. This comparison is relevant because the partisan groups were the two leading parties during the 2006 presidential election and report the largest differences. The few respondents who self-identified in 2018 with the PRD—López Obrador’s former party (2 percent of the sample)—report a probability of 0.66, which suggests that they partially believe the allegations of fraud even though they do not currently identify with López Obrador’s party. In fact, for the 2018 presidential election, the PRD nominated the PAN presidential candidate as part of the PAN-PRD electoral coalition. Finally, PRI partisans report a probability of 0.47 and independents of 0.60. Interestingly, independent voters’ perceptions are closer to those from MORENA than those from the PAN and PRI. This result is probably an outcome of the strong support that López Obrador enjoyed among independents in 2018. Unlike during the previous two presidential elections in which López Obrador lost the independent vote, in 2018 he finally won a strong majority of independents (Aparicio and Castro Cornejo Reference Aparicio and Cornejo2020). In 2021, the differences are not as large, but the general results are consistent with 2018: MORENA supporters are the partisan group most likely to believe in election fraud. Independents, once again, have an intermediate position between MORENA partisans and out-partisans. In this survey, since less than 1 percent of the sample identified as PRD partisans, the models do not report results for this partisan group.

Table 1. OLS models: Probability of believing that there was electoral fraud in 2006

Note: Standard error in parentheses. Values range from 0 = “not true at all” to 1 = “very true.”

***p < 0.01. **p < 0.05. *p < 0.1.

Figure 2. OLS models on the probability of believing that there was electoral fraud in 2006.

Note: values range from 0 = “not true at all” to 1 = “very true.”

Models 2 and 4 estimate the probability of believing in the 2006 fraud across levels of affective polarization, conspiratorial thinking, and political information in order to understand the conditions under which voters are more likely to engage in election denialism—while controlling for opinions of Andrés Manuel López Obrador and voters’ distrust of elections in Mexico (the results also hold when distrust is excluded from the models; Table A6 in the appendix; see supplementary material). Affective polarization is a strong predictor of believing in events that contradict the best available evidence. Moreover, even though the operationalization of conspiratorial thinking differs in both studies, voters with higher levels of conspiratorial thinking are more likely to engage in election denialism (p < 0.01). Political information is loosely associated with such beliefs (p < 0.10) but only in one of the surveys, and the sign is the opposite direction of the expected one. This is, in fact, consistent with the recent literature on misinformation, which finds that highly informed voters tend to have strong political predispositions and are highly motivated to protect their beliefs. It was expected that respondents’ opinion of Andrés Manuel López Obrador would have an important association with election denialism, but it is statistically significant only in 2018, not in 2021. Distrust in elections, in turn, is statistically significant in both years; in other words, voters who report a general distrust in Mexican elections are more likely to believe in the 2006 election fraud.

Figures 3 and 4 show that only two variables have a consistently strong substantive impact on the belief that the presidential election was rigged: affective polarization and voters’ levels of conspiratorial thinking. For example, in 2018 the difference between the lowest value of affective polarization—that is, voters who have a very positive opinion of the PRI and the PAN and a very negative opinion of MORENA—and the highest value—voters who have a very positive opinion of MORENA and a very bad opinion of the PRI and the PAN—is 42 percentage points (from 0.34 to 0.76). The difference in 2021 is about 30 percentage points. In the case of the voters’ levels of conspiratorial thinking, in 2018 the difference between the highest and the lowest values comes down to 18 percentage points (from 0.50 to 0.68); in 2021 the difference is about 12 percentage points. In other words, affective polarization is the variable that best explains why some voters believe in allegations that are inconsistent with the best available evidence (Hypothesis 2a)—followed by voters’ conspiratorial thinking (Hypothesis 2b)—even when controlling for their opinion of López Obrador, ideology, and socioeconomic variables. Distrust in elections, while statistically significant in both years, does not report a strong substantive impact in 2021.

Figure 3. Probability of believing that there was electoral fraud in 2006 (2018 Survey).

Figure 4. Probability of believing that there was electoral fraud in 2006 (2021 Mexican Election Study).

Experimental data

A survey experiment was included in a second nationally representative telephone survey. The survey was conducted between June 26 and June 29, 2018, the week before the presidential election held on July 1. It was conducted by the polling firm BGC Beltrán y Asocs. It included a sample of 782 respondents randomly divided into two groups. Footnote 6 These groups are balanced across observed covariates (Table A7; see Supplementary material). The survey experiment followed an indirect strategy to fact-check the belief that there was fraud in the 2006 presidential election. The survey randomly assigned a question asking whether respondents were aware of a statement made by the president of the INE (the successor of the IFE). Respondents who were part of the control group were not asked that question. It reads as follows: “Did you know that the president of the National Election Institute, Lorenzo Córdova, recently declared that in Mexico there is no possibility of electoral fraud during presidential elections and that there was no evidence that votes were improperly counted in the 2006 presidential election?”

After reading the vignette, respondents were asked the following question, which constitutes the dependent variable of this section: “On a 0–10 scale, where 0 means that there was no fraud in the 2006 presidential election and 10 that there definitely was electoral fraud, how true is it that there was electoral fraud in the 2006 presidential election when Felipe Calderón defeated López Obrador?” (Table A2 reports descriptive statistics; see supplementary material). Random assignment to the treatment condition guarantees that, on average, respondents exposed to treatment and control groups will be identical in both observable and unobservable characteristics. Any systematic difference between the two groups’ answers to the dependent variable question provides an estimate of how fact-checking the accusation that there was an electoral fraud affects voters’ attitudes. It is worth mentioning that while a potential treatment highlighting that a MORENA politician declaring that there was no fraud in that election would have been stronger (or even López Obrador declaring there was no fraud), it would have lacked external validity since most politicians that belong to López Obrador’s party endorse the misperception that there was fraud. To enhance external validity, INE was chosen as a source of information since it in charge of organizing elections in Mexico.

Table 2 displays the results of the OLS models, in which the dependent variable is the respondent’s belief that electoral fraud took place in the 2006 presidential election. Consistent with Hypothesis 1, the likelihood of engaging in election denialism varies across partisan groups. MORENA partisans are the most likely to believe that there was election fraud compared with PAN, PRI, and PRD partisans. Moreover, consistent with Hypothesis 3, a motivated-reasoning effect took place: none of partisan groups updated beliefs, and voters resisted the information about the 2006 presidential election (Figure 5). Only among PRD partisans was there some belief updating—about 3.6 points on the 0–10 scale—but it is not statistically significant under conventional thresholds (p > 0.10), given the few observations that are part of this partisan group (less than 2 percent of the sample self-identified as a PRD partisan). Footnote 7

Table 2. OLS models: Probability of believing that there was electoral fraud in 2006

Note: Standard errors in parentheses, DV = measured on a 0–10 scale; Party ID (Reference Category = MORENA)

***p < 0.01. **p < 0.05. *p < 0.1.

Figure 5. Probability of believing that there was electoral fraud in 2006.

Note: Values range from 0 = “not true at all” to 10 = “very true.”

Consistent with studies in advanced industrial democracies, the survey experiment highlights that fact-checking is sometimes unable to shift voter perceptions. Particularly for highly polarizing issues like the belief in election fraud in the 2006 election, a partisan bias is activated to resist such efforts. As previously discussed, López Obrador voters—PRD partisans after the 2006 presidential election and MORENA partisans after 2015—have reported low levels of trust in the IFE after the 2006 postelection crisis (Figure A1 in the appendix), making them less likely to be responsive to information produced by such source. Therefore, it was a difficult case of belief updating. The results among PAN and PRI voters are particularly interesting. Even though they report high levels of trust in the IFE, they also did not update their beliefs. Respondents identifying with MORENA, PAN, and PRI resisted any kind of belief updating on this topic, one of the most polarizing issues in Mexico.

Discussion

This study makes an important contribution to the literature on election fraud and opinion formation by accounting for the ways that individuals make judgments. As previously discussed, while it is important to analyze the systematic differences between winners’ and losers’ postelection attitudes, this study has sought to understand the factors that shape these attitudes, particularly in a polarized young democracy. This study has also found that voters with high levels of affective polarization and conspiratorial thinking are the most likely to choose interpretations that rationalize their partisan priors. Although it is a young democracy, Mexico is a polarized country in which voters have strong incentives to see events through a partisan lens.

While this research has focused on election fraud in Mexico, its findings are relevant to other contexts in Latin America and beyond. We should observe partisan perceptual bias whenever a fact or an event has partisan relevance (Jerit and Barabas Reference Jerit and Barabas2012), as learning causes partisans to process information differently. As recent studies in partisanship find (Lupu Reference Lupu2015; Castro Cornejo Reference Castro Cornejo2023a; Calvo and Ventura Reference Calvo and Ventura2021; Haime and Cantú Reference Haime and Cantú2022; Melendez Reference Meléndez2022), in many Latin American countries, partisanship and partisan cues are increasingly relevant to understand voters’ behavior and opinion formation. In Mexico, even after more than fifteen years, at least a fourth of the public strongly believe the false claim that López Obrador won in the 2006 presidential election. This has important implications for other countries. In the United States, even though there is no evidence of any election fraud in the 2020 presidential election, around 20 percent of the public “strongly agree” that the election was stolen from Donald Trump (40 percent among Republicans; Ipsos 2021). The results of this study strongly suggest that this misperception will continue to be widespread in the American electorate, particularly because partisan elites continue to endorse false claims (e.g., in the 2021 midterm election in the United States, 60 percent of Americans had an election denier on the ballot). As has been the case in Mexico, it is likely that such claims will continue to be a partisan grievance activated by Republican candidates to mobilize their electorate.

It is important to highlight that election denialism is not likely to emerge in every polarized and competitive election. In many cases, as in the 2019 election in Bolivia, getting at the truth is hard to determine; in other cases, claims of fraud are either easy to justify or easy to dismiss. Regardless, knowing why some winners believe that elections were fair (e.g., Venezuela or Nicaragua) or why some losers believe that an election was stolen (e.g., Bolsonaro voters in Brazil) is important for understanding democracy in the region. These beliefs can undermine the legitimacy of elections and institutions and erode the faith in democratic norms (Norris et al. Reference Norris, Garnett and Gromping2020; Albertson and Guiller Reference Albertson and Guiler2020).

An interesting question relates to the influence of the belief of the 2006 electoral fraud in voting behavior. While this article has focused on why voters believe in fraud despite the lack of evidence, such misperceptions are an important predictor of supporting MORENA (as reported in Table A8 in the appendix; see supplementary material), even when controlling for other relevant variables: partisanship, opinions of López Obrador, retrospective voting, incumbent’s feeling thermometer, among others. Given that election denialism has played an important role in López Obrador’s rhetoric to mobilize MORENA voters, future studies should analyze whether the belief in the 2006 electoral fraud has strengthened MORENA partisanship over time or constitutes a grievance that is activated during campaigns. In that sense, future studies should further analyze the nature of MORENA partisanship—which combines partisanship and a personalistic linkage. MORENA became the most important leftist party in Mexico after inheriting most of the PRD partisanship, Footnote 8 but it is also strongly linked to López Obrador, the most important leftist figure in Mexico since 2000. While the models reported here control for López Obrador’s feeling thermometer—and partisanship and affective polarization represent the strongest variables driving the perception of electoral fraud—future studies should disentangle the different components of MORENA’s partisanship.

Some features of election fraud and partisan bias not investigated here may turn out to be influential when examined in future studies. For example, future studies should consider adding stronger fact-checking treatments to understanding the conditions that encourage MORENA partisans to update their beliefs about the 2006 presidential elections, whether from opposition or friendly co-partisan sources of information (Botero et al. Reference Botero, Cornejo, Gamboa, Pavao and Nickerson2019). Likewise, it would be interesting to expand the analysis of the relationship between conspiratorial thinking and perceptions of election fraud. It is likely that some alleged type of fraud (e.g., “cybernetic” fraud, as the López Obrador campaign team claimed in 2006) is more likely to be believed by voters with higher levels of conspiratorial thinking than others.

Supplementary Material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/lar.2024.11

Footnotes

1 President Fox and the business sector actively campaigned in favor of the PAN’s presidential candidate, which could have clouded the perception about the fairness of the election and made the opportunistic allegation of fraud credible among López Obrador voters (Schedler Reference Schedler2007).

2 Table A1 in the appendix (see Supplementary Material) analyzes the partisan bias in nine issue attitudes, including sociocultural issues, economic issues, and the perception of fraud in the 2006 presidential election.

3 In Table A3 in the Appendix (see Supplementary Material) we include a different operationalization of affective polarization. We include López Obrador’s feeling thermometer instead of MORENA’s feeling thermometer. The results are almost the same. Appendix B includes models that reports affective polarization without averaging the PAN and the PRI but reporting each variable independently. The results are fairly similar.

4 The correlation between conspiratorial thinking and belief in the 2006 election fraud is very weak: 0.19 (2018) and 0.13 (2021).

5 Table A4 (see supplementary material) reports ordinal logistic models. The results do not differ substantially. Table A5 and Figure A2 in the appendix (see Supplementary material) report the same models but only among MORENA supporters.

6 A total of 316 respondents were recontacted from the sample of the first survey (as part of the second wave of a panel survey); 466 respondents were contacted for the first time.

7 Figure A3 in the appendix also reports the effect of the treatment condition across different levels of opinion of López Obrador. The results do not change.

8 In the same survey that this study is based on, around two thirds of MORENA partisans declared that they had previously self-identified with the PRD.

References

Albertson, Bethany, and Guiler, Kimberly. 2020. “Conspiracy Theories, Election Rigging, and Support for Democratic Norms.” Research & Politics 7 (3): 19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amaral, Oswaldo E. do. 2020. “The Victory of Jair Bolsonaro according to the Brazilian Electoral Study of 2018.” Brazilian Political Science Review 14 (1): 113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, Christopher J., Blais, Andre, Bowler, Shaun, Donovan, Todd, and Listhaug, Ola. 2005. Losers’ Consent: Elections and Democratic Legitimacy. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aparicio, Francisco Javier, and Cornejo, Rodrigo. 2020. “Elecciones 2018: Una coyuntura histórica en México.” Política y Gobierno 27 (2): 321.Google Scholar
Aparicio, Javier. 2009. “Análisis estadístico de la elección presidencial de 2006 ¿Fraude o errores aleatorios?Política y Gobierno 16 (2): 225243.Google Scholar
Aruguete, Natalia, Calvo, Ernesto, Cantú, Francisco, Ley, Sandra, Scartascini, Carlos, and Ventura, Tiago. 2021. “Partisan Cues and Perceived Risks: The Effect of Partisan Social Media Frames during the Covid-19 Crisis in Mexico.” Journal of Elections, Public Opinion and Parties 31: 8295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atkeson, Lonna Rae, and Saunders, Kyle L.. 2007. “The Effect of Election Administration on Voter Confidence: A Local Matter?” PS: Political Science & Politics 40 (4): 655660.Google Scholar
Bartels, Larry M. 2002. “Beyond the Running Tally: Partisan Bias in Political Perceptions.” Political Behavior 24: 117150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beltrán, Ulises, Castro Cornejo, Rodrigo, and Ley, Sandra. 2023. 2021 Mexican Election Study—Encuesta Nacional Electoral de México. Mexico City: Centro de Investigación y Docencia Económicas.Google Scholar
Beltrán, Ulises, Ley, Sandra, and Castro Cornejo, Rodrigo. 2020. 2018 Mexican Election Study—Estudio Nacional Electoral de México. Mexico City: Centro de Investigación y Docencia Económicas.Google Scholar
Berinsky, Adam. 2015. “Rumors and Health Care Reform: Experiments in Political Misinformation.” British Journal of Political Science 47 (2): 247252.Google Scholar
Birch, Sarah. 2008. “Electoral Institutions and Popular Confidence in Electoral Processes: A Cross-national Analysis.” Electoral Studies 27 (2): 305320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Birch, Sarah. 2010. “Perceptions of Electoral Fairness and Voter Turnout.” Comparative Political Studies 43 (12): 16011622.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolsen, Toby, Druckman, James N., and Lomax Cook, Fay. 2014. “The Influence of Partisan Motivated Reasoning on Public Opinion.” Political Behavior 36 (2): 235262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Botero, Sandra, Cornejo, Rodrigo Castro, Gamboa, Laura, Pavao, Nara, and Nickerson, David W.. 2019. “Under Friendly Fire: An Experiment on Partisan Press, Fragmented Opposition and Voting Behavior.” Electoral Studies, 60: 102044.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowler, Shaun, Brunell, Thomas, Donovan, Todd, and Gronke, Paul. 2015. “Election Administration and Perceptions of Fair Elections.” Electoral Studies 38: 19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruhn, Kathleen. 2012. “‘To Hell with Your Corrupt Institutions!’: AMLO and Populism in Mexico.” In Populism in Europe and the Americas: Threat or Corrective for Democracy?, edited by Mudde, Cas and Cristóbal Rovira Kaltwasser, 88112. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Calvo, Ernesto, and Ventura, Tiago. 2021. “Will I Get COVID-19? Partisanship, Social Media Frames, and Perceptions of Health Risk in Brazil.” Latin American Politics and Society 63 (1): 126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cantú, Francisco, and García-Ponce, Omar. 2015. “Partisan Losers’ Effects: Perceptions of Electoral Integrity in Mexico.” Electoral Studies 39 (1): 114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carreras, Miguel, and İrepoğlu, Yasemin 2013. “Trust in Elections, Vote Buying, and Turnout in Latin America”. Electoral Studies 32 (4): 609619.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castro Cornejo, Rodrigo. 2019. “Partisanship and Question Wording Effects: Experimental Evidence from Latin AmericaPublic Opinion Quarterly 83 (1): 2645.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castro Cornejo, Rodrigo. 2021. “Short- and Long-Term Partisanship: Campaign Effects and the Stability of Party Identification in Latin America.” Latin American Research Review 56 (2): 300317. https://doi.org/10.25222/larr.638.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castro Cornejo, Rodrigo. 2023a. “Same Scandal, Different Interpretations: Politics of Corruption, Anger, and Partisan Bias in Mexico.” Journal of Elections, Public Opinion and Parties 33 (3): 497518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castro Cornejo, Rodrigo. 2023b. “The AMLO Voter: Affective Polarization and the Rise of the Left in Mexico.” Journal of Politics in Latin America 15 (1): 96112. https://doi.org/10.1177/1866802X221147067.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castro Cornejo, Rodrigo, Ley, Sandra, and Ulises, Beltrán. 2020. “Anger, Partisanship, and the Activation of Populist Attitudes in Mexico.” Política y Gobierno 27 (2): 137.Google Scholar
Coppedge, Michael. 1997. Strong Parties and Lame Ducks: Presidential Partyarchy and Factionalism in Venezuela. Redwood City, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Druckman, James N. 2014. “Pathologies of Studying Public Opinion, Political Communication, and Democratic Responsiveness.” Political Communication 31 (3): 467492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Druckman, James N., and Levendusky, Matthew S. 2019. “What Do We Measure When We Measure Affective Polarization?” Public Opinion Quarterly 83 (1): 114122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Druckman, James N. 2012. “The Politics of Motivation.” Critical Review 24 (2): 199216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edelson, Jack, Alduncin, Alexander, Krewson, Christopher, Sieja, James, and Uscinski, Joseph. 2017. “The Effect of Conspiratorial Thinking and Motivated Reasoning on Belief in Election Fraud.” Political Research Quarterly 70 (4): 933946.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenstadt, Todd. 2007. “The Origins and Rationality of the ‘Legal versus Legitimate’ Dichotomy Invoked in Mexico’s 2006 Post-Electoral Conflict.” PS: Political Science & Politics 40 (1): 3943.Google Scholar
Ellsworth, Brian, and Viga, Rodrigo. 2022. “Bolsonaro Backers Call on Brazil Military to Intervene after Lula Victory” Reuters, November 2. https://www.reuters.com/world/americas/bolsonaro-backers-call-brazil-military-intervene-after-lula-victory-2022-11-02/.Google Scholar
Enders, Adam M., Uscinski, Joseph E., Seelig, Michelle I., Klofstad, Casey A., Wuchty, Stefan, Funchion, John … and Stoler, Justin. 2021. “The Relationship between Social Media Use and Beliefs in Conspiracy Theories and Misinformation.” Political Behavior 124.Google ScholarPubMed
Espino, Luis Antonio. 2021. López Obrador: El poder del discurso populista. Mexico City: Oceano/Turner.Google Scholar
Flynn, D. J., Nyhan, Brendan, and Reifler, Jason. 2017. “The Nature and Origins of Misperceptions: Understanding False and Unsupported Beliefs about Politics. Advances in Political Psychology 38 (S1): 127150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaines, Brian, Kuklinski, James, Quirk, Paul, Peyton, Buddy, and Verkuilen, Jay. 2007. “Same Facts, Different Interpretations: Partisan Motivation and Opinion on Iraq.” Journal of Politics 69 (4): 957974.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garrett, Kelly, Nisbet, Erik, and Lynch, Emily. 2013. “Undermining the Corrective Effects of Media-based Political Fact Checking? The Role of Contextual Cues and Naïve Theory.” Journal of Communication 63 (4): 617637.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerber, Alan, and Green, Donald. 1999. “Misperceptions about Perceptual Bias.” Annual Review of Political Science 2 (1): 189210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greene, Kenneth. 2011. “Campaign Persuasion and Nascent Partisanship in Mexico’s New Democracy.” American Journal of Political Science 55 (2): 398416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haime, Augustina, and Cantú, Francisco. 2022. “Negative Partisanship in Latin America.” Latin American Politics and Society 64 (1): 7292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hart, P. Sol, and Nisbet, Erik 2012. “Boomerang Effects in Science Communication: How Motivated Reasoning and Identity Cues Amplify Opinion Polarization about Climate Mitigation Policies.” Communication Research 39 (6): 701723.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hartman, Todd, and Newmark, Adam. 2012. “Motivated Reasoning, Political Sophistication, and Associations between President Obama and Islam.” PS: Political Science & Politics 45 (3): 449455.Google Scholar
Hernández-Huerta, Victor. 2020. “Disputed Elections in Presidential Democracies: Contexts of Electoral “Blackmail”.” Journal of Politics 82 (1): 89103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hernández-Huerta, Victor, and Cantú, Francisco. 2022. “Public Distrust in Disputed Elections: Evidence from Latin America.” British Journal of Political Science 52 (4): 19231930.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ipsos. 2021. “Majority of Republicans still Believe the 2020 Election Was Stolen from Donald Trump.” April 2. https://www.ipsos.com/en-us/news-polls/majority-republicans-still-believe-2020-election-was-stolen-donald-trump.Google Scholar
Iyengar, Shanto, and Krupenkin, Masha. 2018. “The Strengthening of Partisan Affect.” Political Psychology 39 (S1): 201218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iyengar, Shanto, Sood, Gaurav, and Lelkes, Yphtach. 2012. “Affect, Not Ideology: A Social Identity Perspective on Polarization. Public Opinion Quarterly 76 (3): 405431.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iyengar, Shanto, and Westwood, Shaun. 2015. “Fear and Loathing across Party Lines: New Evidence on Group Polarization. American Journal of Political Science 59 (3): 690707.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jardina, Ashley, and Traugott, Michael. 2019. “The Genesis of the Birther Rumor: Partisanship, Racial Attitudes, and Political Knowledge.” Journal of Race, Ethnicity and Politics 4 (1): 6080.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jerit, Jennifer, and Barabas, Jason. 2012. “Partisan Perceptual Bias and the Information Environment.” Journal of Politics 74 (3): 672684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karp, Jeffrey, Nai, Alessandra, and Norris, Pippa 2018. “Dial ‘F’ for Fraud: Explaining citizens Suspicions about Elections.” Electoral Studies 53: 1119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kernell, Georgia, and Mullinix, Kevin. 2019. “Winners, Losers, and Perceptions of Vote (Mis)Counting.” International Journal of Public Opinion Research 31 (1): 124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klesner, Joseph L. 2007. “The 2006 Mexican Elections: Manifestation of a Divided Society?PS: Political Science & Politics 40 (1): 2732.Google Scholar
Kunda, Ziva. 1990. “The Case for Motivated Reasoning.” Psychological Bulletin 108 (3): 480498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lelkes, Yphtach, and Westwood, Sean 2017. “The Limits of Partisan Prejudice.” Journal of Politics 79 (2): 485501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lodge, Milton, and Taber, Charles. 2013. The Rationalizing Voter. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lupu, Noam. 2015. “Partisanship in Latin America.” In The Latin American Voter: Pursuing Representation and Accountability in Challenging Contexts, edited by Ryan E. Carlin, Matthew M. Singer, and Elizabeth J. Zechmeister, 226–45. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
Mainwaring, Scott, ed. 2018. Party Systems in Latin America: Institutionalization, Decay, and Collapse. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maldonado, Arturo, and Seligson, Mitchell. 2014. “Electoral Trust in Latin America.” In Advancing Electoral Integrity, edited by Norris, Pippa, Frank, Richard, and Martinez i Coma, Ferran, 229245. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mason, Lilliana. 2018. Uncivil Agreement: How Politics became Our Identity. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mason, Lilliana, Wronski, Julie, and Kane, John. 2021. “Activating Animus: The Uniquely Social Roots of Trump Support.” American Political Science Review 115 (4): 15081516.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meléndez, Carlos. 2022. The Post-Partisans: Anti-Partisans, Anti-Establishment Identifiers, and Apartisans in Latin America. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, Joanne, Saunders, Kyle, and Farhart, Christina. 2016. “Conspiracy Endorsement as Motivated Reasoning: The Moderating Roles of Political Knowledge and Trust.” American Journal of Political Science 60 (4): 824844.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Monsiváis-Carrillo, Alejandro. 2023. “Happy Winners, Sore Partisans? Political Trust, Partisanship, and the Populist Assault on Electoral Integrity in Mexico.” Journal of Politics in Latin America 15 (1): 7295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moreno, Alejandro. 2022. “El INE: La convergencia en tiempos de polarización.” El Financiero, November 18.Google Scholar
Morgan, Jana. 2011. Bankrupt Representation and Party System Collapse. University Park, PA: Penn State University Press.Google Scholar
Morgan, Jana. 2007. “Partisanship during the Collapse of Venezuela’s Party System.” Latin American Research Review 42 (1): 7898.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mutz, Diana C. 2018. “Status Threat, Not Economic Hardship, Explains the 2016 Presidential Vote.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 115 (19): E4330E4339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norris, Pippa (Ed.). 1999. Critical Citizens: Global Support for Democratic Government. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norris, Pippa, Garnett, Holly, and Gromping, Max. 2020. “The Paranoid Style of American Elections: Explaining Perceptions of Electoral Integrity in an Age of Populism.” Journal of Elections, Public Opinion and Parties 30: 105125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nyhan, Brendan, and Reifler, Jason. 2010. “When Corrections Fail: The Persistence of Political Misperceptions.” Political Behavior 32 (2): 303330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nyhan, Brendan, Reifler, Jason, and Ubel, Peter. 2013. “The Hazards of Correcting Myths about Health Care Reform.” Medical Care 51 (2): 127132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Renno, Lucio. 2020. “The Bolsonaro Voter: Issue Positions and Vote Choice in the 2018 Brazilian Presidential Elections.” Latin American Politics and Society 62 (4): 123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
David, Samuels, and Zucco, Cesar. 2018. Partisans, Antipartisans, and Nonpartisans: Voting Behavior in Brazil. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Samuels, David, and Zucco, Cesar. 2018. Partisans, Antipartisans, and Nonpartisans: Voting Behavior in Brazil. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sances, Michael, and Stewart III, Charles. 2015. “Partisanship and Confidence in the Vote Count: Evidence from US National Elections since 2000.” Electoral Studies 40: 176188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schedler, Andreas. 2007. “The Mexican Standoff: The Mobilization of Distrust.” Journal of Democracy 18 (1): 88102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seawright, Jason. 2012. Party-System Collapse: The Roots of Crisis in Peru and Venezuela. Redwood City, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Sherif, Muzafer. 1961. Intergroup Conflict and Cooperation: The Robbers Cave Experiment . Vol. 10. Norman, OK: University Book Exchange.Google Scholar
Sides, John, Tesler, Michael, and Vavreck, Lynn 2019. Identity Crisis: The 2016 Presidential Campaign and the Battle for the Meaning of America. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Taber, Charles, and Lodge, Milton. 2006. “Motivated Skepticism in the Evaluation of Political Beliefs.” American Journal of Political Science 50 (3): 755769.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tajfel, Henri. 1981. Human Groups and Social Categories: Studies in Social Psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ugues, Antonio Jr. 2010. “Citizens’ Views on Electoral Governance in Mexico.” Journal of Elections, Public Opinion and Parties 20 (4): 495527.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ugues, Antonio Jr., and Vidal, D. Xavier Medina. 2015. “Public Evaluations of Electoral Institutions in Mexico: An Analysis of the IFE and TRIFE in the 2006 and 2012 Elections.” Electoral Studies 40: 231244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uscinski, Joseph, and Parent, Joseph. 2014. American Conspiracy Theories. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valentino, Nicholas. A. Carly Wayne, and Marzia Oceno. 2018. “Mobilizing Sexism: The Interaction of Emotion and Gender Attitudes in the 2016 US Presidential Election.” Public Opinion Quarterly 82 (S1): 799821.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vermeule, Adrian, and Sunstein, Cass. 2009. “Conspiracy Theories: Causes and Cures.” Journal of Political Philosophy 17 (2): 202227.Google Scholar
Wilson, David C., and King-Meadows, Tyson. 2016. “Perceived Electoral Malfeasance and Resentment over the Election of Barack Obama.” Electoral Studies 44: 3545.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wood, Thomas, and Porter, Ethan. 2018. “The Elusive Backfire Effect: Mass Attitudes’ Steadfast Factual Adherence.” Political Behavior 41: 145163.Google Scholar
Zaller, John. 1992. The Nature and Origins of Mass Opinion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Affective Polarization in Mexico (2000–2018).Note: Average ratings on the feeling thermometer (0 = very bad, and 10 = very good) among voters who self-identify with a political party.Source: Mexican Election Study 2000–2018 (Beltrán, Ley, and Castro Cornejo 2020).

Figure 1

Table 1. OLS models: Probability of believing that there was electoral fraud in 2006

Figure 2

Figure 2. OLS models on the probability of believing that there was electoral fraud in 2006.Note: values range from 0 = “not true at all” to 1 = “very true.”

Figure 3

Figure 3. Probability of believing that there was electoral fraud in 2006 (2018 Survey).

Figure 4

Figure 4. Probability of believing that there was electoral fraud in 2006 (2021 Mexican Election Study).

Figure 5

Table 2. OLS models: Probability of believing that there was electoral fraud in 2006

Figure 6

Figure 5. Probability of believing that there was electoral fraud in 2006.Note: Values range from 0 = “not true at all” to 10 = “very true.”

Supplementary material: File

Castro Cornejo supplementary material

Castro Cornejo supplementary material
Download Castro Cornejo supplementary material(File)
File 258.9 KB