Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-p2v8j Total loading time: 0.001 Render date: 2024-06-01T09:28:08.825Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

18 - Theory of Mind in Nonhuman Primates

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 July 2022

Bennett L. Schwartz
Affiliation:
Florida International University
Michael J. Beran
Affiliation:
Georgia State University
Get access

Summary

Social life demands complex strategies for coordinating and competing with others. In humans, these strategies are supported by rich cognitive mechanisms, such as theory of mind. Theory of mind (i.e., mental state attribution, mentalizing, or mindreading) is the ability to track the unobservable mental states, like desires and beliefs, that guide others’ actions. Deeply social animals, like most nonhuman primates, would surely benefit from the adept capacity to interpret and predict others’ behavior that theory of mind affords. Yet, after forty years of investigation, the extent to which nonhuman primates represent the minds of others remains a topic of contentious debate. In the present chapter, we review evidence consistent with the possibility that monkeys and apes are capable of inferring others’ goals, perceptions, and beliefs. We then evaluate the quality of that evidence and point to the most prominent alternative explanations to be addressed by future research. Finally, we take a more broadly phylogenetic perspective, to identify evolutionary modifications to social cognition that have emerged throughout primate evolutionary history and to consider the selective pressures that may have driven those modifications. Taken together, this approach sheds light on the complex mechanisms that define the social minds of humans and other primates.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2022

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aiello, L. C., & Wheeler, P. (1995). The expensive-tissue hypothesis: The brain and the digestive system in human and primate evolution. Current Anthropology, 36, 199221.Google Scholar
Amici, F., Aureli, F., Visalberghi, E., & Call, J. (2009). Spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi) and capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) follow gaze around barriers: Evidence for perspective taking? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 123, 368374.Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R., Kuroshima, H., Hattori, Y., & Fujita, K. (2010). Flexibility in the use of requesting gestures in squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus). American Journal of Primatology, 72, 707714.Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R., Kuroshima, H., Takimoto, A., & Fujita, K. (2013). Third-party social evaluation of humans by monkeys. Nature Communications, 4, 1561.Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R., & Mitchell, R. W. (1999). Macaques but not lemurs co-orient visually with humans. Folia Primatologica, 70, 1722.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, J. R., Montant, M., & Schmitt, D. (1996). Rhesus monkeys fail to use gaze direction as an experimenter-given cue in an object-choice task. Behavioural Processes, 37, 4755.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, J. R., Takimoto, A., Kuroshima, H., & Fujita, K. (2013). Capuchin monkeys judge third-party reciprocity. Cognition, 127, 140146.Google Scholar
Arre, A. M., Clark, C. S., & Santos, L. R. (2019). Do young rhesus macaques know what others see? A comparative developmental perspective. American Journal of Primatology, 82, e23054.Google Scholar
Arre, A. M., Stumph, E., & Santos, L. R. (2021). Macaque species with varying social tolerance show no differences in understanding what other agents perceive. Animal Cognition, 24(4), 877888.Google Scholar
Aychet, J., Pezzino, P., Rossard, A., Bec, P., Blois-Heulin, C., & Lemasson, A. (2020). Red-capped mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus) adapt their interspecific gestural communication to the recipient’s behaviour. Scientific Reports, 10, 12843.Google Scholar
Baillargeon, R., Scott, R. M., & Bian, L. (2016). Psychological reasoning in infancy. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 159186.Google Scholar
Baker, C. L., Jara-Ettinger, J., Saxe, R., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2017). Rational quantitative attribution of beliefs, desires and percepts in human mentalizing. Nature Human Behaviour, 1, 110.Google Scholar
Bania, A., & Stromberg, E. (2013). The effect of body orientation on judgments of human visual attention in western lowland gorillas (gorilla gorilla gorilla). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 127, 8290.Google Scholar
Barnes, J. L., Hill, T., Langer, M., Martinez, M., & Santos, L. R. (2008). Helping behaviour and regard for others in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Biology Letters, 4, 638640.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21, 3746.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. (2018). Picturing primates and looking at monkeys: Why 21st century primatology needs Wittgenstein. Philosophical Investigations, 41, 161187.Google Scholar
Behne, T., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Unwilling versus unable: Infants’ understanding of intentional action. Developmental Psychology, 41(2), 328337.Google Scholar
Bettle, R., & Rosati, A. (2019). Flexible gaze-following in rhesus monkeys. Animal Cognition, 22, 673686.Google Scholar
Bond, A. B., Kamil, A. C., & Balda, R. P. (2003). Social complexity and transitive inference in corvids. Animal Behaviour, 65, 479487.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Botting, J., & Bastian, M. (2019a). Orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus and hybrid) and gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) modify their visual, but not auditory, communicative behaviors, depending on the attentional state of a human experimenter. International Journal of Primatology, 40, 244262.Google Scholar
Botting, J., & Bastian, M. (2019b). Examining the use of auditory signals as “attention-getters” in zoo-housed gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) and orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus and hybrid). International Journal of Primatology, 40, 573586.Google Scholar
Bourjade, M., Meguerditchian, A., Maille, A., Gaunet, F., & Vauclair, J. (2014). Olive baboons, Papio anubis, adjust their visual and auditory intentional gestures to the visual attention of others. Animal Behaviour, 87, 121128.Google Scholar
Boyd, R. (2006). The puzzle of human sociality. Science, 314, 15551556.Google Scholar
Boysen, S. T., & Berntson, G. G. (1986). Cardiac correlates of individual recognition in the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 100, 321324.Google Scholar
Bräuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2005). All great ape species follow gaze to distant locations and around barriers. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 119, 145154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bräuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Chimpanzees really know what others can see in a competitive situation. Animal Cognition, 10, 439448.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bräuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Chimpanzees do not take into account what others can hear in a competitive situation. Animal Cognition, 11(1), 175178.Google Scholar
Bray, J., Krupenye, C., & Hare, B. (2014). Ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta) exploit information about what others can see but not what they can hear. Animal Cognition, 17, 735744.Google Scholar
Brügger, R. K., Willems, E. P., & Burkart, J. M. (2021). Do marmosets understand others’ conversations? A thermography approach. Science Advances, 7, eabc8790.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bugnyar, T., & Kotrschal, K. (2002). Observational learning and the raiding of food caches in ravens, Corvus corax: Is it “tactical” deception? Animal Behaviour, 64, 185195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bugnyar, T., Reber, S. A., & Buckner, C. (2016). Ravens attribute visual access to unseen competitors. Nature Communications, 7, 10506.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bugnyar, T., Stöwe, M., & Heinrich, B. (2004). Ravens, Corvus corax, follow gaze direction of humans around obstacles. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 271, 13311336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burkart, J., & Heschl, A. (2006). Geometrical gaze following in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 120, 120130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burkart, J., & Heschl, A. (2007). Understanding visual access in common marmosets, Callithrix jacchus: Perspective taking or behaviour reading? Animal Behaviour, 73, 457469.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burkart, J., Kupferberg, A., Glasauer, S., & van Schaik, C. (2012). Even simple forms of social learning rely on intention attribution in marmoset monkeys (Callithrix jacchus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126, 129138.Google Scholar
Burkart, J. M., Fehr, E., Efferson, C., & Schaik, C. P. van. (2007). Other-regarding preferences in a non-human primate: Common marmosets provision food altruistically. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 1976219766.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buttelmann, D., Buttelmann, F., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2017). Great apes distinguish true from false beliefs in an interactive helping task. PLoS ONE, 12, e0173793.Google Scholar
Buttelmann, D., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Enculturated chimpanzees imitate rationally. Developmental Science, 10, F31F38.Google Scholar
Buttelmann, D., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Eighteen-month-old infants show false belief understanding in an active helping paradigm. Cognition, 112(2), 337342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buttelmann, D., Over, H., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Eighteen-month-olds understand false beliefs in an unexpected-contents task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 119, 120126.Google Scholar
Buttelmann, D., Schütte, S., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Great apes infer others’ goals based on context. Animal Cognition, 15, 10371053.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butterfill, S. A., & Apperly, I. A. (2013). How to construct a minimal theory of mind. Mind & Language, 28, 606637.Google Scholar
Butterworth, G., & Jarrett, N. (1991). What minds have in common is space: Spatial mechanisms serving joint visual attention in infancy. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9(1), 5572.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. W., & Bates, L. A. (2007). Sociality, evolution and cognition. Current Biology, 17, R714R723.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. W., & Whiten, A. (1988). Toward the next generation in data quality: A new survey of primate tactical deception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11, 267273.Google Scholar
Call, J., Agnetta, B., & Tomasello, M. (2000). Cues that chimpanzees do and do not use to find hidden objects. Animal Cognition, 3, 2334.Google Scholar
Call, J., Hare, B., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2004). “Unwilling” versus “unable”: Chimpanzees’ understanding of human intentional action. Developmental Science, 7, 488498.Google Scholar
Call, J., Hare, B. A., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Chimpanzee gaze following in an object-choice task. Animal Cognition, 1, 8999.Google Scholar
Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (1994). Production and comprehension of referential pointing by orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 108, 307317.Google Scholar
Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Distinguishing intentional from accidental actions in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 112, 192206.Google Scholar
Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (1999). A nonverbal false belief task: The performance of children and great apes. Child Development, 70, 381395.Google Scholar
Canteloup, C., Bovet, D., & Meunier, H. (2015). Intentional gestural communication and discrimination of human attentional states in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Animal Cognition, 18, 875883.Google Scholar
Canteloup, C., & Meunier, H. (2017). “Unwilling” versus “unable”: Tonkean macaques’ understanding of human goal-directed actions. PeerJ, 5, e3227.Google Scholar
Canteloup, C., Piraux, E., Poulin, N., & Meunier, H. (2016). Do Tonkean macaques (Macaca tonkeana) perceive what conspecifics do and do not see? PeerJ, 4, e1693.Google Scholar
Canteloup, C., Poitrasson, I., Anderson, J. R., Poulin, N., & Meunier, H. (2017). Factors influencing deceptive behaviours in Tonkean macaques (Macaca tonkeana). Behaviour, 154, 765784.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Breton, C. (2002). How specific is the relation between executive function and theory of mind? Contributions of inhibitory control and working memory. Infant and Child Development, 11, 7392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, M., Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Fourteen- through 18-month-old infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions. Infant Behavior and Development, 21(2), 315330.Google Scholar
Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2002). Understanding “prior intentions” enables two-year-olds to imitatively learn a complex task. Child Development, 73(5), 14311441.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Twelve- and 18-month-olds copy actions in terms of goals. Developmental Science, 8(1), F13F20.Google Scholar
Cheney, D., & Seyfarth, R. (1990). Attending to behaviour versus attending to knowledge: Examining monkeys’ attribution of mental states. Animal Behaviour, 40, 742753.Google Scholar
Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (2007). Baboon metaphysics: The evolution of a social mind. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Clements, W. A., & Perner, J. (1994). Implicit understanding of belief. Cognitive Development, 9, 377395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clutton‐Brock, T. H., & Harvey, P. H. (1980). Primates, brains and ecology. Journal of Zoology, 190, 309323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costes-Thiré, M., Levé, M., Uhlrich, P., Pasquaretta, C., De Marco, A., & Thierry, B. (2015). Evidence that monkeys (Macaca tonkeana and Sapajus apella) read moves, but no evidence that they read goals. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 129, 304310.Google Scholar
Crockford, C., Wittig, R. M., Mundry, R., & Zuberbühler, K. (2012). Wild chimpanzees inform ignorant group members of danger. Current Biology, 22, 142146.Google Scholar
Crockford, C., Wittig, R. M., & Zuberbühler, K. (2017). Vocalizing in chimpanzees is influenced by social-cognitive processes. Science Advances, 3, e1701742.Google Scholar
de Waal, F. (1982). Chimpanzee politics: Power and sex among apes. Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
DeCasien, A. R., Williams, S. A., & Higham, J. P. (2017). Primate brain size is predicted by diet but not sociality. Nature Ecology & Evolution, 1(5), 17.Google Scholar
Defolie, C., Malassis, R., Serre, M., & Meunier, H. (2015). Tufted capuchins (Cebus apella) adapt their communicative behaviour to human’s attentional states. Animal Cognition, 18, 747755.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. (1978). Beliefs about beliefs. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 568570.Google Scholar
Deshpande, A., Gupta, S., & Sinha, A. (2018). Intentional communication between wild bonnet macaques and humans. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 5147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Drayton, L. A., & Santos, L. R. (2014). Capuchins’ (Cebus apella) sensitivity to others’ goal-directed actions in a helping context. Animal Cognition, 17, 689700.Google Scholar
Drayton, L. A., & Santos, L. R. (2017). Do rhesus macaques, Macaca mulatta, understand what others know when gaze following? Animal Behaviour, 134, 193199.Google Scholar
Drayton, L. A., & Santos, L. R. (2018). What do monkeys know about others’ knowledge? Cognition, 170, 201208.Google Scholar
Drayton, L. A., Varman, L., & Santos, L. R. (2016). Capuchins (Cebus apella) are limited in their ability to infer others’ goals based on context. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 130(1), 7175.Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M., & Shultz, S. (2007). Evolution in the social brain. Science, 317, 13441347.Google Scholar
Durdevic, K., & Krupenye, C. (2021). Representing knowledge, belief, and everything in between: Representational complexity in humans and other apes. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 44, e150. In press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckert, J., Rakoczy, H., Call, J., Herrmann, E., & Hanus, D. (2018). Chimpanzees consider humans’ psychological states when drawing statistical inferences. Current Biology, 28, 19591963.Google Scholar
Emery, N. J., & Clayton, N. S. (2001). Effects of experience and social context on prospective caching strategies by scrub jays. Nature, 414, 443446.Google Scholar
Emery, N. J., & Clayton, N. S. (2004). The mentality of crows: Convergent evolution of intelligence in corvids and apes. Science, 306, 19031907.Google Scholar
Flombaum, J. I., & Santos, L. R. (2005). Rhesus monkeys attribute perceptions to others. Current Biology, 15, 447452.Google Scholar
Foerster, S., McLellan, K., Schroepfer-Walker, K., Murray, C. M., Krupenye, C., Gilby, I. C., & Pusey, A. E. (2015). Social bonds in the dispersing sex: Partner preferences among adult female chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 105, 139152.Google Scholar
Gergely, G., Bekkering, H., & Király, I. (2002). Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature, 415(6873), 755755.Google Scholar
Gergely, G., & Csibra, G. (2003). Teleological reasoning in infancy: The naïve theory of rational action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 287292.Google Scholar
Gergely, G., Nadasdy, Z., Csibra, G., & Bíró, S. (1995). Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition, 56(2), 165193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilby, I. C., Brent, L. J. N., Wroblewski, E. E., Rudicell, R. S., Hahn, B. H., Goodall, J., & Pusey, A. E. (2013). Fitness benefits of coalitionary aggression in male chimpanzees. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 67, 373381.Google Scholar
Goossens, B. M. A., Dekleva, M., Reader, S. M., Sterck, E. H. M., & Bolhuis, J. J. (2008). Gaze following in monkeys is modulated by observed facial expressions. Animal Behaviour, 75, 16731681.Google Scholar
Goossens, B., Reader, S. M., van den Berg, L., & Sterck, E. H. M. (2012). An analysis of gaze following to a hidden location in long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). Behaviour, 149, 13191337.Google Scholar
Grosse Wiesmann, C., Friederici, A. D., Singer, T., & Steinbeis, N. (2017). Implicit and explicit false belief development in preschool children. Developmental Science, 20(5), e12445.Google Scholar
Grueneisen, S., Duguid, S., Saur, H., & Tomasello, M. (2017). Children, chimpanzees, and bonobos adjust the visibility of their actions for cooperators and competitors. Scientific Reports, 7, 8504.Google Scholar
Hall, K., Oram, M. W., Campbell, M. W., Eppley, T. M., Byrne, R. W., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2017). Chimpanzee uses manipulative gaze cues to conceal and reveal information to foraging competitor. American Journal of Primatology, 79(3), e22622.Google Scholar
Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2007). Social evaluation by preverbal infants. Nature, 450(7169), 557559.Google Scholar
Hammerstein, P. (Ed.). (2003). Genetic and cultural evolution of cooperation. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hare, B. (2001). Can competitive paradigms increase the validity of experiments on primate social cognition? Animal Cognition, 4, 269280.Google Scholar
Hare, B. (2011). From hominoid to hominid mind: What changed and why? Annual Review of Anthropology, 40, 293309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hare, B., Addessi, E., Call, J., Tomasello, M., & Visalberghi, E. (2003). Do capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella, know what conspecifics do and do not see? Animal Behaviour, 65(1), 131142.Google Scholar
Hare, B., Call, J., Agnetta, B., & Tomasello, M. (2000). Chimpanzees know what conspecifics do and do not see. Animal Behaviour, 59, 771785.Google Scholar
Hare, B., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2001). Do chimpanzees know what conspecifics know? Animal Behaviour, 61, 139151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hare, B., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Chimpanzees deceive a human competitor by hiding. Cognition, 101, 495514.Google Scholar
Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2004). Chimpanzees are more skillful in competitive than in cooperative cognitive tasks. Animal Behaviour, 68, 571581.Google Scholar
Hare, B., Wobber, V., & Wrangham, R. (2012). The self-domestication hypothesis: Evolution of bonobo psychology is due to selection against aggression. Animal Behaviour, 83, 573585.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hattori, Y., Kuroshima, H., & Fujita, K. (2007). I know you are not looking at me: Capuchin monkeys’ (Cebus apella) sensitivity to human attentional states. Animal Cognition, 10, 141148.Google Scholar
Hattori, Y., Kuroshima, H., & Fujita, K. (2010). Tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) show understanding of human attentional states when requesting food held by a human. Animal Cognition, 13, 8792.Google Scholar
Hayashi, T., Akikawa, R., Kawasaki, K., Egawa, J., Minamimoto, T., Kobayashi, K., Kato, S., Hori, Y., Nagai, Y., Iijima, A., Someya, T., & Hasegawa, I. (2020). Macaques exhibit implicit gaze bias anticipating others’ false-belief-driven actions via medial prefrontal cortex. Cell Reports, 30, 44334444.Google Scholar
Heesen, R., Bangerter, A., Zuberbühler, K., Rossano, F., Iglesias, K., Guéry, J.-P., & Genty, E. (2020). Bonobos engage in joint commitment. Science Advances, 6, eabd1306.Google Scholar
Henley, T., & Povinelli, D. J. (2020). Seeing through: An analysis of Kano et al. (2019). Animal Behavior and Cognition, 7, 658660.Google Scholar
Herrmann, E., Hernández-Lloreda, M. V., Call, J., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2010). The structure of individual differences in the cognitive abilities of children and chimpanzees. Psychological Science, 21(1), 102110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herrmann, E., Call, J., Hernandez-Lloreda, M. V., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition: The cultural intelligence hypothesis. Science, 317, 13601366.Google Scholar
Herrmann, E., Keupp, S., Hare, B., Vaish, A., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Direct and indirect reputation formation in nonhuman great apes (Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, Gorilla gorilla, Pongo pygmaeus) and human children (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 127(1), 6375.Google Scholar
Heyes, C. M. (1993). Anecdotes, training, trapping and triangulating: Do animals attribute mental states? Animal Behaviour, 46, 177188.Google Scholar
Heyes, C. M. (1998). Theory of mind in nonhuman primates. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 101114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heyes, C. (2014a). Submentalizing: I am not really reading your mind. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9, 131143.Google Scholar
Heyes, C. (2014b). False belief in infancy: A fresh look. Developmental Science, 17, 647659.Google Scholar
Heyes, C. (2015). Animal mindreading: What’s the problem? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22, 313327.Google Scholar
Heyes, C. (2017). Apes submentalise. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(1), 12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heyes, C. M., & Frith, C. D. (2014). The cultural evolution of mind reading. Science, 344, 12430911243091.Google Scholar
Horner, V., & Whiten, A. (2005). Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens). Animal Cognition, 8, 164181.Google Scholar
Horschler, D. J., Santos, L. R., & MacLean, E. L. (2019). Do non-human primates really represent others’ ignorance? A test of the awareness relations hypothesis. Cognition, 190, 7280.Google Scholar
Horschler, D. J., Santos, L. R., & MacLean, E. L. (2021). How do non-human primates represent others’ awareness of where objects are hidden? Cognition, 212, 104658.Google Scholar
Horton, K. E., & Caldwell, C. A. (2006). Visual co-orientation and expectations about attentional orientation in pileated gibbons (Hylobates pileatus). Behavioural Processes, 72, 6573.Google Scholar
Hostetter, A. B., Cantero, M., & Hopkins, W. D. (2001). Differential use of vocal and gestural communication by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in response to the attentional status of a human (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 115, 337343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humphrey, N. (1976). The social function of intellect. In Hinde, R. A. and Bateson, P. P. R. (Eds.), Growing points in ethology (Vol. 37, pp. 303317). CUP Archive.Google Scholar
Itakura, S. (1996). An exploratory study of gaze-monitoring in nonhuman primates. Japanese Psychological Research, 38(3), 174180.Google Scholar
Jolly, A. (1966). Lemur social behavior and primate intelligence. Science, 153, 501506.Google Scholar
Joly, M., Micheletta, J., De Marco, A., Langermans, J. A., Sterck, E. H. M., & Waller, B. M. (2017). Comparing physical and social cognitive skills in macaque species with different degrees of social tolerance. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 284(1862), 20162738.Google Scholar
Kaminski, J. (2011). Communicative cues among and between human and non-human primates: Attending to specificity in triadic gestural interactions. In Boos, M., Kolbe, M., Kappeler, P. M., & Ellwart, T. (Eds.), Coordination in human and primate groups (pp. 245261). Springer Berlin Heidelberg.Google Scholar
Kaminski, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2004). Body orientation and face orientation: Two factors controlling apes’ begging behavior from humans. Animal Cognition, 7, 216223.Google Scholar
Kaminski, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Chimpanzees know what others know, but not what they believe. Cognition, 109, 224234.Google Scholar
Kaminski, J., Riedel, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Domestic goats, Capra hircus, follow gaze direction and use social cues in an object choice task. Animal Behaviour, 69, 1118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kano, F., & Call, J. (2014a). Cross-species variation in gaze following and conspecific preference among great apes, human infants and adults. Animal Behaviour, 91, 137150.Google Scholar
Kano, F., & Call, J. (2014b). Great apes generate goal-based action predictions: An eye-tracking study. Psychological Science, 25, 16911698.Google Scholar
Kano, F., Call, J., & Krupenye, C. (2020). Primates pass dynamically social anticipatory-looking false-belief tests. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24, 777778.Google Scholar
Kano, F., Krupenye, C., Hirata, S., & Call, J. (2017). Eye tracking uncovered great apes’ ability to anticipate that other individuals will act according to false beliefs. Communicative & Integrative Biology, 10, e1299836.Google Scholar
Kano, F., Krupenye, C., Hirata, S., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2017). Submentalizing cannot explain belief-based action anticipation in apes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21, 633634.Google Scholar
Kano, F., Krupenye, C., Hirata, S., Tomonaga, M., & Call, J. (2019). Great apes use self-experience to anticipate an agent’s action in a false-belief test. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116, 2090420909.Google Scholar
Kappeler, P., & Schaik, C. (2002). Evolution of primate social systems. International Journal of Primatology, 23, 707740.Google Scholar
Karg, K., Schmelz, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2015a). The goggles experiment: Can chimpanzees use self-experience to infer what a competitor can see? Animal Behaviour, 105, 211221.Google Scholar
Karg, K., Schmelz, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2015b). Chimpanzees strategically manipulate what others can see. Animal Cognition, 18, 10691076.Google Scholar
Karg, K., Schmelz, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Differing views: Can chimpanzees do Level 2 perspective-taking? Animal Cognition, 19, 555564.Google Scholar
Karin-D’Arcy, R. M., & Povinelli, D. J. (2002). Do chimpanzees know what each other see? A closer look. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 15, 2154.Google Scholar
Kawai, N., Nakagami, A., Yasue, M., Koda, H., & Ichinohe, N. (2019). Common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) evaluate third-party social interactions of human actors but Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata) do not. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 133, 488495.Google Scholar
Kawai, N., Yasue, M., Banno, T., & Ichinohe, N. (2014). Marmoset monkeys evaluate third-party reciprocity. Biology Letters, 10, 20140058.Google Scholar
Kovacs, A. M., Teglas, E., & Endress, A. D. (2010). The social sense: Susceptibility to others’ beliefs in human infants and adults. Science, 330, 18301834.Google Scholar
Krachun, C., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). A competitive nonverbal false belief task for children and apes. Developmental Science, 12, 521535.Google Scholar
Krachun, C., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2010). A new change-of-contents false belief test: Children and chimpanzees compared. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 23, 145165.Google Scholar
Krachun, C., Lurz, R., Mahovetz, L. M., & Hopkins, W. D. (2019). Mirror self-recognition and its relationship to social cognition in chimpanzees. Animal Cognition, 22, 11711183.Google Scholar
Krupenye, C. (2020). The evolution of mentalizing in humans and other primates. In Gilead, M. & Ochsner, K. (Eds.), The neural basis of mentalizing: A social-cognitive and affective neuroscience perspective. Springer Press.Google Scholar
Krupenye, C., & Call, J. (2019). Theory of mind in animals: Current and future directions. WIREs Cognitive Science, 10, e1503.Google Scholar
Krupenye, C., & Hare, B. (2018). Bonobos prefer individuals that hinder others over those that help. Current Biology, 28, 280286.Google Scholar
Krupenye, C., Kano, F., Hirata, S., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Great apes anticipate that other individuals will act according to false beliefs. Science, 354, 110114.Google Scholar
Krupenye, C., Kano, F., Hirata, S., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2017). A test of the submentalizing hypothesis: Apes’ performance in a false belief task inanimate control. Communicative & Integrative Biology, 10, e1343771.Google Scholar
Krupenye, C., MacLean, E., & Hare, B. (2017). Does the bonobo have a (chimpanzee-like) theory of mind? In Hare, B. & Yamamoto, S. (Eds.), Bonobos: Unique in mind, brain and behavior. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Krupenye, C., Tan, J., & Hare, B. (2018). Bonobos voluntarily hand food to others but not toys or tools. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 285, 20181536.Google Scholar
Kummer, H., Anzenberger, G., & Hemelrijk, C. K. (1996). Hiding and perspective taking in long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 110, 97102.Google Scholar
Kupferberg, A., Glasauer, S., & Burkart, J. M. (2013). Do robots have goals? How agent cues influence action understanding in non-human primates. Behavioural Brain Research, 246, 4754.Google Scholar
Kuroshima, H., Fujita, K., Adachi, I., Iwata, K., & Fuyuki, A. (2003). A capuchin monkey (Cebus apella) recognizes when people do and do not know the location of food. Animal Cognition, 6, 283291.Google Scholar
Kuroshima, H., Fujita, K., Fuyuki, A., & Masuda, T. (2002). Understanding of the relationship between seeing and knowing by tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Animal Cognition, 5, 4148.Google Scholar
Lake, B. M., Ullman, T. D., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Gershman, S. J. (2017). Building machines that learn and think like people. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 40.Google Scholar
Lamaury, A., Cochet, H., & Bourjade, M. (2019). Acquisition of joint attention by olive baboons gesturing toward humans. Animal Cognition, 22(4), 567575.Google Scholar
Leavens, D. A. (1998). Having a concept “see” does not imply attribution of knowledge: Some general considerations in measuring “theories of mind.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 123124.Google Scholar
Leavens, D. A., Bard, K. A., & Hopkins, W. D. (2019). The mismeasure of ape social cognition. Animal Cognition, 22, 487504.Google Scholar
Leavens, D. A., Hopkins, W. D., & Thomas, R. K. (2004). Referential communication by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118, 4857.Google Scholar
Lewis, L., Kano, F., Stevens, J., DuBois, J., Call, J., & Krupenye, C. (2021). Bonobos and chimpanzees preferentially attend to familiar members of the dominant sex. Animal Behaviour, 177, 193206.Google Scholar
Liebal, K., Call, J., Tomasello, M., & Pika, S. (2004). To move or not to move: How apes adjust to the attentional state of others. Interaction Studies, 5, 199219.Google Scholar
Liebal, K., & Kaminski, J. (2012). Gibbons (Hylobates pileatus, H. moloch, H. lar, Symphalangus syndactylus) follow human gaze, but do not take the visual perspective of others. Animal Cognition, 15, 12111216.Google Scholar
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Twelve-month-olds communicate helpfully and appropriately for knowledgeable and ignorant partners. Cognition, 108(3), 732739.Google Scholar
Liu, D., Wellman, H. M., Tardif, T., & Sabbagh, M. A. (2008). Theory of mind development in Chinese children: A meta-analysis of false-belief understanding across cultures and languages. Developmental Psychology, 44, 523531.Google Scholar
Lohmann, H., & Tomasello, M. (2003). The role of language in the development of false belief understanding: A training study. Child Development, 74, 11301144.Google Scholar
Lorincz, E., Jellema, T., Barraclough, N., Xiao, D., & Perrett, D. (2005). From monkey brain to human brain: A Fyssen Foundation Symposium. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Luo, Y., & Baillargeon, R. (2007). Do 12.5-month-old infants consider what objects others can see when interpreting their actions? Cognition, 105(3), 489512.Google Scholar
Lurz, R. (2009). If chimpanzees are mindreaders, could behavioral science tell? Toward a solution of the logical problem. Philosophical Psychology, 22, 305328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lurz, R., Krachun, C., Mahovetz, L., Wilson, M. J. G., & Hopkins, W. (2018). Chimpanzees gesture to humans in mirrors: Using reflection to dissociate seeing from line of gaze. Animal Behaviour, 135, 239249.Google Scholar
Lyons, D. E., & Santos, L. R. (2006). Ecology, domain specificity, and the origins of theory of mind: Is competition the catalyst? Philosophy Compass, 1, 481492.Google Scholar
MacLean, E. L., & Hare, B. (2012). Bonobos and chimpanzees infer the target of another’s attention. Animal Behaviour, 83, 345353.Google Scholar
MacLean, E. L., Hare, B., Nunn, C. L., Addessi, E., Amici, F., Anderson, R. C., Aureli, F., Baker, J. M., Bania, A. E., Barnard, A. M., Boogert, N. J., Brannon, E. M., Bray, E. E., Bray, J., Brent, L. J. N., Burkart, J. M., Call, J., Cantlon, J. F., Cheke, L. G., … & Zhao, Y. (2014). The evolution of self-control. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111, E2140E2148.Google Scholar
MacLean, E. L., Merritt, D. J., & Brannon, E. M. (2008). Social complexity predicts transitive reasoning in prosimian primates. Animal Behaviour, 76, 479486.Google Scholar
MacLean, E. L., Sandel, A. A., Bray, J., Oldenkamp, R. E., Reddy, R. B., & Hare, B. A. (2013). Group size predicts social but not nonsocial cognition in lemurs. PLoS ONE, 8, e66359.Google Scholar
Maille, A., Engelhart, L., Bourjade, M., & Blois-Heulin, C. (2012). To beg, or not to beg? That is the question: Mangabeys modify their production of requesting gestures in response to human’s attentional states. PLoS ONE, 7, e41197.Google Scholar
Marechal, L., Genty, E., & Roeder, J. J. (2010). Recognition of faces of known individuals in two lemur species (Eulemur fulvus and E. macaco). Animal Behaviour, 79, 11571163.Google Scholar
Marticorena, D. C. W., Ruiz, A. M., Mukerji, C., Goddu, A., & Santos, L. R. (2011). Monkeys represent others’ knowledge but not their beliefs. Developmental Science, 14, 14061416.Google Scholar
Martin, A. (2019). Belief representation in great apes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23, 985986.Google Scholar
Martin, A., & Santos, L. R. (2014). The origins of belief representation: Monkeys fail to automatically represent others’ beliefs. Cognition, 130, 300308.Google Scholar
Martin, A., & Santos, L. R. (2016). What cognitive representations support primate theory of mind? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20, 375382.Google Scholar
Melis, A. P., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2006a). Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) conceal visual and auditory information from others. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 120, 154162.Google Scholar
Melis, A. P., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2010). 36-month-olds conceal visual and auditory information from others. Developmental Science, 13(3), 479489.Google Scholar
Melis, A. P., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2006b). Engineering cooperation in chimpanzees: Tolerance constraints on cooperation. Animal Behaviour, 72, 275286.Google Scholar
Melis, A. P., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Chimpanzees’ (Pan troglodytes) strategic helping in a collaborative task. Biology Letters, 9, 20130009.Google Scholar
Melis, A. P., & Tomasello, M. (2019). Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) coordinate by communicating in a collaborative problem-solving task. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 286, 20190408.Google Scholar
Melis, A. P., Warneken, F., Jensen, K., Schneider, A.-C., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Chimpanzees help conspecifics obtain food and non-food items. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 278, 14051413.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. (1995). Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology, 31(5), 838850.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N., & Brooks, R. (2008). Self-experience as a mechanism for learning about others: A training study in social cognition. Developmental Psychology, 44, 12571265.Google Scholar
Met, A., Miklósi, Á., & Lakatos, G. (2014). Gaze-following behind barriers in domestic dogs. Animal Cognition, 17, 14011405.Google Scholar
Meunier, H. (2017). Do monkeys have a theory of mind? How to answer the question. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 82, 110123.Google Scholar
Meunier, H., Prieur, J., & Vauclair, J. (2013). Olive baboons communicate intentionally by pointing. Animal Cognition, 16(2), 155163.Google Scholar
Micheletta, J., & Waller, B. (2012). Friendship affects gaze following in a tolerant species of macaque, Macaca nigra. Animal Behaviour, 83, 459467.Google Scholar
Milligan, K., Astington, J. W., & Dack, L. A. (2007). Language and theory of mind: Meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding. Child Development, 78, 622646.Google Scholar
Milton, K. (1998). The social behavior of chimpanzees and bonobos. Current Anthropology, 39, 411412.Google Scholar
Moll, H., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2011). How does it look? Level 2 perspective-taking at 36 months of age. Child Development, 82(2), 661673.Google Scholar
Myowa-Yamakoshi, M., & Matsuzawa, T. (2000). Imitation of intentional manipulatory actions in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 114, 381391.Google Scholar
Myowa-Yamakoshi, M., Scola, C., & Hirata, S. (2012). Humans and chimpanzees attend differently to goal-directed actions. Nature Communications, 3, 693.Google Scholar
Novey, M. S. (1979). The development of knowledge of others’ ability to see. PhD Thesis. Harvard University.Google Scholar
O’Connell, S., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2003). A test for comprehension of false belief in chimpanzees. Evolution and Cognition, 9(2), 131140.Google Scholar
Okamoto, S., Tanaka, M., & Tomonaga, M. (2004). Looking back: The “representational mechanism” of joint attention in an infant chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). Japanese Psychological Research, 46, 236245.Google Scholar
Okamoto-Barth, S., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Great apes’ understanding of other individuals’ line of sight. Psychological Science, 18, 462468.Google Scholar
Olineck, K. M., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2005). Infants’ ability to distinguish between intentional and accidental actions and its relation to internal state language. Infancy, 8(1), 91100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Onishi, K. H., & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science, 308(5719), 255258.Google Scholar
Overduin-de Vries, A., Spruijt, B., & Sterck, E. (2013). Long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis) understand what conspecifics can see in a competitive situation. Animal Cognition, 17, 7784.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palagi, E., & Dapporto, L. (2006). Beyond odor discrimination: Demonstrating individual recognition by scent in Lemur catta. Chemical Senses, 31, 437443.Google Scholar
Penn, D. C., Holyoak, K. J., & Povinelli, D. J. (2008). Darwin’s mistake: Explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 109130.Google Scholar
Penn, D. C., & Povinelli, D. J. (2007). On the lack of evidence that non-human animals possess anything remotely resembling a “theory of mind.” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 362, 731744.Google Scholar
Perner, J. (2012). MiniMeta: In search of minimal criteria for metacognition. In Beran, M. J., Brandl, J., Perner, J., & Proust, J. (Eds.), Foundations of metacognition (pp. 94116). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Perner, J., Leekam, S. R., & Wimmer, H. (1987). Three-year-olds’ difficulty with false belief: The case for a conceptual deficit. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5(2), 125137.Google Scholar
Phillips, J., & Norby, A. (2019). Factive theory of mind. Mind & Language, 36, 326.Google Scholar
Phillips, W., Barnes, J. L., Mahajan, N., Yamaguchi, M., & Santos, L. R. (2009). “Unwilling” versus “unable”: Capuchin monkeys’ (Cebus apella) understanding of human intentional action. Developmental Science, 12, 938945.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pillow, B. H., & Pearson, R. M. (2012). Children’s evaluation of the certainty of another person’s inductive inferences and guesses. Cognitive Development, 27(3), 299313.Google Scholar
Pokorny, J. J., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2009a). Monkeys recognize the faces of group mates in photographs. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 2153921543.Google Scholar
Pokorny, J. J., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2009b). Face recognition in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 123, 151160.Google Scholar
Pontzer, H., Durazo-Arvizu, R., Dugas, L. R., Plange-Rhule, J., Bovet, P., Forrester, T. E., Lambert, E. V., Cooper, R. S., Schoeller, D. A., & Luke, A. (2016). Constrained total energy expenditure and metabolic adaptation to physical activity in adult humans. Current Biology, 26, 410417.Google Scholar
Poss, S. R., Kuhar, C., Stoinski, T. S., & Hopkins, W. D. (2006). Differential use of attentional and visual communicative signaling by orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) and gorillas (Gorilla gorilla) in response to the attentional status of a human. American Journal of Primatology, 68(10), 978992.Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J., & DeBlois, S. (1992). Young children’s (Homo sapiens) understanding of knowledge formation in themselves and others. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 106(3), 228238.Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J., & Eddy, T. J. (1996). Chimpanzees: Joint visual attention. Psychological Science, 7, 129135.Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J., & Eddy, T. J. (1997). Specificity of gaze-following in young chimpanzees. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15, 213222.Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J., Nelson, K. E., & Boysen, S. T. (1990). Inferences about guessing and knowing by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 104, 203210.Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J., Parks, K. A., & Novak, M. A. (1991). Do rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) attribute knowledge and ignorance to others? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 105, 318325.Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J., Perilloux, H. K., Reaux, J. E., & Bierschwale, D. T. (1998). Young and juvenile chimpanzees’ (Pan troglodytes) reactions to intentional versus accidental and inadvertent actions. Behavioral Processes, 42, 205218.Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J., Rulf, A. B., & Bierschwale, D. T. (1994). Absence of knowledge attribution and self-recognition in young chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 108, 7480.Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J., Theall, L. A., Reaux, J. E., & Dunphy-Lelii, S. (2003). Chimpanzees spontaneously alter the location of their gestures to match the attentional orientation of others. Animal Behaviour, 66(1), 7179.Google Scholar
Powell, L. E., Isler, K., & Barton, R. A. (2017). Re-evaluating the link between brain size and behavioural ecology in primates. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 284, 20171765.Google Scholar
Powell, L. J., & Carey, S. (2017). Executive function depletion in children and its impact on theory of mind. Cognition, 164, 150162.Google Scholar
Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 515526.Google Scholar
Pyers, J. E., & Senghas, A. (2009). Language promotes false-belief understanding: Evidence from learners of a new sign language. Psychological Science, 20, 805812.Google Scholar
Pylyshyn, Z. (1978). When is attribution of beliefs justified? The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 592593.Google Scholar
Reader, S. M., Hager, Y., & Laland, K. N. (2011). The evolution of primate general and cultural intelligence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366, 10171027.Google Scholar
Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2020). The human life history is adapted to exploit the adaptive advantages of culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 375, 20190498.Google Scholar
Rochat, M. J., Serra, E., Fadiga, L., & Gallese, V. (2008). The evolution of social cognition: Goal familiarity shapes monkeys’ action understanding. Current Biology, 18, 227232.Google Scholar
Rosati, A. G. (2018). Chimpanzee cognition and the roots of the human mind. In Muller, M. N. (Ed.), Chimpanzees and human evolution (pp. 703745). Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Rosati, A. G., & Hare, B. (2009). Looking past the model species: Diversity in gaze-following skills across primates. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 19, 4551.Google Scholar
Ruiz, A., Gómez, J. C., Roeder, J. J., & Byrne, R. W. (2009). Gaze following and gaze priming in lemurs. Animal Cognition, 12, 427434.Google Scholar
Russell, Y. I., Call, J., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2008). Image scoring in great apes. Behavioural Processes, 78(1), 108111.Google Scholar
Sánchez-Amaro, A., Tan, J., Kaufhold, S. P., & Rossano, F. (2020). Gibbons exploit information about what a competitor can see. Animal Cognition, 23, 289299.Google Scholar
Sandel, A. A., MacLean, E. L., & Hare, B. (2011). Evidence from four lemur species that ringtailed lemur social cognition converges with that of haplorhine primates. Animal Behaviour, 81, 925931.Google Scholar
Santos, L. R., Nissen, A. G., & Ferrugia, J. A. (2006). Rhesus monkeys, Macaca mulatta, know what others can and cannot hear. Animal Behaviour, 71, 11751181.Google Scholar
Scaife, M., & Bruner, J. S. (1975). The capacity for joint visual attention in the infant. Nature, 253(5489), 265266.Google Scholar
Scerif, G., Gomez, J.-C., & Byrne, R. W. (2004). What do Diana monkeys know about the focus of attention of a conspecific? Animal Behaviour, 68, 12391247.Google Scholar
Schloegl, C., Kotrschal, K., & Bugnyar, T. (2007). Gaze following in common ravens, Corvus corax: Ontogeny and habituation. Animal Behaviour, 74, 769778.Google Scholar
Schmelz, M., & Call, J. (2016). The psychology of primate cooperation and competition: A call for realigning research agendas. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371, 20150067.Google Scholar
Schmelz, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Chimpanzees know that others make inferences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 30773079.Google Scholar
Schmelz, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Chimpanzees predict that a competitor’s preference will match their own. Biology Letters, 9, 20120829.Google Scholar
Schnoell, A. V., & Fichtel, C. (2012). Wild redfronted lemurs (Eulemur rufifrons) use social information to learn new foraging techniques. Animal Cognition, 15, 505516.Google Scholar
Senju, A., Southgate, V., Snape, C., Leonard, M., & Csibra, G. (2011). Do 18-month-olds really attribute mental states to others? A critical test. Psychological Science, 22, 878880.Google Scholar
Shepherd, S. V., & Platt, M. L. (2007). Spontaneous social orienting and gaze following in ringtailed lemurs (Lemur catta). Animal Cognition, 11, 1320.Google Scholar
Shultz, S., Opie, C., & Atkinson, Q. D. (2011). Stepwise evolution of stable sociality in primates. Nature, 479, 219222.Google Scholar
Silk, J. B., Beehner, J. C., Bergman, T. J., Crockford, C., Engh, A. L., Moscovice, L. R., Wittig, R. M., Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (2010). Strong and consistent social bonds enhance the longevity of female baboons. Current Biology, 20, 13591361.Google Scholar
Slocombe, K. E., & Zuberbuhler, K. (2007). Chimpanzees modify recruitment screams as a function of audience composition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(43), 1722817233.Google Scholar
Snyder-Mackler, N., Burger, J. R., Gaydosh, L., Belsky, D. W., Noppert, G. A., Campos, F. A., Bartolomucci, A., Yang, Y. C., Aiello, A. E., O’Rand, A., Harris, K. M., Shively, C. A., Alberts, S. C., & Tung, J. (2020). Social determinants of health and survival in humans and other animals. Science, 368(6493).Google Scholar
Spadacenta, S., Dicke, P. W., & Thier, P. (2019). Reflexive gaze following in common marmoset monkeys. Scientific Reports, 9, 15292.Google Scholar
Steiper, M. E., & Young, N. M. (2006). Primate molecular divergence dates. Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution, 41, 384394.Google Scholar
Sterelny, K. (2019). The origins of multi-level society. Topoi, 40, 207220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Subiaul, F., Vonk, J., Okamoto-Barth, S., & Barth, J. (2008). Do chimpanzees learn reputation by observation? Evidence from direct and indirect experience with generous and selfish strangers. Animal Cognition, 11(4), 611623.Google Scholar
Surian, L., & Franchin, L. (2020). On the domain specificity of the mechanisms underpinning spontaneous anticipatory looks in false-belief tasks. Developmental Science, 23, e12955.Google Scholar
Téglás, E., Gergely, A., Kupán, K., Miklósi, Á., & Topál, J. (2012). Dogs’ gaze following is tuned to human communicative signals. Current Biology, 22, 209212.Google Scholar
Tempelmann, S., Kaminski, J., & Liebal, K. (2011). Focus on the essential: All great apes know when others are being attentive. Animal Cognition, 14, 433439.Google Scholar
Theall, L., & Povinelli, D. (1999). Do chimpanzees tailor their gestural signals to fit the attentional state of others? Animal Cognition, 2, 207214.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1999). The human adaptation for culture. Annual Review of Anthropology, 28, 509529.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (1997). Primate cognition. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (2008). Assessing the validity of ape–human comparisons: A reply to Boesch (2007). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 122, 449452.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Call, J., & Hare, B. (1998). Five primate species follow the visual gaze of conspecifics. Animal Behaviour, 55, 10631069.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Call, J., & Hare, B. (2003). Chimpanzees understand psychological states – The question is which ones and to what extent. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 153156.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2005). Intention reading and imitative learning. Perspectives on Imitation: From Neuroscience to Social Science, 2, 133148.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 675691.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Haberl, K. (2003). Understanding attention: 12- and 18-month-olds know what is new for other persons. Undefined. /paper/Understanding-attention%3A-12-and-18-month-olds-know-Tomasello-Haberl/a3831c9014190b01f27907df20c5fc74ada9c1bcGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Hare, B., & Agnetta, B. (1999). Chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, follow gaze direction geometrically. Animal Behaviour, 58, 769777.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Hare, B., & Fogleman, T. (2001). The ontogeny of gaze following in chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, and rhesus macaques, Macaca mulatta. Animal Behaviour, 61(2), 335343.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Hare, B., Lehmann, H., & Call, J. (2007). Reliance on head versus eyes in the gaze following of great apes and human infants: The cooperative eye hypothesis. Journal of Human Evolution, 52, 314320.Google Scholar
Uller, C. (2004). Disposition to recognize goals in infant chimpanzees. Animal Cognition, 7, 154161.Google Scholar
Ullman, T. D., Baker, C. L., Macindoe, O., Evans, O., Goodman, N. D., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2009). Help or hinder: Bayesian models of social goal inference. MIT Press.Google Scholar
van de Waal, E., Claidière, N., & Whiten, A. (2015). Wild vervet monkeys copy alternative methods for opening an artificial fruit. Animal Cognition, 18, 617627.Google Scholar
van de Waal, E., & Whiten, A. (2012). Spontaneous emergence, imitation and spread of alternative foraging techniques among groups of vervet monkeys. PLoS ONE, 7, e47008.Google Scholar
Vick, S.-J., & Anderson, J. R. (2003). Use of human visual attention cues by olive baboons (Papio anubis) in a competitive task. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 117(2), 209216.Google Scholar
Voelkl, B., & Huber, L. (2000). True imitation in marmosets. Animal Behaviour, 60, 195202.Google Scholar
Völter, C. J., Mundry, R., Call, J., & Seed, A. M. (2019). Chimpanzees flexibly update working memory contents and show susceptibility to distraction in the self-ordered search task. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 286, 20190715.Google Scholar
Völter, C. J., Tinklenberg, B., Call, J., & Seed, A. M. (2018). Comparative psychometrics: Establishing what differs is central to understanding what evolves. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373, 20170283.Google Scholar
Warneken, F., Hare, B., Melis, A. P., Hanus, D., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Spontaneous altruism by chimpanzees and young children. PLOS Biology, 5, e184.Google Scholar
Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees. Science, 311, 13011303.Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72, 655684.Google Scholar
Whiten, A. (1994). Grades of mindreading. In Lewis, C. & Mitchell, P., P. (Eds.), Children’s early understanding of mind: Origins and development (pp. 4794). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Whiten, A. (1996). When does smart behaviour reading become mindreading? In Carruthers, P. & Smith, P. K. (Eds.), Theories of theories of mind (pp. 277292). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Whiten, A. (2013). Humans are not alone in computing how others see the world. Animal Behaviour, 86, 213221.Google Scholar
Whiten, A, & Byrne, R. (1988). Tactical deception in primates. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11, 233244.Google Scholar
Whiten, A., & Byrne, R. W. (1997). Machiavellian intelligence II: Extensions and evaluations. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wilkinson, A., Mandl, I., Bugnyar, T., & Huber, L. (2010). Gaze following in the red-footed tortoise (Geochelone carbonaria). Animal Cognition, 13, 765769.Google Scholar
Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103128.Google Scholar
Wobber, V., Wrangham, R., & Hare, B. (2010). Bonobos exhibit delayed development of social behavior and cognition relative to chimpanzees. Current Biology, 20, 226230.Google Scholar
Wood, J. N., Glynn, D. D., Phillips, B. C., & Hauser, M. D. (2007). The perception of rational, goal-directed action in nonhuman primates. Science, 317, 14021405.Google Scholar
Woodward, A. L. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor’s reach. Cognition, 69(1), 134.Google Scholar
Wrangham, R. (2009). Catching fire: How cooking made us human. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Yamamoto, S., Humle, T., & Tanaka, M. (2009). Chimpanzees help each other upon request. PLoS ONE, 4, e7416.Google Scholar
Yamamoto, S., Humle, T., & Tanaka, M. (2012). Chimpanzees’ flexible targeted helping based on an understanding of conspecifics’ goals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 35883592.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×