Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-p2v8j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-13T07:31:31.398Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 1 - Exercise Addiction

Evolution and Challenges for Its Recognition as a Clinical Disorder

from Section 1 - From Exercise to Addiction: An Introduction to the Phenomenon

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 March 2023

Ornella Corazza
Affiliation:
University of Hertfordshire and University of Trento, Italy
Artemisa Rocha Dores
Affiliation:
Polytechnic Institute of Porto and University of Porto, Portugal

Summary

The potential negative effects of exercise addiction (EA) were first reported over 50 years ago, but it has only recently been formally recognized as a disorder in the leading clinical manuals. The inclusion of exercise behaviour as a potentially addictive behaviour will require greater consensus on how to define this disorder, with diagnostic criteria and course descriptions clearly supported by scientific evidence, and on how to categorize it in relation to other mental disorders. This chapter presents an overview of attempts to identify the defining features of EA, the development of instruments to measure it, estimates of its prevalence, and the main strategies for treating it. The diverse terminology used to describe this disorder reflects both the range of perspectives from which it has been examined, and the different manifestations of EA. The chapter concludes by recognizing that the development and validation of specific diagnostic criteria for EA pose many challenges.

Type
Chapter
Information
The Body in the Mind
Exercise Addiction, Body Image and the Use of Enhancement Drugs
, pp. 1 - 24
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2023

1.1 Introduction

Extensive research has shown that exercise has health benefits for everyone [Reference Ashdown-Franks, Firth and Carney1]. However, studies have also suggested that a small minority of individuals may experience negative physical and psychological effects, as they may become addicted to exercise and crave it in a similar way to those who are addicted to substances such as alcohol, nicotine, or other drugs [Reference Davis2,Reference Szabo, Demetrovics, Griffiths, Budde and Wegner3]. Although the possible negative effects of exercise addiction (EA) were first indicated more than 50 years ago [Reference Baekeland4Reference Fernandez, Kuss and Griffiths7], to date it has never received formal recognition as a mental disorder in the leading clinical manuals [8,9]. In 2013, the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) [8] incorporated gambling disorder along with substance-related disorders, so the title of this chapter in Section II was changed from ‘Substance-Related Disorders’ to ‘Substance-Related and Addictive Disorders’. This new classification appears to be based on new evidence which suggests that addiction is a disorder of the brain’s reward system, regardless of whether the system is activated by a behaviour or a substance [Reference Grant, Potenza, Weinstein and Gorelick10Reference Sussman and Sussman12].

The DSM-5 only includes gambling as a behavioural addiction, as well as criteria for internet gaming disorder (IGD) as a behavioural addiction within Section III (‘Emerging Measures and Models’). This decision was based on the Substance Use Disorder Workgroup’s conclusion that there was insufficient evidence to warrant the full inclusion of IGD as an official mental disorder in Section II. However, the new conceptualization of the diagnosis of addiction opens the door for research into other forms of excessive behaviour that can be potentially addictive. Furthermore, another group of repetitive behaviours, including exercise, was not included due to the lack of scientific evidence for establishing the diagnostic criteria and course descriptions needed to identify these behaviours as mental disorders [8]. Therefore, at the time of writing, EA should be considered as a potentially addictive behaviour and not a behavioural addiction – that is, a behavioural condition with a psychiatric entity that requires a diagnosis [Reference Spada13].

The future incorporation of exercise behaviour as an addiction appears to be contingent on the scientific community reaching some degree of consensus on two important questions that research has been addressing in recent decades [Reference Grant and Chamberlain14]. First, there is a need to define the phenomenon of EA and establish a clear rationale, supported by sufficient scientific evidence, for the mechanism by which exercise can be shown to be an addiction. Second, an evidence base is needed to determine how to categorize EA in relation to other possible mental disorders, and whether EA should be considered a distinct disorder. Logistically, only when the first question has been answered will we know the relevance of the second one. If no evidence can be found that exercise may have negative consequences additional to those caused by overtraining, there would be no need to consider exercise as a potential addiction or the processes relating to the development and maintenance of EA and its relationship with other mental disorders. This chapter describes in detail how these issues have been addressed over the past 50 years, and shows that although there is some consensus on the potential harm that can be caused by exercise, there is no agreement about the criteria and the mechanism of action by which this behaviour can become addictive. This lack of unanimity has limited the definition, measurement, and treatment of EA.

1.2 The Paradox of Exercise Addiction

The overwhelming evidence for the positive effects of exercise has led to campaigns to promote exercise among the general population since the mid-twentieth century. These exercise promotion policies must be understood in the context of the development of welfare states, which have been implemented in developed countries since the Second World War [Reference Harvey, Beamish and Defrance15]. However, as a larger percentage of the global population has started to exercise regularly, awareness that exercise can also have detrimental effects on some individuals has increased.

The debate about the harmful effects of exercise – and the fact that some people can become addicted to exercise and crave it in a similar way to those who are addicted to psychoactive substances such as alcohol, nicotine, or other drugs [Reference Davis2] – took place in North America in the late 1960s and the 1970s. Darcy Plymire reported a 10-fold increase in the number of regular runners within just one decade in North America in the 1970s [Reference Plymire16]. However, what Plymire found most striking was not the notable rise in the number of individuals exercising, but the fact that many of these runners reported running for longer than the average time recommended by institutions for achieving exercise-related health benefits (e.g., improvement in aerobic fitness and cardiac health). This finding suggested that there had to be motivations other than the health benefits highlighted by institutions for spending so much time on this activity. In the context of this debate, the question arises as to when exercise can be considered to be intense and prolonged.

Some scholars have argued that the motivation for runners to engage in long-term activity was more about pursuing human potential or what Abraham Maslow called ‘self-actualization’ [Reference Maslow17]. In fact, in line with positive psychology, terms such as ‘optimal experience’ or ‘peak moments’ have been used to describe the positive states of consciousness that many people with a regular exercise regime claim to experience during practice or competition [Reference Kimiecik, Jackson and Horn18,Reference McInman and Grove19]. However, in his book titled Positive Addiction, William Glasser proposed that a long-distance run of more than an hour could produce a state of euphoria and expansion of the mind which he called ‘positive addiction’ [Reference Glasser20]. By coining this term, Glasser wanted to highlight the beneficial effects of exercise, in contrast to addiction to other behaviours that could have negative consequences.

The use of the term ‘addiction’ in relation to a behaviour such as exercise, which has long been considered a positive activity, has been controversial. However, Glasser connected exercising for long periods with withdrawal symptoms, and therefore described it as a positive addiction [Reference Glasser20]. He formulated his hypothesis by studying individuals who practised transcendental meditation. People who engaged in this practice reported an altered state of consciousness that had allowed them to make decisive choices and improve their lives. If they did not exercise, they reported experiencing withdrawal symptoms such as depression, anxiety, and guilt. Glasser studied the psychology of running, and found that many runners fitted the criteria for positive addiction.

Glasser’s conceptualization of EA as a positive addiction [Reference Glasser20] was soon called into question. In fact, some researchers had already pointed out at the end of the 1960s that some exercisers continued to train despite the contraindications they showed for doing so [Reference Little5]. Baekeland reinforced this observation by indicating the difficulty he had experienced in recruiting athletes to an experiment that attempted to assess the effects of exercise deprivation on sleep [Reference Baekeland4]. Further support came from some psychiatric case studies reported by Morgan, who found that physical exercise could lead not only to physical injury but also to the serious neglect of daily work and/or family responsibilities [Reference Morgan6]. Morgan recognized the existence of negative effects of EA, warning about its dangers [Reference Morgan6]. Thereafter, negative factors associated with exercise, such as injury, overtraining, and psychological dysfunction, were progressively identified and included in the conceptualization of EA [Reference Adams21,Reference Allegre, Souville, Therme and Griffiths22]. Although the positive effects of exercise are still recognized today, there is a consensus that excessive and uncontrolled exercise can have negative consequences for a minority of individuals.

1.3 Criteria for Exercise Addiction

Historically, the debate about EA was focused on long-distance running, specifically because of the amount of time that some individuals spent exercising [Reference Plymire16,Reference Estok and Rudy23]. Unsurprisingly, therefore, the first signs of EA to be addressed were those that defined the characteristics of exercise (e.g., frequency, duration, intensity). Possible harmful effects of addictive exercise were identified, such as biomedical factors (e.g., withdrawal symptoms, tolerance), mental health problems (e.g., depression, anxiety), and social consequences (e.g., neglect of work, family, or other social obligations).

In view of the evidence of various features and consequences of problematic exercise, there is now reasonable consensus that EA, like other substance addictions, should be defined as a set of cognitive, behavioural, physiological, and psychological characteristics [Reference Hausenblas and Symons-Downs24,Reference Hausenblas and Symons-Downs25]. Research has shown such close similarities between addiction to behaviours (such as exercise) and drug dependence [Reference Davis2,Reference Sussman and Sussman12] that EA has been defined as a strong desire for physical activity, to the extent that it may involve a lack of control which is manifested as physiological characteristics (e.g., tolerance, withdrawal symptoms) and/or mental health problems (e.g. anxiety, depression) [Reference Hausenblas and Symons-Downs24,Reference Chapman and De Castro26]. However, despite the consensus on the multidimensionality of EA, the criteria for its diagnosis are less clear. Several strategies have been adopted to identify the features that define EA, four of which are highlighted here.

The first strategy examined the features of EA inductively, through the informed response of individuals who exercise regularly. For example, after interviewing 56 adult female exercisers, Bamber et al. suggested that EA has two essential features: (i) impaired psychological, social, physical, and/or behavioural functioning of the individual and (ii) lack of abstinence or the experience of withdrawal symptoms, related to a reluctance to modify the type of exercise or an inability to reduce the amount of exercise [Reference Bamber, Cockerill, Rodgers and Carroll27]. In addition, Bamber et al. considered a number of features that may be indicative of EA, such as exercising in secret or lying about the amount of exercise performed, denial that there is a problem with exercise, exercising alone, and the development of tolerance (i.e., increasing volume, frequency, and/or intensity of exercise required over time) [Reference Bamber, Cockerill, Rodgers and Carroll27]. This strategy may be of interest if it is assumed that addictions may have multiple and heterogeneous characteristics and comorbidity [Reference Billieux, Schimmenti, Khazaal, Maurage and Heeren28], as it would then be important to identify the specific characteristics that distinguish EA from other types of behavioural addictions.

The second strategy involves identifying features of EA and relating these to substance dependence. In this context, research has focused on defining EA based on the criteria for substance use that are listed in some of the leading clinical manuals. For example, Hausenblas et al. operationalized EA as a multidimensional maladaptive pattern leading to a clinically significant disability or affliction and manifested by the presence of at least three of the following seven criteria that are listed in the fourth edition of the DSM [Reference Hausenblas and Symons-Downs24]: (i) tolerance, defined either as a need to increase the amount of exercise to achieve the intended effect, or a decrease in the effect with continued use of the same amount of exercise; (ii) withdrawal, manifested by either withdrawal symptoms when unable to exercise (e.g., anxiety, moodiness, irritability), or requiring the same (or a similar) amount of exercise to relieve or avoid withdrawal symptoms; (iii) intention effects – engaging in larger amounts of exercise, or exercising over a longer time period; (iv) lack of control – a persistent desire or unsuccessful attempts to reduce or control the amount of exercise; (v) time – a large amount of time is spent engaged in activities involving exercise; (vi) reduction in other activities – social, occupational, and/or recreational activities are reduced or stopped in order to exercise instead; (vii) continuance – exercising continually despite awareness that this is causing a persistent psychological or physical problem [29].

The third strategy focuses on adapting the criteria defined for behavioural addictions to the context of exercise. This type of strategy assumes that there are common components that all behavioural addictions share. On the basis of this assumption, Sussman and Sussman conducted an extensive review of the literature on addiction and, after identifying common elements, highlighted the following five essential characteristics that EA would share with any type of behavioural addiction: (i) a high level of commitment to the behaviour in order to obtain the desired effects that it produces, rather than because of the activity itself; (ii) the activity occupies an important place in the individual’s life and is prioritized over other activities; (iii) the satisfaction that is experienced during or at the end of the activity leads to a distraction from life’s problems, and generates a feeling of happiness; (iv) a lack of control over decision-making about when to stop the activity; (v) the behaviour has negative consequences, which may vary depending on the type of addictive behaviour, but in exercise become more evident over time as individuals cease engaging in other activities and/or spending time with others (e.g., partner, family) [Reference Sussman and Sussman12]. Following this approach, research by Carnes (cited by Griffiths [Reference Griffiths11]) compared the signs that could be shared by behavioural addiction and substance use disorder, highlighting elements such as a pattern of uncontrolled behaviour, unsuccessful attempts to limit that behaviour, use of the behaviour as an escape mechanism, significant mood changes, and a reduction in other life activities as a consequence of the behaviour [Reference Griffiths11]. Other authors, such as Brown [Reference Brown, Eadington and Cornelius30] and Griffiths [Reference Griffiths31Reference Griffiths, Szabo and Terry35], view addictions as comprising several core components. The ‘addiction components model’ proposed by Griffiths [Reference Griffiths33] has been used to define EA according to six core criteria for behavioural addiction: (i) salience (exercise becomes the most important activity and dominates the other areas of the individual’s life); (ii) mood modification (a subjective mood-altering experience is reported as a consequence of engaging in exercise); (iii) tolerance (a tendency to need to increase the amount of exercise in order to experience the desired mood-modifying effects); (iv) withdrawal (an unpleasant feeling caused by stopping or drastically reducing exercise); (v) conflict (exercise being prioritized over other educational and/or occupational activities and interpersonal relationships, and intra-psychic conflict, where the individual knows they should cut down the amount of exercise they are doing but is unable to do so, and experiences a subjective loss of control; (vi) relapse (a tendency to repeat the same exercise patterns after a certain time without engaging in exercise).

The fourth strategy involves trying to identify and define features of EA in relation to other associated disorders. The most important example of this strategy is represented by the view that EA is primarily a behaviour used to maintain body weight and shape (e.g., to lose weight) [Reference Meyer, Taranis, Goodwin and Haycraft36Reference McCabe and Vincent39]. Although the authors of these studies suggest that EA has similar features to substance use disorder and other forms of behavioural addiction (e.g., withdrawal symptoms), they acknowledge that additional features need to be defined to characterize EA in terms of the main disorder from which it is derived. Hence the use of exercise as a form of weight control, the rigid behavioural pattern, and the positive reinforcement of exercise by its effect on mood are all specific features that have been described for EA associated with eating disorders [Reference Meyer, Taranis, Goodwin and Haycraft36,Reference Taranis, Touyz and Meyer37]. However, as EA may have high comorbidity and might also be associated with disorders other than eating disorders, it is likely that new specific features will be needed to define EA in relation to these additional disorders. This diversity of features essentially reflects the different ways in which EA can be understood and defined, and highlights the ongoing debate about the relationship between EA and other already recognized disorders.

1.4 The Relationship between Exercise Addiction and Other Mental Disorders

One key question that must be addressed before EA is recognized as a disorder is its relationship to other mental disorders. Underlying this question is the need to clarify whether EA can be considered a distinct disorder – that is, whether the problems associated with EA are due to the behaviour itself, or to other associated disorders [Reference Veale, Annett, Cripps and Steinberg40,Reference Yates, Leehey and Shisslak41]. It is essential to clarify this in order to enable future categorization of EA within the broader spectrum of mental disorders.

A key point in this debate is the distinction that Veale made between primary and secondary forms of EA [Reference Veale42]. EA is classified as primary when it represents a behavioural addiction in itself, whereas it is considered to be secondary when it co-occurs with another disorder, and according to the literature, secondary EA has generally been identified with an eating disorder, such as anorexia or bulimia nervosa [Reference Veale, Annett, Cripps and Steinberg40,Reference Veale42]. In a primary addiction, the purpose of excessive exercise is to avoid something negative, although the affected individual may be unaware of this motivation. Here exercise provides a way to escape from a disturbing, persistent, and/or uncontrollable source of stress. However, in a secondary addiction, exercise is used as a means of achieving a goal that is characteristic of another dysfunction. For example, in individuals with anorexia nervosa, exercise may be used in addition to a strict diet as a means of losing weight.

The distinction between primary and secondary EA introduced by Veale has important clinical implications, as secondary EA would have a different aetiology to primary addiction, even though many symptoms of EA would be similar in both cases. The key feature suggested by Veale for distinguishing between the two forms of EA was that in primary addiction, exercise is the goal, whereas in secondary addiction, exercise is used to achieve another goal (e.g., weight loss), so exercise is only one of the possible means used to achieve this goal.

However, despite the clinical implications of this, there is currently no consensus on how to differentiate between primary and secondary EA. Although some authors assert that exercise may be a primary source of problems for some individuals [Reference Griffiths31], other authors maintain that primary EA has rarely been documented and is difficult to differentiate from problematic exercise associated with other disorders (e.g., eating disorders) [Reference Adams21,Reference Bamber, Cockerill, Rodgers and Carroll27,Reference Blaydon, Lindner and Kerr43,Reference Blaydon and Lindner44]. Several factors may have contributed to the lack of agreement on how to recognize and differentiate between primary and secondary EA.

First, exercise has traditionally been seen as a healthy behaviour, so its harmful effects are often more likely to be recognized when they are associated with other disorders (e.g., eating disorders) than in relation to exercise itself [Reference Johnston, Reilly and Kremer45]. Second, research has shown a relatively strong association between EA and eating disorders [Reference Alcaraz-Ibáñez, Paterna, Sicilia and Griffiths46Reference Trott, Jackson and Firth49]. For example, Shroff et al. found that among a group of women who met the criteria for a diagnosis of anorexia and/or bulimia, 39% over-exercised, and this percentage rose to 54% in the subgroup of women with the purging subtype of anorexia nervosa [Reference Shroff, Reba and Thornton50]. Similarly, Klein et al. found that 48% of the women who were being treated for anorexia nervosa in their study were at risk of EA [Reference Klein, Bennett and Schebendach51]. A recent systematic review and meta-analysis found that the effect size of the relationship between EA and eating disorders is larger in clinical populations [Reference Alcaraz-Ibáñez, Paterna, Sicilia and Griffiths46], and it has been suggested that the risk of EA among individuals with eating disorders is more than 3.5-fold higher than that for individuals without an eating disorder [Reference Trott, Jackson and Firth49].

Finally, a number of studies have understood the difference between primary and secondary addiction as being based on eating disorders alone [Reference Bamber, Cockerill, Rodgers and Carroll27,Reference Blaydon and Lindner44,Reference Zmijewski and Howard52,Reference Maselli, Gobbi, Probst and Carraro53]. These studies generally define primary EA in terms of the absence of an eating disorder. This view may have been influenced by the fact that Veale himself initially distinguished between primary and secondary dependence mainly with reference to the absence of an eating disorder. In 1987, he stated that ‘A distinction should be made between primary exercise dependence and exercise dependence which is secondary to an eating disorder. A diagnostic hierarchy occurs in a case of exercise dependence, whereby the diagnosis of Anorexia Nervosa should be first excluded, followed by Bulimia Nervosa. A diagnosis of primary exercise dependence should only then be made’ [Reference Veale42, p. 737].

However, a definition of EA based on the absence of eating disorders does not allow for the possibility that other disorders might be found to be associated with addiction to exercise. It is possible that secondary EA may be associated with other disorders as well as those related to eating. Subsequent research has indeed shown that eating disorders are only one type of disorder associated with EA. For example, in 1995, Veale established the operational diagnostic criteria for primary exercise dependence, and among them he highlighted the criterion that ‘the preoccupation with exercise is not better accounted for by another mental disorder (e.g., as a means of losing weight or controlling calorie intake as in an eating disorder)’ [Reference Veale, Annett, Cripps and Steinberg40, p. 2]. Therefore Veale’s distinction between primary and secondary EA appears to be more nuanced than the interpretation that has sometimes been conveyed in the literature.

To date, there has been little explicit recognition that a secondary form of EA may exist related to disorders other than eating disorders. For example, although Blaydon et al. defended the idea that EA is always secondary, they recognized that it may be linked either to a form of eating disorder or to excessive concern about body image [Reference Blaydon, Lindner and Kerr54]. In the same vein, McCabe and Vincent argued that exercise, together with dieting, is one of the most common ways of modifying body size and shape [Reference McCabe and Vincent39]. However, they understood that excessive exercise should be studied not only in relation to eating disorders, but also in relation to other disorders linked to modification of body size and shape. Exercise, together with control of nutrition, may be used to achieve specific body ideals (e.g., a muscular, thin, lean body) [Reference Tod and Edwards55,Reference Tod, Edwards and Hall56]. Research has indicated associations between EA and body dysmorphic disorder [Reference Corazza, Simonato and Demetrovics57], and it has been suggested that muscle dysmorphia could be reclassified as an addiction to body image [Reference Foster, Shorter and Griffiths58]. In addition, EA has been associated with other potential disorders related to body care and a concern about health, such as orthorexia nervosa [Reference Oberle, Watkins and Burkot59,Reference Cena, Barthels and Cuzzolaro60]. Finally, some studies have found an association between behavioural addictions and other disorders, such as bipolar disorder [Reference Di Nicola, Tedeschi and Mazza61,Reference Di Nicola, Martinotti and Mazza62]. Therefore classifications that establish a prevalence of primary addiction in individuals who do not present with a secondary exercise dependence associated with an eating disorder should be avoided, because they do not take into account the possibility that these individuals may have another associated mental disorder. This distinction has important practical and interventional implications, and should therefore be considered when defining what exactly authors consider to be EA.

1.5 Instruments for Assessing Exercise Addiction

To date, several self-report instruments have been designed to assess EA. In parallel with the evolution of the concept, the earliest instruments were one-dimensional assessments and generally referred to a specific activity. One of the first was the Commitment to Running Scale, in which EA was conceptualized as lying at one end of a continuum of exercise characterized by a strong commitment to running [Reference Carmack and Martens63]. This scale was subsequently revised so that it could be applied to commitment to exercise more generally [Reference Corbin, Nielsen, Borsdorf and Laurie64]. Other instruments that applied to running, such as the Obligatory Running Questionnaire [Reference Blumenthal, Toole and Jonathan65], were based on the view that running addiction is a compulsive activity that shares psychological and behavioural symptoms with those seen in patients with anorexia nervosa. The Obligatory Running Questionnaire was later adapted so that it could be applied to exercise more generally, in the form of the Obligatory Exercise Questionnaire [Reference Pasman and Thompson66]. As the harmful consequences of exercise became more widely acknowledged, new scales such as the Running Addiction Scale [Reference Chapman and De Castro26] and the Negative Addiction Scale [Reference Hailey and Bailey67] were developed to assess both running addiction and general EA. However, these one-dimensional measures assessed only specific aspects of addiction, and did not provide a more comprehensive assessment of the construct [Reference Hausenblas and Symons-Downs24].

Subsequently, multidimensional approaches have been developed that have drawn parallels between EA and substance addictions, and consequently defined EA as a varied set of symptoms [Reference Hausenblas and Symons-Downs25,Reference Freimuth, Moniz and Kim68]. For instance, Davis et al. developed the Commitment to Exercise Scale [Reference Davis, Brewer and Ratusny69] after examining several published case studies of men and women with clear pathological or excessive exercise habits. The instrument has a two-factor, eight-item structure, and evaluates both the compulsive nature of exercise (e.g., feelings of guilt about missing a training session) and its pathological aspects (e.g., continuing to exercise despite illness or injuries).

Another early multidimensional instrument was the Exercise Dependence Questionnaire (EDQ), developed by Ogden et al. [Reference Ogden, Veale and Summers70]. The authors based their conceptualization of problematic exercise on several criteria for substance dependence included in the DSM-IV, as well as on motivational factors (e.g., physical health, psychological health). Therefore, the EDQ conceptualizes EA as a combination of traditional elements of addiction (e.g., tolerance, withdrawal, repetitive behaviour), but also recognizes the psychosocial aspects of the problem (e.g., effects on interpersonal relationships). However, all of the measures that have been discussed so far lacked a cut-off criterion that could classify individuals at risk of addiction as symptomatic or asymptomatic.

According to recent reviews [Reference Colledge, Buchner, Schmidt and Walter71,Reference Marques, Peralta and Sarmento72] the three most widely used instruments are the Exercise Dependence Scale (EDS) [Reference Hausenblas and Symons-Downs24], the Exercise Addiction Inventory (EAI) [Reference Terry, Szabo and Griffiths34], and the Compulsive Exercise Test (CET) [Reference Taranis, Touyz and Meyer37]. These three instruments, unlike previous ones, have established cut-off points for classifying individuals at risk of exercise as symptomatic or asymptomatic. However, there are differences between these instruments with regard to the conceptualization of EA.

The EDS [Reference Hausenblas and Symons-Downs24] defines EA in terms of the DSM-IV criteria for substance dependence [29]. This instrument has undergone a revision process [Reference Symons-Downs, Hausenblas and Nigg73], and its latest version contains a total of 21 items, comprising seven factors: (i) tolerance, (ii) withdrawal, (iii) intention effects, (iv) lack of control, (v) time, (vi) reduction in other activities, and (vii) continuance. Each of the subscales is represented by three items, and respondents are asked to indicate their response using a Likert scale ranging from 1 (never) to 6 (always). By operationalizing EA according to the seven criteria set out in the DSM-IV, the EDS provides information on the average of each of the factors or the average of the total score. Considering the first option, the EDS allows the differentiation of individuals into three groups, namely those who are at risk of addiction (i.e., with scores of 5–6 on the Likert scale for at least three of the seven criteria), those who are symptomatic (i.e., with scores of 3–4 on the Likert scale for at least three criteria, or scores of 5–6 combined with scores of 3–4 for three criteria, but without meeting the conditions for being at risk), and those who are asymptomatic (i.e., with scores of 1–2 on the Likert scale for at least three criteria, but without meeting the conditions for being symptomatic). The structure of the EDS has been validated in a number of different countries [Reference Lindwall and Palmeira74Reference Costa77]. Although the EDS has a sound theoretical basis in that it utilizes the symptoms of dependence according to the criteria established in the DSM-IV, given the time it takes to administer the instrument and the complexity of the calculations necessary to identify individuals at risk of addiction, it is arguably not practicable for use in the daily work of sports doctors, physiotherapists, occupational therapists, and sports science professionals.

Unlike the EDS, the EAI [Reference Terry, Szabo and Griffiths34,Reference Griffiths, Szabo and Terry35] is an abbreviated and practical instrument for assessing the risk of EA. It operationalizes EA on the basis of the components of behavioural addictions [Reference Griffiths11,Reference Griffiths31Reference Griffiths33], which is more in line with the new classification offered by the DSM-5. In the EAI, the symptoms of EA are operationalized through six components of behavioural addiction defined by Griffiths, namely salience, mood modification, tolerance, withdrawal symptoms, conflict, and relapse [Reference Griffiths33]. Each of the six items of the instrument reflects a component of addiction. In addition, based on the total score of its items, utilizing a five-point Likert scale, the EAI serves as a screening tool that can distinguish between individuals who are at risk of EA (i.e., scores of 24 or higher), have some symptoms (scores between 13 and 23), or have no symptoms of addiction (scores between 0 and 12). In a recent update of the EAI, a modification to the item response rating and a new cut-off point (≥29) for identifying individuals at risk of EA were established [Reference Szabo, Pinto, Griffiths, Kovácsik and Demetrovics78].

Finally, the CET [Reference Taranis, Touyz and Meyer37] is based on a cognitive–behavioural conceptualization of EA, viewing it primarily as a weight control behaviour that is maintained by concerns about body weight and body shape [Reference Meyer, Taranis, Goodwin and Haycraft36]. This instrument was specifically designed for use within the eating disorders domain. After the authors had examined the functioning of an initial pool of 31 items through three studies involving independent samples of women, they proposed a final model consisting of 24 items grouped into five factors: (i) avoidance and rule-driven behaviour; (ii) weight control exercise; (iii) mood improvement; (iv) lack of exercise enjoyment; and (v) exercise rigidity. Subsequent studies have reviewed the use of the instrument among different populations [Reference Meyer, Taranis, Goodwin and Haycraft36,Reference Meyer, Plateau and Taranis79], including athletes [Reference Plateau, Shanmugam and Duckham80]. Initially the authors did not propose cut-off points for the CET, but in a subsequent review of a sample of women with clinical eating disorders, Meyer et al. proposed a cut-off point (score of 15) to identify compulsive exercise among patients with eating disorders [Reference Meyer, Plateau and Taranis79].

Although the EDS, EAI, and CET are currently the most commonly used psychometric instruments in EA research, the possibility cannot be ruled out that new instruments will be developed that will define EA according to criteria in newer editions of the clinical manuals (e.g., DSM-5), or in relation to other disorders as well as eating disorders. Moreover, with a few exceptions, such as the EAI [Reference Griffiths, Urbán and Demetrovics81], there is a lack of evidence for the instruments’ validity and reliability, which would enable them to be used in different countries, cultures, and populations of people who exercise. This has made it difficult to compare the results of studies conducted in different settings and countries, including research in which a range of different instruments have been used to assess EA prevalence.

1.6 Prevalence of Exercise Addiction

The range of different criteria used to define EA, and the diversity of instruments used to assess it, have prevented an accurate estimate of its prevalence. A large number of studies indicate that the prevalence of EA is fairly low, ranging from 0.3% in the general population to 3% in populations who exercise regularly [Reference Allegre, Souville, Therme and Griffiths22,Reference Griffiths33,Reference Symons-Downs, Hausenblas and Nigg73,Reference Mónok, Berczik and Urbán82]. However, a few studies have reported EA prevalence rates of over 40% [Reference Blaydon and Lindner44,Reference Lejoyeux, Avril, Richoux, Embouazza and Nivoli83,Reference Serier, Smith and Lash84] – that is, five to ten times higher than the prevalence rates reported in most studies. The large discrepancy in EA prevalence may be related to three issues that have already been mentioned in this chapter.

First, there is no consensus on a precise conceptualization of the phenomenon. Therefore, as has been highlighted by Sicilia et al. [Reference Sicilia, Paterna, Alcaraz-Ibáñez and Griffiths85], psychometric assessment tools are based on significant conceptual differences with regard to EA. For example, the early instruments, which use the term ‘commitment’ [Reference Carmack and Martens63,Reference Corbin, Nielsen, Borsdorf and Laurie64,Reference Davis, Brewer and Ratusny69], conceptualize EA only as one end of the exercise continuum. However, as Szabo et al. have warned, the literature may include many studies of EA that measured excessive commitment to this behaviour but which did not assess negative consequences for the individual [Reference Szabo, Frenkl and Caputo86]. On the other hand, the fact that instruments with different conceptualizations assess different features of EA, can lead to differences between population groups. For instance, Weik and Hale assessed EA among people who exercised at a sports centre [Reference Weik and Hale87], using the Exercise Dependence Scale-Revised [Reference Symons-Downs, Hausenblas and Nigg73] and the Exercise Dependence Questionnaire [Reference Ogden, Veale and Summers70]. The results were interesting, indicating that when the EDS-R was used, men scored higher than women on abstinence, continuation, tolerance, lack of control, time, desired effects, and the total dependency value, whereas when the EDQ was used, women scored higher. These types of results show that the EDQ and the EDS-R assess different aspects of EA and classify either men or women as more addicted, depending on the instrument used and the dimension highlighted. Therefore it is logical to assume that instruments designed according to different conceptualizations of EA will show differences in the prevalence of EA [Reference Mónok, Berczik and Urbán82], and systematic reviews that compare the prevalence of EA using instruments with different theoretical conceptualizations should take this fact into account.

Second, when a particular instrument has been modified or used differently, the reported prevalence is no longer informative. For example, Lejoyeux et al. reported an EA prevalence of 42% among a sample of fitness centre users [Reference Lejoyeux, Avril, Richoux, Embouazza and Nivoli83]. However, as the authors themselves acknowledged, they had designed a specific scale for this study according to the diagnostic criteria of Hausenblas and Giacobbi [Reference Hausenblas and Giacobbi88], which may explain the discrepancy between their findings and the prevalence reported with other instruments, such as the EDS. Similarly, Serier et al. found an EA prevalence of 41.7% in a group of women with unequivocal body dissatisfaction [Reference Serier, Smith and Lash84]. However, although these authors assessed EA using the Obligatory Eating Questionnaire [Reference Pasman and Thompson66], for this study they arbitrarily created a cut-off point of above 50 to classify individuals as being at risk of EA. These practices may account for the significant discrepancy in the prevalence of EA even when it was assessed using the same instrument.

Third, the target populations in different studies of the prevalence of EA have varied widely, and have included, for example, runners, ultra-marathoners, cyclists, university students, university student subgroups (e.g., sports science students), bodybuilders, fitness centre users, and elite athletes. Associated with this wide range of samples, not only sports modalities but also the amount of time spent exercising by participants in the studies have varied widely. Due to this great diversity, the items on the assessment instruments have been interpreted in different ways. For example, Szabo et al. suggest that the high scores on psychometric instruments that are shown by elite athletes compared with other populations should not be attributed to higher morbidity, but interpreted in relation to a high level of commitment to their profession [Reference Szabo, Griffiths, de La Vega, Mervó and Demetrovics89]. Such differences in interpretation may well occur in relation to other population groups depending on, for example, gender or culture, and they highlight the need to provide evidence that instruments which are used to compare prevalence show measurement invariance across different population groups.

Finally, most studies of EA have been conducted using self-report assessment instruments. Consequently, the questionnaires and scales used have assessed the susceptibility to dysfunction in terms of the presence or intensity of symptoms associated with the addiction. These screening tools have limited diagnostic value, as a high score may not necessarily indicate harm or damage to the individual. If a person is identified as being at high risk for EA on the basis of psychometric instruments, this should be verified with a subsequent clinical interview and the use of scientifically validated criteria to confirm whether exercise behaviour has negative consequences for this individual. As has been highlighted by Szabo et al., there is a ‘grey area’ between the classification of being at risk and having a disorder, because the context of the individual and what exercise entails within that context is not known [Reference Szabo, Griffiths, de La Vega, Mervó and Demetrovics89]. For the analysis of this context the clinical interview is a more precise approach than a self-report scale, especially with regard to assessment of the possible harm and lack of control that the behaviour produces in an individual.

1.7 Working towards a Consensus on the Definition of Exercise Addiction

Since the research conducted in the late 1970s, there has been a continuous attempt to identify the negative aspects of physical exercise as opposed to its more familiar positive effects. During that time, many terms associated with EA have emerged, such as ‘exercise commitment’, ‘addictive exercise’, ‘abusive exercise’, ‘compulsive exercise’, ‘excessive exercise’, ‘exercise dependence’, and ‘obligatory exercise’ [Reference Hausenblas and Symons-Downs24,Reference Bamber, Cockerill, Rodgers and Carroll27,Reference Meyer, Taranis, Goodwin and Haycraft36,Reference Blumenthal, Toole and Jonathan65,Reference Pasman and Thompson66,Reference Davis, Brewer and Ratusny69]. These terms reflect the historical evolution of the phenomenon and the debates that have taken place around it [Reference Sicilia, Paterna, Alcaraz-Ibáñez and Griffiths85]. Despite their conceptual diversity, all of these terms highlight possible negative effects of physical exercise, and describe a condition in which the practice of moderate or intense exercise becomes a disruptive behaviour for the individual. In an attempt to focus more on this common characteristic than on the differences, the terms ‘problematic exercise’ and ‘morbid exercise’ have been introduced [Reference Szabo, Demetrovics, Griffiths, Budde and Wegner3,Reference Alcaraz-Ibáñez, Paterna, Sicilia and Griffiths46,Reference Chamberlain and Grant90]. These more generic terms refer to a form of exercise in which the individual loses control over their behaviour, and the behaviour is maintained despite the physical and psychological damage that it causes.

Despite the acceptance of the multidimensionality and complexity of EA, and the fact that this form of uncontrolled practice has negative consequences for the individual, the research findings have been somewhat ambiguous, due to the fact that to date the terminology of the constructs surrounding this phenomenon, their definition, and the measures designed to evaluate them have been so diverse. This presents a challenge for fundamental and applied research in the future, as the factors that prevail in, co-occur with, and perpetuate EA are still not entirely clear, making its prevention and treatment difficult.

1.8 Treatment of Exercise Addiction

Approaches to EA treatment often vary depending on whether the EA is viewed as primary or secondary. In cases of secondary addiction, treatment has been more frequent when EA has been associated with an eating disorder. Since eating disorders have long been recognized in the major diagnostic manuals (specifically the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, and the International Classification of Diseases), there is much more literature available on how to treat them. However, there is also a risk that when EA and an eating disorder co-occur, the eating disorder will be treated at the expense of EA, as has been pointed out by Freitmuth et al. [Reference Freimuth, Moniz and Kim68]. As eating disorders are the best known and officially recognized disorders, they may be the focus of treatment, and the problem of EA may remain hidden. Moreover, treatment for EA associated with other disorders and for primary EA is even more limited, since its prevalence is lower.

Since EA is not a recognized clinical disorder, one approach to looking for treatment strategies that may be effective has been to observe what specialists do in their daily practice. For example, Adams and Kirkby interviewed 24 sports physiotherapists to examine the problems they encountered and the strategies they used to treat patients with EA [Reference Adams and Kirkby91]. Re-education of the individual and especially their relationship with exercise can help to prevent the transformation of exercise activity from healthy to unhealthy. The authors highlighted the fact that many people who exercise are not aware that exercise is a problem [Reference Adams21,Reference Adams and Kirkby91], and consequently have difficulty recognizing and accepting a diagnosis of EA [Reference Johnston, Reilly and Kremer45]. This inability to accept that exercise has become problematic reduces the likelihood of treatment success. Education programmes generally involve providing effective information and also details about training programmes to enable the individual to learn how to differentiate between unhealthy exercise-related behaviour and healthy behaviour. These education programmes are advisable when unhealthy behaviour (e.g., overtraining) appears to be the result of a poor understanding of the negative health consequences of the indivduals’s actions [Reference Adams21]. However, in combination with re-education of exercisers, physiotherapists sometimes recommend alternative exercise activities, since by changing their exercise routine or alternating it with other types of exercise, people who exercise are sometimes able to modify their training schemes. One of the challenges for physiotherapists when implementing EA interventions is to differentiate between healthy and unhealthy exercise. Unlike other behaviours or substance intake, where engaging in the behaviour or consuming the substance do not bring any positive benefit, exercise is a type of behaviour that has proven health benefits when performed properly, and indeed is used as an adjunct for the treatment of other addictions and disorders, such as anxiety and stress disorders, schizophrenia, eating disorders, prenatal and postnatal depression, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, and substance use disorders [Reference Ashdown-Franks, Firth and Carney1,Reference Fernandez, Kuss and Griffiths7]. Research has shown that prolonged exercise withdrawal can have negative effects on mental health [Reference Ashdown-Franks, Firth and Carney1,Reference Weinstein, Koehmstedt and Kop92]. Therefore interventions should be focused not on the elimination of exercise behaviour, but on its control – that is, on re-education about its practice, where changing the activity or the way it is carried out can be an appropriate strategy.

Along with physiotherapists, other exercise-related professionals (e.g., trainers, instructors, personal trainers) may help to identify and attempt to prevent the negative consequences that exercise may have for some individuals. A survey by Colledge et al. showed that more than 70% of sports centre employees reported that they had observed at least one client whom they suspected of having an eating disorder or engaging in excessive exercise [Reference Colledge, Cody, Pühse and Gerber93]. In addition, these employees reported that they felt able to identify when excessive exercise might be linked to eating disorders. This suggests that professionals who guide or supervise the daily exercise of population groups could (with the help of guideline documents or in collaboration with associations and centres for the treatment of these disorders) offer qualified guidance and/or educational programmes for the re-education of individuals with suspected EA.

However, in cases where a combination of educational programmes and the proposal of alternative forms of exercise is unsuccessful, physiotherapists and other professionals should direct the exerciser towards psychological treatment. For example, EA treatment can be based on treatments for other addictions, even though the processes involved in the different forms and manifestations of EA have not yet been fully clarified. Two interventions derived from the treatment of other addictions, namely motivational interviewing and cognitive–behavioural therapy, appear promising when applied to EA. Based on the transtheoretical model of motivation [Reference Prochaska, Redding, Evers, Glanz, Rimer and Viswanath94], motivational interviewing techniques aim to generate an intrinsic motivation for change. Individuals move from a pre-contemplative phase, in which there is not yet an intention to initiate treatment, to a phase that is more committed to action and behaviour change. This intervention is based on the view that an individual who is not at an appropriate stage to change is unlikely to commit to treatment. The second type of intervention, cognitive–behavioural therapy, is an effective form of therapy that has been used in substance abuse and other behavioural addictions, such as gambling disorder [Reference Petry95]. Here the therapist encourages the exercise addict to identify and correct the automatic and irrational thought processes that lead to emotions and maladaptive behaviours for that individual. In this way, the therapist helps them to understand how their irrational thinking can result in uncontrollable over-exercise. Cognitive–behavioural therapy can be combined with contingency management, in which the client is regularly tested to ensure that they do not engage in exercise in an undesirable way, so that their behaviour change is associated with rewards [Reference Adams21].

1.9 Future Directions in Exercise Addiction

In general, EA can be understood as a person’s inner urge to exercise in an uncontrolled way, as they maintain the intention to exercise despite the harmful consequences of this behaviour [Reference Hausenblas and Symons-Downs24,Reference Griffiths31,Reference Veale42]. However, EA is manifested in various forms, and can occur in individuals who exercise in different ways and for different reasons. Studies that examine features which are characteristic of individuals who regularly exercise may be informative. However, one limitation is that the lack of clinical criteria for defining EA makes it difficult to identify individuals as clinically addicted to exercise. This has led to the identification of features not only in other behavioural addictions and substance use disorders, but also in other disorders that might be associated with EA. However, the diagnosis and treatment of EA are still difficult to implement, due to the lack of specific diagnostic criteria for this particular addiction. Several points relating to the future development of these criteria should be considered.

First, the concept of EA has evolved over the past few decades, and a more satisfactory definition is needed that includes other associated terms and concepts. Although problematic exercise has been widely associated with eating disorders, there is increasing evidence that underlying some forms of EA there may also be other potential disorders associated with body image [Reference Foster, Shorter and Griffiths58,Reference Oberle, Watkins and Burkot59,Reference Corazza, Simonato and Demetrovics96]. Therefore, the diversity of terms used to refer to problematic exercise may indicate that there are a number of different manifestations of EA. Therefore the development and validation of diagnostic criteria for EA will need to ensure that these are broad enough to capture the different features of all the possible manifestations of this disorder.

Second, although the multidimensional nature of EA is now well recognized, there is no consensus on the core criteria for this disorder. Furthermore, attempts to define addiction include different elements or dimensions that can occur separately depending not only on the type of addiction, but also on the personal characteristics and specific socio-cultural circumstances surrounding this behaviour. As a result, there has been a tendency to define this phenomenon as a set of elements when in fact each of those elements might represent different phenomena that do not usually occur together [Reference Sussman and Sussman12]. Some authors, such as Shaffer et al., have proposed alternative ways of thinking about this phenomenon, and suggested that addiction (which by definition could include EA) should be thought of as a syndrome that can have different forms of expression [Reference Shaffer, LaPlante and LaBrie97]. In this sense, a syndrome would allow EA to be thought of as a cluster of symptoms reflecting an abnormal underlying condition. Viewing EA as different clusters of symptoms would enable researchers in the field to explore this addiction as a broad family of different manifestations that can be individually distinguished by their specific combinations of factors, rather than as a single pathological phenomenon.

Finally, excessive exercising was conceptualized as a problem when the negative consequences of this behaviour for some individuals began to be investigated and assessed. If excessive exercise had no negative consequences, and the experience was always positive and beneficial in the ongoing development of the individual, it would not make sense to talk about addiction, at least in the sense in which this term is used to refer to other substances or behaviours [Reference Szabo, Demetrovics, Griffiths, Budde and Wegner3,Reference Szabo, Griffiths, de La Vega, Mervó and Demetrovics89,Reference Egorov and Szabo98]. Therefore, one future challenge will be to characterize the defining elements of EA. In this process, only elements that result in functional impairment, psychological distress, and/or a clear separation from normative behaviour in context should be considered [Reference Kardefelt-Winther, Heeren and Schimmenti99]. This will require a more detailed examination of precisely when the individual’s relationship to exercise behaviour becomes problematic. Similarly, although psychometric instruments can serve as a screening tool, context assessment is crucial when evaluating the harm that exercise causes in the individual’s life [Reference Johnston, Reilly and Kremer45,Reference Szabo, Griffiths, de La Vega, Mervó and Demetrovics89].

References

Ashdown-Franks, G, Firth, J, Carney, R et al. Exercise as medicine for mental and substance use disorders: a meta-review of the benefits for neuropsychiatric and cognitive outcomes. Sports Med 2020; 50: 151–70.Google Scholar
Davis, C. Exercise abuse. Int J Sport Psychol 2000; 31: 278–89.Google Scholar
Szabo, A, Demetrovics, Z, Griffiths, MD. Morbid exercise behavior: addiction or psychological escape? In: Budde, H, Wegner, M, eds. The Exercise Effect on Mental Health: Neurobiological Mechanisms. Routledge, 2018: 277311.Google Scholar
Baekeland, F. Exercise deprivation: sleep and psychological reactions. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1970; 22: 365–9.Google Scholar
Little, JC. Athletic neurosis: a deprivation crisis. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1969; 45: 187–97.Google Scholar
Morgan, WP. Negative addiction in runners. Phys Sportsmed 1979; 7: 5670.Google Scholar
Fernandez, DP, Kuss, DJ, Griffiths, MD. Short-term abstinence effects across potential behavioral addictions: a systematic review. Clin Psychol Rev 2020; 76: 101828.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed. American Psychiatric Association, 2013.Google Scholar
World Health Organization. ICD-11 for Mortality and Morbidity Statistics. World Health Organization, 2019.Google Scholar
Grant, JE, Potenza, MN, Weinstein, A, Gorelick, DA. Introduction to behavioral addictions. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse 2010; 36: 233–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Griffiths, MD. Behavioural addiction: an issue for everybody? Empl Couns Today 1996; 8: 1925.Google Scholar
Sussman, S, Sussman, AN. Considering the definition of addiction. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2011; 8: 4025–38.Google Scholar
Spada, MM. Commentary on: are we overpathologizing everyday life? A tenable blueprint for behavioral addiction research. Problems with atheoretical and confirmatory research approaches in the study of behavioral addictions. J Behav Addict 2015; 4: 124–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grant, JE, Chamberlain, SR. Expanding the definition of addiction: DSM-5 vs. ICD-11. CNS Spectr 2016; 21: 300–3.Google Scholar
Harvey, J, Beamish, R, Defrance, J. Physical exercise policy and the welfare state: a framework for comparative analysis. Int Rev Sociol Sport 1993; 28: 5364.Google Scholar
Plymire, DC. Positive addiction: running and human potential in the 1970s. J Sport Hist 2004; 31: 297315.Google Scholar
Maslow, AH. The Power of Self-Actualization. Sounds True, 1992.Google Scholar
Kimiecik, JC, Jackson, SA. Optimal experience in sport: a flow perspective. In: Horn, TS, ed. Advances in Sport Psychology. Human Kinetics, 2002: 501–27.Google Scholar
McInman, AD, Grove, JR. Peak moments in sport: a literature review. Quest 1991; 43: 333–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glasser, W. Positive Addiction. Harper & Row, 1976.Google Scholar
Adams, J. Understanding exercise dependence. J Contemp Psychother 2009; 39: 231–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allegre, B, Souville, M, Therme, P, Griffiths, MD. Definitions and measures of exercise dependence. Addict Res Theory 2006; 14: 631–46.Google Scholar
Estok, PJ, Rudy, EB. Physical, psychosocial, menstrual changes/risks, and addiction in the female marathon and nonmarathon runner. Health Care Women Int 1986; 7: 187202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hausenblas, HA, Symons-Downs, D. How much is too much? The development and validation of the exercise dependence scale. Psychol Health 2002; 17: 387404.Google Scholar
Hausenblas, HA, Symons-Downs, D. Exercise dependence: a systematic review. Psychol Sport Exerc 2002; 3: 89123.Google Scholar
Chapman, CL, De Castro, JM. Running addiction: measurement and associated psychological characteristics. J Sports Med Phys Fitness 1990; 30: 283–90.Google Scholar
Bamber, DJ, Cockerill, IM, Rodgers, S, Carroll, D. Diagnostic criteria for exercise dependence in women. Br J Sports Med 2003; 37: 393400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Billieux, J, Schimmenti, A, Khazaal, Y, Maurage, P, Heeren, A. Are we overpathologizing everyday life? A tenable blueprint for behavioral addiction research. J Behav Addict 2015; 4: 119–23.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed. American Psychiatric Association, 2000.Google Scholar
Brown, R. Some contributions of the study of gambling to the study of other addictions. In: Eadington, WR, Cornelius, JA, eds. Gambling Behavior and Problem Gambling. University of Nevada Press, 1993: 241–72.Google Scholar
Griffiths, MD. Exercise addiction: a case study. Addict Res 1997; 5: 161–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffiths, MD. The evolution of the ‘components model of addiction’ and the need for a confirmatory approach in conceptualizing behavioral addictions. Düşünen Adam J Psychiatry Neurol Sci 2019; 32: 179–84.Google Scholar
Griffiths, MD. A ‘components’ model of addiction within a biopsychosocial framework. J Subst Use 2005; 10: 191–7.Google Scholar
Terry, A, Szabo, A, Griffiths, MD. The Exercise Addiction Inventory: a new brief screening tool. Addict Res Theory 2004; 12: 489–99.Google Scholar
Griffiths, MD, Szabo, A, Terry, A. The Exercise Addiction Inventory: a quick and easy screening tool for health practitioners. Br J Sports Med 2005; 39: e30.Google Scholar
Meyer, C, Taranis, L, Goodwin, H, Haycraft, E. Compulsive exercise and eating disorders. Eur Eat Disord Rev 2011; 19: 174–89.Google ScholarPubMed
Taranis, L, Touyz, S, Meyer, C. Disordered eating and exercise: development and preliminary validation of the Compulsive Exercise Test (CET). Eur Eat Disord Rev 2011; 19: 256–68.Google Scholar
Yates, A, Leehey, K, Shisslak, CM. Running—an analogue of anorexia? N Engl J Med 1983; 308: 251–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCabe, MP, Vincent, MA. Development of body modification and excessive exercise scales for adolescents. Assessment 2002; 9: 131–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Veale, D. Does primary exercise dependence really exist? In: Annett, J, Cripps, B, Steinberg, H, eds. Exercise Addiction: Motivation for Participation in Sport and Exercise: Proceedings of British Psychological Society, Division of Sport and Exercise Psychology. British Psychological Society, 1995: 7175.Google Scholar
Yates, A, Leehey, K, Shisslak, CM. Running—an analogue of anorexia? N Engl J Med 1983; 308: 251–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Veale, D. Exercise dependence. Br J Addict 1987; 82: 735–40.Google Scholar
Blaydon, MJ, Lindner, KJ, Kerr, JH. Metamotivational characteristics of eating-disordered and exercise-dependent triathletes: an application of reversal theory. Psychol Sport Exerc 2002; 3: 223–36.Google Scholar
Blaydon, MJ, Lindner, KJ. Eating disorders and exercise dependence in triathletes. Eat Disord 2002; 10: 4960.Google Scholar
Johnston, O, Reilly, J, Kremer, J. Excessive exercise: from quantitative categorisation to a qualitative continuum approach. Eur Eat Disord Rev 2011; 19: 237–48.Google Scholar
Alcaraz-Ibáñez, M, Paterna, A, Sicilia, A, Griffiths, MD. Morbid exercise behaviour and eating disorders: a meta-analysis. J Behav Addict 2020; 9: 206–24.Google Scholar
Bamber, DJ, Cockerill, IM, Rodgers, S, Carroll, D. “It’s exercise or nothing”: a qualitative analysis of exercise dependence. Br J Sports Med 2000; 34: 423–30.Google Scholar
Cook, B, Hausenblas, HA. The role of exercise dependence for the relationship between exercise behavior and eating pathology: mediator or moderator? J Health Psychol 2008; 13: 495502.Google Scholar
Trott, M, Jackson, SE, Firth, J et al. A comparative meta-analysis of the prevalence of exercise addiction in adults with and without indicated eating disorders. Eat Weight Disord 2021; 26: 3746.Google Scholar
Shroff, H, Reba, L, Thornton, LM et al. Features associated with excessive exercise in women with eating disorders. Int J Eat Disord 2006; 39: 454–61.Google Scholar
Klein, DA, Bennett, AS, Schebendach, J et al. Exercise ‘addiction’ in anorexia nervosa: model development and pilot data. CNS Spectr 2004; 9: 531–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zmijewski, CF. Howard, MO. Exercise dependence and attitudes toward eating among young adults. Eat Behav 2003; 4: 181–95.Google Scholar
Maselli, M, Gobbi, E, Probst, M, Carraro, A. Prevalence of primary and secondary exercise dependence and its correlation with drive for thinness in practitioners of different sports and physical activities. Int J Ment Health Addict 2019; 17: 89101.Google Scholar
Blaydon, MJ, Lindner, KJ, Kerr, JH. Metamotivational characteristics of exercise dependence and eating disorders in highly active amateur sport participants. Pers Individ Dif 2004; 36: 1419–32.Google Scholar
Tod, D, Edwards, C. A meta-analysis of the drive for muscularity’s relationships with exercise behaviour, disordered eating, supplement consumption, and exercise dependence. Int Rev Sport Exerc Psychol 2015; 8: 185203.Google Scholar
Tod, D, Edwards, C, Hall, G. Drive for leanness and health-related behavior within a social/cultural perspective. Body Image 2013; 10: 640–3.Google Scholar
Corazza, O, Simonato, P, Demetrovics, Z et al. The emergence of exercise addiction, body dysmorphic disorder, and other image-related psychopathological correlates in fitness settings: a cross sectional study. PLoS ONE 2019; 14: e0213060.Google Scholar
Foster, AC, Shorter, GW, Griffiths, MD. Muscle dysmorphia: could it be classified as an addiction to body image? J Behav Addict 2015; 4: 15.Google Scholar
Oberle, CD, Watkins, RS, Burkot, AJ. Orthorexic eating behaviors related to exercise addiction and internal motivations in a sample of university students. Eat Weight Disord 2018; 23: 6774.Google Scholar
Cena, H, Barthels, F, Cuzzolaro, M et al. Definition and diagnostic criteria for orthorexia nervosa: a narrative review of the literature. Eat Weight Disord 2019; 24: 209–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Di Nicola, M, Tedeschi, D, Mazza, M et al. Behavioural addictions in bipolar disorder patients: role of impulsivity and personality dimensions. J Affect Disord 2010; 125: 82–8.Google Scholar
Di Nicola, M, Martinotti, G, Mazza, M et al. Quetiapine as add-on treatment for bipolar I disorder with comorbid compulsive buying and physical exercise addiction. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2010; 34: 713–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carmack, MA, Martens, R. Measuring commitment to running: a survey of runners’ attitudes and mental states. J Sport Psychol 1979; 1: 2542.Google Scholar
Corbin, CB, Nielsen, AB, Borsdorf, LL, Laurie, DR. Commitment to physical activity. Int J Sport Psychol 1987; 18: 215–22.Google Scholar
Blumenthal, JA, Toole, LCO, Jonathan, L. Is running an analogue of anorexia nervosa? An empirical study of obligatory running and anorexia nervosa. JAMA 1984; 27: 520–3.Google Scholar
Pasman, LN, Thompson, JK. Body image and eating disturbance in obligatory runners, obligatory weightlifters, and sedentary individuals. Int J Eat Disord 1988; 7: 759–69.3.0.CO;2-G>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hailey, BJ, Bailey, LA. Negative addiction in runners: a quantitative approach. J Sport Behav 1982; 5: 150–4.Google Scholar
Freimuth, M, Moniz, S, Kim, SR. Clarifying exercise addiction: differential diagnosis, co-occurring disorders, and phases of addiction. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2011; 8: 4069–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, C, Brewer, H, Ratusny, D. Behavioral frequency and psychological commitment: necessary concepts in the study of excessive exercising. J Behav Med 1993; 16: 611–28.Google Scholar
Ogden, J, Veale, D, Summers, Z. The development and validation of the Exercise Dependence Questionnaire. Addict Res 1997; 5: 343–56.Google Scholar
Colledge, F, Buchner, U, Schmidt, A, Walter, M. Does exercise addiction exist? A brief review on current measurement tools and future directions. Ment Health Addict Res 2019; 4: 14.Google Scholar
Marques, A, Peralta, M, Sarmento, H et al. Prevalence of risk for exercise dependence: a systematic review. Sports Med 2019; 49: 319–30.Google Scholar
Symons-Downs, D, Hausenblas, HA, Nigg, CR. Factorial validity and psychometric examination of the Exercise Dependence Scale-Revised. Meas Phys Educ Exerc Sci 2004; 8: 183201.Google Scholar
Lindwall, M, Palmeira, A. Factorial validity and invariance testing of the Exercise Dependence Scale-Revised in Swedish and Portuguese exercisers. Meas Phys Educ Exerc Sci 2009; 13: 166–79.Google Scholar
Sicilia, A, González-Cutre, D. Dependence and physical exercise: Spanish validation of the Exercise Dependence Scale-Revised (EDS-R). Span J Psychol 2011; 14: 421–31.Google Scholar
Müller, A, Claes, L, Smits, D et al. Validation of the German version of the Exercise Dependence Scale. Eur J Psychol Assess 2013; 29: 213–19.Google Scholar
Costa, S. Psychometric examination and factorial validity of the Exercise Dependence Scale-Revised in Italian exercisers. J Behav Addict 2012; 1: 186–90.Google Scholar
Szabo, A, Pinto, A, Griffiths, MD, Kovácsik, R, Demetrovics, Z. The psychometric evaluation of the Revised Exercise Addiction Inventory: improved psychometric properties by changing item response rating. J Behav Addict 2019; 8: 157–61.Google Scholar
Meyer, C, Plateau, CR, Taranis, L et al. The Compulsive Exercise Test: confirmatory factor analysis and links with eating psychopathology among women with clinical eating disorders. J Eat Disord 2016; 4: 19.Google Scholar
Plateau, CR, Shanmugam, V, Duckham, RL et al. Use of the Compulsive Exercise Test with athletes: norms and links with eating psychopathology. J Appl Sport Psychol 2014; 26: 287301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffiths, MD, Urbán, R, Demetrovics, Z et al. A cross-cultural re-evaluation of the Exercise Addiction Inventory (EAI) in five countries. Sports Med Open 2015; 1: 17.Google Scholar
Mónok, K, Berczik, K, Urbán, R et al. Psychometric properties and concurrent validity of two exercise addiction measures: a population wide study. Psychol Sport Exerc 2012; 13: 739–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lejoyeux, M, Avril, M, Richoux, C, Embouazza, H, Nivoli, F. Prevalence of exercise dependence and other behavioral addictions among clients of a Parisian fitness room. Compr Psychiatry 2008; 49: 353–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Serier, KN, Smith, JE, Lash, DN et al. Obligatory exercise and coping in treatment-seeking women with poor body image. Eat Weight Disord 2018; 23: 331–8.Google Scholar
Sicilia, A, Paterna, A, Alcaraz-Ibáñez, M, Griffiths, MD. Theoretical conceptualizations of problematic exercise in psychometric assessment instruments: a systematic review. J Behav Addict 2021; 10: 420.Google Scholar
Szabo, A, Frenkl, R, Caputo, A. Relationship between addiction to running, commitment to running, and deprivation from running: a study on the Internet. In: European Yearbook of Sport Psychology, Vol. 1. European Federation of Sport Psychology, 1997: 130–47.Google Scholar
Weik, M, Hale, BD. Contrasting gender differences on two measures of exercise dependence. Br J Sports Med 2009; 43: 204–7.Google Scholar
Hausenblas, HA, Giacobbi, PR. Relationship between exercise dependence symptoms and personality. Pers Individ Dif 2004; 36: 1265–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Szabo, A, Griffiths, MD, de La Vega, R, Mervó, B, Demetrovics, Z. Methodological and conceptual limitations in exercise addiction research. Yale J Biol Med 2015; 88: 303–8.Google Scholar
Chamberlain, SR, Grant, JE. Is problematic exercise really problematic? A dimensional approach. CNS Spectr 2020; 25: 6470.Google Scholar
Adams, J, Kirkby, R. Exercise dependence: a problem for sports physiotherapists. Aust J Physiother 1997; 43: 53–8.Google Scholar
Weinstein, AA, Koehmstedt, C, Kop, WJ. Mental health consequences of exercise withdrawal: a systematic review. Gen Hosp Psychiatry 2017; 49: 1118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colledge, F, Cody, R, Pühse, U, Gerber, M. Responses of fitness center employees to cases of suspected eating disorders or excessive exercise. J Eat Disord 2020; 8: 19.Google Scholar
Prochaska, JO, Redding, CA, Evers, KE. The transtheoretical model and stages of change. In: Glanz, K, Rimer, BK, Viswanath, K, eds. Health Behavior. Theory, Research, and Practice. Jossey-Bass, 2015: 125–48.Google Scholar
Petry, NM. Pathological Gambling: Etiology, Comorbidity, and Treatment. American Psychological Association, 2005.Google Scholar
Corazza, O, Simonato, P, Demetrovics, Z et al. The emergence of exercise addiction, body dysmorphic disorder, and other image-related psychopathological correlates in fitness settings: a cross sectional study. PLoS ONE 2019; 14: 117.Google Scholar
Shaffer, HJ, LaPlante, DA, LaBrie, RA et al. Toward a syndrome model of addiction: multiple expressions, common etiology. Harv Rev Psychiatry 2004; 12: 367–74.Google Scholar
Egorov, AY, Szabo, A. The exercise paradox: an interactional model for a clearer conceptualization of exercise addiction. J Behav Addict 2013; 2: 199208.Google Scholar
Kardefelt-Winther, D, Heeren, A, Schimmenti, A et al. How can we conceptualize behavioural addiction without pathologizing common behaviours? Addiction 2017; 112: 1709–15.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×