Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-p2v8j Total loading time: 0.001 Render date: 2024-05-31T16:52:07.524Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

5 - Language and Thought in Multilingual Children

from Part Two - Cognition and Faculties in Multilinguals

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 August 2022

Anat Stavans
Affiliation:
Beit Berl College, Israel
Ulrike Jessner
Affiliation:
Leopold-Franzens-Universität Innsbruck, Austria
Get access

Summary

A large body of research has contributed to a complex picture in which bilingualism is generally associated with better performance on some cognitive tasks, particularly those that are based on executive functioning, but poorer performance on measures of verbal proficiency. However, not all studies find these effects, particularly the positive effects on cognitive function. What is now clear is that the potential impact of bilingualism on children’s cognition must be explained through multifaceted examinations of relevant factors and clarification of the specific language context from which the results emerged on an individual basis. We review the evidence for cognitive performance of children in multilingual environments and evaluate those results in terms of the type of cognitive ability being assessed and the type of environment children are experiencing. We also review how early the effects of multilingualism are detected, how long these effects last, and how childhood multilingualism can lead to brain plasticity. We conclude with a brief discussion of how multilingualism impacts other areas of cognitive functioning, such as theory of mind, creativity, and problem solving.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2022

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adesope, O. O., Lavin, T., Thompson, T., & Ungerleider, C. (2010). Systematic review and meta-analysis on the cognitive benefits of bilingualism. Review of Educational Research, 80, 207–45, https://doi.org/10.3102%2F0034654310368803.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adi-Japha, E., Berberich-Artzi, J., & Libnawi, A. (2010). Cognitive flexibility in drawings of bilingual children. Child Development, 81(5), 1356–66, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01477.x.Google Scholar
Altman, C., Goldstein, T., & Armon-Lotem, S. (2018). Vocabulary, metalinguistic awareness and language dominance among bilingual preschool children. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, e1953, https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01953.Google Scholar
Antón, E., Duñabeitia, J. A., Estévez, A., Hernández, J. A., Castillo, A., Fuentes, L. J., Davidson, D. J., & Carreiras, M. (2014). Is there a bilingual advantage in the ANT task? Evidence from children. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, e398, https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00398.Google Scholar
Arredondo, M. M., Hu, X-S., Satterfield, T., & Kovelman, I. (2017). Bilingualism alters children’s frontal love functioning for attentional control. Developmental Science, 20(3), e12377, https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12377.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barac, R., Bialystok, E., Castro, D. C., & Sanchez, M. (2014). The cognitive development of young dual language learners: A critical review. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29(4), 699714, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.02.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barac, R., Moreno, S., & Bialystok, E. (2016). Behavioral and electrophysiological differences in executive control between monolingual and bilingual children. Child Development, 87(4), 1277–90, https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12538.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21, 3746, https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8.Google Scholar
Beatty-Martínez, A. L., Navarro-Torres, C. A., Dussias, P. E., Bajo, M. T., Guzzardo Tamargo, R. E., & Kroll, J. F. (2020). Interactional context mediates the consequences of bilingualism for language and cognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 46(6), 1022–47, https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm00007.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2017). The bilingual adaptation: How minds accommodate experience. Psychological Bulletin, 143(3), 233–62, https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000099.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2018). Bilingual education for young children: Review of the effects and consequences. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 21, 666–79, https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2016.1203859.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., & Barac, R. (2012). Emerging bilingualism: Dissociating advantages for metalinguistic awareness and executive control. Cognition, 122(1), 6773, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.08.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Green, D. W., & Gollan, T. H. (2009). Bilingual minds. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 10(3), 89129, https://doi.org/10.1177%2F1529100610387084.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Luk, G., Peets, K. F., & Yang, S. (2010). Receptive vocabulary differences in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism, 13(4), 525–31, https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728909990423.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Martin, M. M. (2004). Attention and inhibition in bilingual children: Evidence from the dimensional change card sort task. Developmental Science, 7(3), 325–29, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00351.x.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Martin, M. M., & Viswanathan, M. (2005). Bilingualism across the lifespan: The rise and fall of inhibitory control. International Journal of Bilingualism, 9(1), 103–19, https://doi.org/10.1177%2F13670069050090010701.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Shapero, D. (2005). Ambiguous benefits: The effect of bilingualism on reversing ambiguous figures. Developmental Science, 8(6), 595604, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00451.x.Google Scholar
Blakemore, S. J., & Choudhury, S. (2006). Development of the adolescent brain: Implications for executive function and social cognition. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(3–4), 296312, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01611.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bull, R., & Lee, K. (2014). Executive functioning and mathematics achievement. Child Development Perspectives, 8(1), 3641, https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12059.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byers-Heinlein, K., Esposito, A. G., Winsler, A., Marian, V., Castro, D. C., & Luk, G. (2019). The case for measuring and reporting bilingualism in developmental research. Collabra: Psychology, 5(1), 37, https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.233.Google Scholar
Carlson, S. M., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science, 11(2), 282–98, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00675.x.Google Scholar
Carlson, S. M., & Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Development, 72, 1032–53, https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00333.Google Scholar
Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Breton, C. (2002). How specific is the relation between executive function and theory of mind? Contributions of inhibitory control and working memory. Infant and Child Development, 11, 7392.Google Scholar
Carlson, S. M., Zelazo, P. D., & Faja, S. (2013). Executive function. In Zelazo, P. D., ed., Oxford Library of Psychology. The Oxford Handbook of Developmental Psychology (Vol. 1): Body and Mind. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 706–43.Google Scholar
Choi, D., Black, A. K., & Werker, J. F. (2018). Cascading and multisensory influences on speech perception development. Mind, Brain, and Education, 12(4), 212–23, https://doi.org/10.1111/mbe.12162.Google Scholar
Christoffels, I. K., de Haan, A. M., Steenbergen, L., van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., & Colzato, L. S. (2015). Two is better than one: Bilingual education promotes the flexible mind. Psychological Research, 79(3), 371–79, https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0575-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chung-Fat-Yim, A., Himel, C., & Bialystok, E. (2019). The impact of bilingualism on executive function in adolescents. International Journal of Bilingualism, 23(6), 1278–90, https://doi.org/10.1177%2F1367006918781059.Google Scholar
Chung-Fat-Yim, A., Sorge, G., & Bialystok, E. (2020). Continuous effects of bilingualism and attention on Flanker task performance. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23(5), 11061111, https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728920000036.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Comishen, K. J., Bialystok, E., & Adler, S. A. (2019). The impact of bilingual environments on selective attention in infancy. Developmental Science, 22(4), e12797, https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12797.Google Scholar
Costa, A., Hernández, M., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2008). Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition, 106(1), 5986, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.12.013.Google Scholar
Cropley, A. (2006). In praise of convergent thinking. Creativity Research Journal, 18(3), 391404, https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326934crj1803_13.Google Scholar
Darcy, N. T. (1953). A review of the literature on the effects of bilingualism upon the measurement of intelligence. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 82, 2157, https://doi.org/10.1080/08856559.1953.10533654.Google Scholar
Della Rosa, P. A., Videsott, G., Borsa, V. M., Canini, M., Weekes, B. S., Franceschini, R., & Abutalebi, J. (2013). A neural interactive location for multilingual talent. Cortex, 49(2), 605–08, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2012.12.001.Google Scholar
Deutsch, G. K., Dougherty, R. F., Bammer, R., Siok, W. T., Gabrieli, J. D., & Wandell, B. (2005). Children’s reading performance is correlated with white matter structure measured by diffusion tensor imaging. Cortex, 41, 354–63, https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70272-7.Google Scholar
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135–68, https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750.Google Scholar
Duñabeitia, J. A., Hernández, J. A., Antón, E., Macizo, P., Estévez, A., Fuentes, L. J., & Carreiras, M. (2014). The inhibitory advantage in bilingual children revisited: myth or reality? Experimental Psychology, 61, 234–51, https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1027/1618-3169/a000243.Google Scholar
Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception and Psychophysics, 16(1), 143–49.Google Scholar
Espinosa, L. M., LaForett, D. R., Burchinal, M., Winsler, A., Tien, H.-C., Peisner-Feinberg, E. S., & Castro, D. C. (2017). Child care experiences among dual language learners in the United States: Analyses of the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study–Birth Cohort. AERA Open, 3(2), 115.Google Scholar
Fan, S. P., Liberman, Z., Keysar, B., & Kinzler, K. D. (2015). The exposure advantage: Early exposure to a multilingual environment promotes effective communication. Psychological Science, 26, 1090–97, https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615574699.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A. & Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(3), 340–7, https://doi.org/10.1162/089892902317361886.Google Scholar
Farhadian, M., Abdullah, R., Mansor, M., Redzuan, M., Gazanizadand, N., & Kumar, V. (2010). Theory of mind in bilingual and monolingual preschool children. Journal of Psychology, 1, 3946, https://doi.org/10.1080/09764224.2010.11885444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferjan Ramírez, N., Ramírez, R. R., Clarke, M., Taulu, S., & Kuhl, P. K. (2017). Speech discrimination in 11-month-old bilingual and monolingual infants: A magnetoencephalography study. Developmental Science, 20(1), e12427, https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12427.Google Scholar
Friesen, D. C., & Bialystok, E. (2012). Metalinguistic ability in bilingual children. Rivista di Psycolinguistica Applicata, 12(3), 4756.Google Scholar
Gathercole, V. C. M., Thomas, E. M., Kennedy, I., Prys, C., Young, N., Guasch, N. V., Roberts, E. J., Hughes, E. K., & Jones, L. (2014). Does language dominance affect cognitive performance in bilinguals? Lifespan evidence from preschoolers through older adults on card sorting, Simon, and metalinguistic tasks. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, e11, https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00011.Google Scholar
Goetz, P. J. (2003). The effects of bilingualism on theory of mind development. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 6(1), 115, https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728903001007.Google Scholar
Goriot, C., Broersma, M., McQueen, J. M., Unsworth, S., & van Hout, R. (2018). Language balance and switching ability in children acquiring English as a second language. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 173, 168–86, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2018.03.019.Google Scholar
Green, D. W., & Abutalebi, J. (2013). Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 515–30, https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.796377.Google Scholar
Greenberg, A., Bellana, B., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Perspective-taking ability in bilingual children: Extending advantages in executive control to spatial reasoning. Cognitive Development, 28(1), 4150, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2012.10.002.Google Scholar
Grosjean, F. (2012). Bilingualism: A short introduction. In Grosjean, F. & Li, P., eds., The Psycholinguistics of Bilingualism. Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons, pp. 525.Google Scholar
Hartanto, A., Yang, H., & Yang, S. (2018). Bilingualism positively predicts mathematical competence: Evidence from two large-scale studies. Learning and Individual Differences, 61, 216–27.Google Scholar
Jasinska, K. K., & Pettito, L. A. (2013). How age of bilingual exposure can change the neural systems for language in the developing brain: A functional near infrared spectroscopy investigation of syntactic processing in monolingual and bilingual children. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 6, 87101, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2013.06.005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jasinska, K. K., & Pettito, L. A. (2014). Development of neural systems for reading in the monolingual and bilingual brain: New insights from functional near infrared spectroscopy neuroimaging. Developmental Neuropsychology, 6, 421–39, https://doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2014.939180.Google Scholar
Kapa, L. L., & Colombo, J. (2013). Attentional control in early and later bilingual children. Cognitive Development, 28(3), 233–46, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2013.01.011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1990). Constraints on representational change: Evidence from children’s drawing. Cognition, 34, 5783, https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(90)90031-e.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kempert, S., Saalbach, H., & Hardy, I. (2011). Cognitive benefits and costs of bilingualism in elementary school students: The case of mathematical word problems. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(3), 547–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kovács, Á. M. (2009). Early bilingualism enhances mechanisms of false belief reasoning. Developmental Science, 12(1), 4854, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00742.x.Google Scholar
Kovács, Á. M., & Mehler, J. (2009). Cognitive gains in 7-month-old bilingual infants. PNAS, 106(16), 6556–60, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0811323106.Google Scholar
Krizman, J., Marian, V., Shook, A., Skoe, E., & Kraus, N. (2012). Subcortical encoding of sound is enhanced in bilinguals and relates to executive function advantages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(20), 7877–81, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1201575109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krizman, J., Skoe, E., Marian, V., & Kraus, N. (2014). Bilingualism increases neural response consistency and attentional control: Evidence for sensory and cognitive coupling. Brain and Language, 128(1), 3440, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2013.11.006.Google Scholar
Kroll, J. F., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Understanding the consequences of bilingualism for language processing and cognition. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 497514, https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.799170.Google Scholar
Kroll, J. F., Dussias, P. E., Bogulski, C. A., & Valdes-Kroff, J. (2012). Juggling two languages in one mind: What bilinguals tell us about language processing and its consequences for cognition. In Ross, B., ed., The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, Volume 56. San Diego: Academic Press, pp. 229–62.Google Scholar
Kuipers, J. R., & Thierry, G. (2012). Event-related potential correlated of language change detection in bilingual toddlers. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2(1), 97102, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2011.08.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuipers, J. R., & Thierry, G. (2015). Bilingualism and increased attention to speech: Evidence from event-related potentials. Brain and Language, 149 , 2732, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2015.07.004.Google Scholar
Lee, H., & Kim, K. H. (2011). Can speaking more languages enhance your creativity? Relationship between bilingualism and creative potential among Korean American students with multicultural link. Personality and Individual Differences, 50(8), 1186–90.Google Scholar
Leikin, M., & Tovli, E. (2014). Bilingualism and creativity in early childhood. Creativity Research Journal, 26(4), 411–7, https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2014.961779.Google Scholar
Lewkowicz, D. J., & Hansen-Tift, A. M. (2012). Infants deploy selective attention to the mouth of a talking face when learning speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109(5), 1431–6, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1114783109.Google Scholar
Li, W. (2008). Research perspectives on bilingualism and multilingualism. In Li, W. & Moyer, M., eds., The Blackwell Handbook of Research Methods on Bilingualism and Multilingualism. Oxford: Blackwell, pp. 317.Google Scholar
Lu, C.-H., & Proctor, R. W. (1995). The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 2, 174207, https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03210959.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luk, G., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Bilingualism is not a categorical variable: Interaction between language proficiency and usage. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 605–21, https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.795574.Google Scholar
Macnamara, J. (1966). Bilingualism and Primary Education. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Marian, V., & Hayakawa, S. (2021). Measuring bilingualism: The quest for a “bilingualism quotient”. Applied Psycholinguistics, 42(2), 527548.Google Scholar
Marian, V., Shook, A., & Schroeder, S. R. (2013). Bilingual two-way immersion programs benefit academic achievement. Bilingual Research Journal, 36(2), 167–83, https://doi.org/10.1080/15235882.2013.818075.Google Scholar
Marian, V., & Spivey, M. (2003). Competing activation in bilingual language processing: Within- and between-language competition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 6(2), 97115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin-Rhee, M. M., & Bialystok, E. (2008). The development of two types of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11(1), 8193, https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1017/S1366728907003227.Google Scholar
Mohades, S. G., Struys, E., Van Schuerbeek, P., Mondt, K., Van De Craen, P., & Luypaert, R. (2012). DTI reveals structural differences in white matter tracts between bilingual and monolingual children. Brain Research, 1435, 7280, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2011.12.005.Google Scholar
Mohades, S. G., Van Schuerbeek, P., Rosseel, Y., Van De Craen, P., Luypaert, R., & Baeken, C. (2015). White-matter development is different in bilingual and monolingual children: a longitudinal DTI study. PloS One, 10(2), e0117968, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0117968.Google Scholar
Morales, J., Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Working memory development in monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114(2), 187202, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.002.Google Scholar
Okanda, M., Moriguchi, Y., & Itakura, S. (2010). Language and cognitive shifting: Evidence from young monolingual and bilingual children. Psychological Reports, 107(1), 6878, https://doi.org/10.2466/03.10.28.PR0.107.4.68-78.Google Scholar
O’Sullivan, M., Morris, R. G., Huckstep, B., Jones, D., Williams, S., & Markus, H. (2004). Diffusion tensor MRI correlates with executive dysfunction in patients with ischaemic leukoaraiosis. Journal Neurolology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 75, 441–7, http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.2003.014910.Google Scholar
Paap, K. R., & Greenberg, Z. I. (2013). There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66(2), 232–58, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.12.002.Google Scholar
Papageorgiou, A., Bright, P., Periche Thomas, E., & Filippi, R. (2018). Evidence against a cognitive advantage in the older bilingual population. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72(6), 1354–63, https://doi.org/10.1177%2F1747021818796475.Google Scholar
Peal, E. & Lambert, W. E. (1962). The relation of bilingualism to intelligence. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 76, 123, https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/h0093840.Google Scholar
Poarch, G. J., & Bialystok, E. (2015). Bilingualism as a model for multitasking. Developmental Review, 35, 113–24, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2014.12.003.Google Scholar
Poarch, G. J., & Van Hell, J. G. (2012). Executive functions and inhibitory control in multilingual children: Evidence from second language learners, bilinguals, and trilinguals. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113(4), 535–51, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.06.013.Google Scholar
Pons, F., Bosch, L., & Lewkowicz, D. J. (2015). Bilingualism modulates infants’ selective attention to the mouth of a talking face. Psychological Science, 26(4), 490–8, https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1177/0956797614568320.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 325, https://doi.org/10.1080/00335558008248231.Google Scholar
Prior, A., & MacWhinney, B. (2010). A bilingual advantage in task switching. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(2), 253–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubio-Fernández, P. (2017). Why are bilinguals better than monolingual children at false-belief tasks? Psychonomic Bulletin Review, 24(3), 987–98, https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-016-1143-1.Google Scholar
Rueda, M. R., Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Halparin, J. D., Gruber, D. B., Pappert Lercari, L., & Posner, M. I. (2004). Development of attentional networks in childhood. Neuropsychologia, 42(8), 1029–40, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.12.012.Google Scholar
Saer, D. J. (1923). The effects of bilingualism on intelligence. British Journal of Psychology, 14, 2538, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1923.tb00110.x.Google Scholar
Schroeder, S. R. (2018). Do bilinguals have an advantage in theory of mind? A meta-analysis. Frontiers in Communication, 3, e36, https://doi.org/10.3389/fcomm.2018.00036.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sebastián-Gallés, N., Albareda-Castellot, B., Weikum, W. M., & Werker, J. F. (2012). A bilingual advantage in visual language discrimination in infancy. Psychological Science, 23(9), 994–99, https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612436817.Google Scholar
Sebastián-Gallés, N., Echeverría, S., & Bosch, L. (2005). The influence of initial exposure on lexical representation: Comparing early and simultaneous bilinguals. Journal of Memory and Language, 52(2), 240–55.Google Scholar
Simon, J. R., & Rudell, A. P. (1967). Auditory S-R compatibility: The effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 51(3), 300–4, https://doi.org/10.1037/h0020586.Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (2008). Bilingualism and creativity. In Altarriba, J. & Heredia, R. R., eds., An Introduction to Bilingualism: Principles and Processes. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 147–66.Google Scholar
Sorge, G. B., Toplak, M. E., & Bialystok, E. (2017). Interactions between levels of attention and levels of bilingualism in children’s executive functioning. Developmental Science, 20(1), e12408, https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12408.Google Scholar
Surrain, S., & Luk, G. (2017). Describing bilinguals: A systematic review of labels and descriptions used in the literature between 2005–2015. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 22(2), 401–15, https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728917000682.Google Scholar
Swanson, J. M., Schuck, S., Mann Porter, M., Carlson, C., Hartman, C. A., Sergeant, J. A., Clevenger, W., Wasdell, M., McCleary, R., Lakes, K., & Wigal, T. (2012). Categorical and dimensional definitions and evaluations of symptoms of ADHD: History of the SNAP and the SWAN rating scales. International Journal of Educational Psychological Assessment, 10(1), 5170.Google Scholar
Thomas-Sunesson, D., Hakuta, K., & Bialystok, E. (2018). Degree of bilingualism modifies executive control in Hispanic children in the USA. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 21(2), 197206.Google Scholar
Torrance, E. P. (1998). The Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking Norms – Technical Manual Figural (Streamlined) Forms A & B. Bensenville: Scholastic Testing Service.Google Scholar
Tse, C-S., & Altarriba, J. (2014). The relationship between language proficiency and attentional control in Cantonese-English bilingual children: Evidence from Simon, Simon switching, and working memory tasks. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, e954, https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00954.Google Scholar
Valian, V. (2015). Bilingualism and cognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(1), 324, https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728914000522.Google Scholar
Vega, C., & Fernandez, M. (2011). Errors on the WCST correlate with language proficiency scores in Spanish-English bilingual children. Archives in Clinical Neuropsychology, 26(2), 158–64, https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acq097.Google Scholar
Videsott, G., Della Rosa, P. A., Wiater, W., Franceschini, R., & Abutalebi, J. (2012). How does linguistic competence enhance cognitive functions in children? A study in multilingual children with different linguistic competences. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15(4), 884–95, https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728912000119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wei, M., Joshi, A. A., Zhang, M., Mei, L. Manis, F. R., He, Q., Beattie, R. L., … Lu, Z-L. (2015). How age of acquisition influences brain architecture in bilinguals. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 36, 3555, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2015.05.001.Google Scholar
Weikum, W. M., Vouloumanos, A., Navarra, J., Soto-Faraco, S., Sebastián-Gallés, N., Werker, J. F. (2007). Visual language discrimination in infancy. Science, 316, 1159, https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1126/science.1137686.Google Scholar
Wimmer, M. C., & Marx, C. (2014). Inhibitory processes in visual perception: A bilingual advantage. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 126, 412–9, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.03.004.Google Scholar
Yang, S., Yang, H., & Lust, B. (2011). Early childhood bilingualism leads to advances in executive attention: Dissociating culture and language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14(3), 412–22.Google Scholar
Yoshida, H., Tran, D. N., Benitez, V., & Kuwabara, M. (2011). Inhibition and adjective learning in bilingual and monolingual children. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, e210, https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00210.Google Scholar
Zelazo, P. D. (2006). The dimensional change card sort (DCCS): A method of assessing executive function in children. Nature Protocols, 1(1), 297301, https://doi.org/10.1038/nprot.2006.46.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×