Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-75dct Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-02T09:50:04.050Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

24 - Morphology of Heritage Languages

from Part III - Grammatical Aspects of Heritage Languages

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 November 2021

Silvina Montrul
Affiliation:
University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign
Maria Polinsky
Affiliation:
University of Maryland, College Park
Get access

Summary

The acquisition, maintenance, and attrition of morphological properties of heritage languages (HLs) has been a central research focus since the inception of the systematic study of these linguistic varieties. Both child and adult heritage language speakers (HSs) experience difficulty in producing target-like inflectional morphology, and in some instances, the errors in their production are similar to those found in the speech of L2 learners. This chapter offers a broad survey of developmental trends of derivational and inflectional morphology in the nominal (e.g., gender and case) and verbal (e.g., agreement, tense, aspect, mood) domains. Different morphological types (e.g., inflectional, agglutinative, fusion, isolating) are discussed, focusing on whether certain properties of heritage morphology are specific to each type and whether certain properties cut across all of them. Claims regarding the effects of maturational constraints and continued activation on the ultimate attainment of heritage morphology are reviewed. This chapter also considers the issue of age effects in connection with heritage morphology and concludes with a brief discussion of the implications that these findings have on linguistic theory as well as highlighting future directions for the study of heritage morphology.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2021

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aarssen, J. 2001. Development of Temporal Relations in Narratives by Turkish-Dutch Bilingual Children. In Verhoeven, L. and Stömqvist, S. (eds.), Narrative Development in a Multilingual Context. Amsterdam: John Benjmains, 209232.Google Scholar
Akıncı, M. 2003. Temporal Anchoring of Texts in Turkish of First and Second Generation Turkish Immigrants in France. In Ozsoy, S., Akar, D., Nkipoglu-Demiralp, N., Erguvanli-Taylan, A., and Aksu-Koç, A. (eds.), Studies in Turkish Linguistics. Istanbul: Boğaziçi University Press, 289297.Google Scholar
Aksu-Koç, A. 1998. The Acquisition of Aspect and Modality: The Case of Past Reference in Turkish. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Aksu-Koç, A. 2000. Some Aspects of the Acquisition of Evidential in Turkish. In Johnson, L. and Utas, B. (eds.), Evidential: Turkic, Iranian and Neighbouring Languages. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, 1528.Google Scholar
Amaral, L. and Roeper, T.. 2014. Multiple Grammars and Second Language Representation. Second Language Research 30(1), 336.Google Scholar
Anstatt, T. 2011. Sprachattrition: Abbau der Erstsprache bei russisch-deutschen Jugendlichen. Wiener Slawistischer Almanach 67, 731.Google Scholar
Árnbjörnsdóttir, B. 2006. North American Icelandic: The Life of a Language. Manitoba: University of Manitoba Press.Google Scholar
Árnbjörnsdóttir, B. 2015. Reexamining Icelandic as a Heritage Language in North America. In Johannessen, J. B. and Salmons, J. C. (eds.), Germanic Heritage Languages in North America: Acquisition, Attrition and Change. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 7293.Google Scholar
Arslan, S., DeKok, D., and Bastiaanse, R.. 2015. Processing Grammatical Evidentiality and Time Reference in Turkish Heritage and Monolingual Speakers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 20(5), 457472.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baker, M. 1996. The Polysynthesis Parameter. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bauer, L. 2017. Compounds and Compounding. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bayram, F., Pascual y Cabo, D., and Rothman, J.. 2019. Intra-generational Attrition: Contributions to Heritage Speaker Competence. In Schmid, M. and Köpke, B. (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Language Attrition. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 446457.Google Scholar
Benmamoun, E., Montrul, S., and Polinsky, M.. 2013. Heritage Languages and Their Speakers: Opportunities and Challenges for Linguistics. Theoretical Linguistics 39, 129181.Google Scholar
Binnick, R. I. 1991. Time and the Verb: A Guide to Tense and Aspect. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boas, H. C. 2009. Case Loss in Texas German: The Influence of Semantic and Pragmatic Factors. In Barðdal, J. and Chelliah, S. L. (eds.), The Role of Semantics, Pragmatic, and Discourse Factors in the Development of Case. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 347373.Google Scholar
Bolonyai, A. 2007. (In) Vulnerable Agreement in Incomplete Bilingual L1 Learners. International Journal of Bilingualism 11(1), 323.Google Scholar
Borer, H. 2013. Structuring Sense: Volume III: Taking Form. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bousquette, J. and Putnam, M. T.. 2020. Reassessing Language Death: Evidence from Moribund Grammars. Language Learning 70(S1), 188225.Google Scholar
Bybee, J. L., Perkins, R. D., and Pagliuca, W.. 1994. The Evolution of Grammar: Tense, Aspect, and Modality in the Languages of the World. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Campbell, L. and Muntzel, M. C.. 1989. The Structural Consequences of Language Death. In Investigating Obsolescence: Studies in Language Contraction and Death. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 181196.Google Scholar
Carstairs-McCarthy, A. 2010. The Evolution of Morphology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cinque, G. 1999. Adverbs and Functional Heads: A Cross-Linguistic Perspective. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Croft, W. 2003. Typology and Universals, 2nd edition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Culicover, P. 2013. Grammar & Complexity: Language at the Intersection of Competence and Performance. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
De Reuse, W. J. 1994. Noun Incorporation in Lakota (Siouan). International Journal of American Linguistics 60(3), 199260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dixon, R. M. and Aikhenvald, A. Y.. 2002. Word: A Typological Framework. In Dixon, R. M. and Aikhenvald, A. Y. (eds.), Word: A Cross-Linguistic Typology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 141.Google Scholar
Di Sciullo, A. M. and Edwin Williams, E.. 1987. On the Definition of Word. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Eikel, F. 1949. The Use of Cases in New Braunfels German. American Speech 24(4), 278281.Google Scholar
Evans, N and Sasse, H.-J.. 2002. Problems of Polysynthesis. Berlin: Akademie Verlag.Google Scholar
Fenyvesi, A. 2000. The Affectedness of the Verbal Complex in American Hungarian. In Language Contact and the Verb Complex of Dutch and Hungarian. Szeged: University of Szeged, 94107.Google Scholar
Fernández-Dobao, A. and Herschensohn, J.. 2019. Present Tense Verb Morphology on Spanish HL and L2 Children in Dual Immersion: Feature Reassembly Revisited. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism. Online First.Google Scholar
Filip, H. 1999. Aspect, Eventuality Types, and Noun Phrase Semantics. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Flores, C. 2015. Losing a Childhood Language: A Longitudinal Study on Language Attrition. Journal of Child Language 42, 562590.Google Scholar
Flores, C. 2020. Attrition and Reactivation of a Childhood Language: The Case of Returnee Heritage Speakers. Language Learning 70(S1), 85121.Google Scholar
Fortescue, M. 1994. Morphology Polysynthetic, In Asher, R. E. (ed.), The Encylopedia of Language and Linguistics. Oxford: Pergamon, 26002602.Google Scholar
Gilbert, G. G. 1965. Dative vs. Accusative in the German Dialects of Central Texas. Zeitschrift für Dialektologie und Linguistik 32(3), 288.Google Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., Cera, C., and Sandoval, T. C.. 2008. More Use almost always Means a Smaller Frequency Effect: Aging, Bilingualism, and the Weaker Links Hypothesis. Journal of Memory and Language 58(3), 787814.Google Scholar
Gruzdeva, E. 2015. Sociolinguistic and Linguistic Outcomes of Nivkh-Russian Language Contact. Language Empires in Comparative Perspective 6, 153181.Google Scholar
Gruzdeva, E. and Vakhtin, N.. 2017. Language Obsolescence in Polysynthetic Languages. In Fortescue, M., Mithun, M., and Evans, N. (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Polysynthesis. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 428448.Google Scholar
Haspelmath, M. 2011. The Indeterminancy of Word Segmentation and the Nature of Morphology and Syntax. Folia Linguistica 45(1), 3180.Google Scholar
Hawkins, J. A. 2004. Efficiency and Complexity in Grammars. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Huls, E. and van de Mond, A.. 1992. Some Aspects of Language Attrition in Turkish Families in the Netherlands. In Fase, W., Jaspaert, K., and Kroom, S. (eds.), Maintenance and Loss of Minority Languages. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 99117.Google Scholar
Jia, L. and Bailey, R.. 2008. The (re)acquisition of Perfective Aspect Marking by Chinese Heritage Language Learners. In He, A. W. and Xiao, Yun (eds.), Chinese as a Heritage Language: Fostering Rooted World Citizenry. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Mànoa, 205224.Google Scholar
Johannessen, J. B. and Salmons, J. C., Forthcoming. Heritage Germanic. In M. Polinsky and S. Montrul (eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Heritage Languages. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. 2016. Object case assignment in heritage Finnish. Presentation. 7th Annual Workshop on Immigrant Languages in the Americas, Oct 2016.Google Scholar
Karayayla, T. 2018. Turkish as an Immigrant and Heritage Language in the UK: Effects of Exposure and Age at Onset of Bilingualism on Grammatical and Lexical Development of the First Language. PhD thesis, University of Essex.Google Scholar
Keine, S. 2007. Reanalyzing Hindi Split-Ergativity as a Morphological Phenomenon. Linguistische Arbeitsberichte 85, 73127.Google Scholar
Kroll, J. F. and Gollan, T. H.. 2014. Speech Planning in Two Languages: What Bilinguals Tell Us about Language Production. In Goldrick, M., Ferreira, V., and Miozzo, M. (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Language Production. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 165181.Google Scholar
Laleko, O. 2008. Compositional Telicity and Heritage Russian Aspect. Proceedings of the Thirty-Eighth Western Conference on Linguistics (WECOL) 19, 150160.Google Scholar
Laleko, O. 2010. The Syntax-Pragmatics Interface in Language Loss: Covert Restructuring of Aspect in Heritage Russian. PhD thesis, University of Minnesota.Google Scholar
Laleko, O. 2018. What Is Difficult about Grammatical Gender? Evidence from Heritage Russian. Journal of Language Contact 11(2), 233267.Google Scholar
Laleko, O. 2019. Resolving Indeterminacy in Gender Agreement: Comparing Heritage Speakers and L2 Learners of Russian. Heritage Language Journal 16(2), 151182.Google Scholar
Laleko, O. and Polinsky, M.. 2017. Silence Is Difficult: On Missing Elements in Bilingual Grammars. Zeitschrift für Sprachwissenschaft 36(1), 135163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lohndal, T. 2014. Phrase Structure and Argument Structure: A Case Study of the Syntax-Semantics Interface. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Łyskawa, P. and Nagy, N.. 2020. Case Marking Variation in Heritage Slavic Languages in Toronto: Not So Different. Language Learning 70(S1), 122156.Google Scholar
Marantz, A. 1997. No Escape from Syntax: Don’t Try Morphological Analysis in the Privacy of Your Own Lexicon. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics 4(2), 200225.Google Scholar
Ming, T. and Tao, H.. 2008. Developing a Chinese Heritage Language Corpus: Issues and a Preliminary Report. In He, A. W. and Xiao, Y. (eds.), Chinese as a Heritage Language: Fostering Rooted World Citizenry. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Mànoa, 167188.Google Scholar
Mithun, M. 1984. The Evolution of Noun Incorporation. Language 60(4), 847894.Google Scholar
Mithun, M. 1989. The Incipient Obsolescence of Polysynthesis: Cayuga in Ontario and Oklahoma. In Dorian, N. C. (ed.), Investigating Obsolescence: Studies in Language Contraction and Death. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 243257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S. 2004. Subject and Object Expression in Spanish Heritage Speakers: A Case of Morpho-syntactic Convergence. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 7, 125142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S. 2008. Incomplete Acquisition in Bilingualism: Re-examining the Age Factor. Amsterdam: John Benjmains.Google Scholar
Montrul, S. 2016. The Acquisition of Heritage Languages. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Montrul, S. and Bowles, M. A.. 2009. Back to Basics: Differential Object Marking under Incomplete Acquisition in Spanish Heritage Speakers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 12(3), 363383.Google Scholar
Montrul, S. and Potowski, K.. 2007. Command of Gender Agreement in School-Age Spanish-English Bilingual Children. International Journal of Bilingualism 11(3), 301328.Google Scholar
Montrul, S., Bhatt, R. M., and Bhatia, A.. 2012. Erosion of Case and Agreement in Hindi Heritage Speakers. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 2(2), 141176.Google Scholar
Montrul, S., Bhatt, R. M., and Girju, R.. 2015. Differential Object Marking in Spanish, Hindi, and Romanian as Heritage Languages. Language 91(3), 564610.Google Scholar
Montrul, S., Foote, R., and Perpiñán, S.. 2008. Gender Agreement in Adult Second Language Learners and Spanish Heritage Speakers: The Effects of Age and Context of Acquisition. Language Learning 58(3), 503553.Google Scholar
Montrul, S., Bhatia, A., Bhatt, R. M., and Puri, V.. 2019. Case Marking in Hindi as the Weaker Language. Frontiers in Psychology 10(461).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mühlhäuser, P. 1974. Pidginization and Simplification of Language. Canberra: Austrailian National University.Google Scholar
Muravyova, I. A. 2004. Tipologija Incorporatsii [The Typology of Incorporation]. Habilitationsschrift (MS). University of Moscow.Google Scholar
Newell, H., Noonan, M., Piggott, G., and Travis, L. D.. 2017. Introduction. In Newell, H., Noonan, M., Piggot, G., and Travis, L. D. (eds.), The Structure of Words at the Interfaces. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 119.Google Scholar
Nützel, D. and Salmons, J.. 2011. Language Contact and New Dialect Formation: Evidence from German in North America. Language and Linguistics Compass 5(10), 705717.Google Scholar
Ortega, L. 2014. Ways Forward for a Bi/Multilingual Turn in SLA. In May, S. (ed.), The Multilingual Turn: Implications for SLA, TESOL, and Bilingual Education. New York: Routledge, 3253.Google Scholar
Pascual y Cabo, D. 2013. Agreement Reflexes of Emerging Optionality in Heritage Speaker Spanish. PhD thesis, University of Florida.Google Scholar
Payne, T. E. 1997. Describing Morphosyntax: A Guide for Field Linguists. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pfaff, C. W. 1993. Turkish Language Development in Germany. In Extra, G. and Verhoeven, L. T. (eds.), Immigrant Languages in Europe. Bristol: Multilingual Matters, 119146.Google Scholar
Polinsky, M. 1995. Cross-Linguistic Parallels in Language Loss. Southwest Journal of Linguistics 14(1–2), 87123.Google Scholar
Polinsky, M. 2006. Incomplete Acquisition: American Russian. Journal of Slavic Linguistics 14(2), 161219.Google Scholar
Polinsky, M. 2008. Without Aspect. In Corbett, G. and Noonan, M. (eds.), Case and Grammatical Relations. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 263282.Google Scholar
Polinsky, M. 2018. Heritage Languages and Their Speakers. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Polinsky, M. and Scontras, G.. 2020. Understanding Heritage Languages. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 23(1), 420.Google Scholar
Putnam, M. T. and Árnbjörnsdóttir, B.. 2015. Minimizing (Interface) Domains: The Loss of Long-Distance Binding in North American Icelandic. In Page, B. R. and Putnam, M. T. (eds.), Moribund Germanic Heritage Languages in North America. Leiden: Brill, 203223.Google Scholar
Putnam, M. T. and Sánchez, L.. 2013. What’s So Incomplete about Incomplete Acquisition? A Prolegomenon to Modeling Heritage Language Grammars. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 3(4), 478508.Google Scholar
Putnam, M. T., Carlson, M., and Reitter, D.. 2018. Integrated, Not Isolated: Defining Typological Proximity in an Integrated Multilingual Architecture. Frontiers in Psychology 8(2212).Google Scholar
Putnam, M. T., Pascual y Cabo, D., and Kupisch, T.. 2018. Different Situations, Similar Outcomes: Heritage Grammars across the Lifespan. In Bayram, F., Denhovska, N., Miller, D., Rothman, J., and Serratrice, L. (eds.), Bilingual Cognition and Language: The State of the Science across Its Subfields. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 251279.Google Scholar
Ramchand, G. and Svenonius, P.. 2014. Deriving the Functional Hierarchy. Language Sciences 46, 152174.Google Scholar
Riksem, B. R. 2017. Language Mixing and Diachronic Change: American Norwegian Noun Phrases Then and Now. Languages 2(3).Google Scholar
Romaine, S. 1995. Bilingualism, 2nd edition. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Rothman, J. and Treffers-Daller, J.. 2014. A Prolegomenon to the Construct of the Native Speaker: Heritage Speaker Bilinguals Are Natives Too! Applied Linguistics 35(1), 9398.Google Scholar
Schwarz, L. S. 2019. (In)Stability in Heritage Germanic: Examining the Role of Form and Function. PhD thesis, Penn State University.Google Scholar
Scontras, G., Polinsky, M., and Fuchs, Z.. 2015. Heritage Language and Linguistic Theory. Frontiers in Psychology 6(1545).Google Scholar
Scontras, G., Polinsky, M., and Fuchs, Z.. 2018. In Support of Representational Economy: Agreement in Heritage Spanish. Glossa 3(1), 129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sherkina-Lieber, M. 2015. Tense, Aspect, and Agreement in Heritage Labrador Inuttitut: Do Receptive Bilinguals Understand Functional Morphology? Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 5(1), 3061.Google Scholar
Silva-Corvalán, C. 1994. Language Contact and Change: Spanish in Los Angeles. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Silva-Corvalán, C. 2014. Bilingual Language Acquisition: Spanish and English in the First Six Years. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sorace, A. and Filiaci, F.. 2006. Anaphora Resolution in Near-Native Speakers of Italian. Second Language Research 22(3), 339368.Google Scholar
Spencer, A. 1995. Incorporation in Chukchi. Language 71(3), 439489.Google Scholar
Svenonius, P. 2016. Spans and Words. In Siddiqi, D. and Harley, H. (eds.), Morphological Metatheory. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 201222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tóth, G. 2007. Linguistic Interference and First-Language Attrition: German and Hungarian in the San Francisco Bay Area. Frankfurt: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Treffers-Daller, J., Ozsoy, A. S., and van Hout, R.. 2007. (In)complete Acquisition of Turkish among Turkish-English Bilinguals in Germany and Turkey: An Analysis of Complex Embeddings in Narratives. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism 10(3), 248276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wei, L. and Lee, S.. 2001. L1 Development in an l2 Environment: The Use of Cantonese Classifiers and Quantifiers by Young British-Born Chinese in Tyneside. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism 4, 359382.Google Scholar
Wiltschko, M. 2014. The Universal Structure of Categories: Towards a Formal Typology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Woodbury, A. C. 1975. Onondaga Noun Incorporation: Some Notes on the Interdependence of Syntax and Semantics. International Journal of American Linguistics 41(1), 1020.Google Scholar
Woodbury, A. C. 2002. The Word in Cup’ik. In Dixon, R. M. W. and Aikhenvald, A. (eds.), Word: A Cross-linguistic Typology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 7999.Google Scholar
Yager, L., Hellmold, N., Joo, H.-A., Putnam, M. T., Rossi, E., Stafford, C., and Salmons, J.. 2015. New Structural Patterns in Moribund Grammar: Case Marking in Heritage German. Frontiers in Psychology 6(1716).Google Scholar
Zyzik, E. 2020. Creativity and Conventionality in Heritage Language Bilingualism. Language Learning 70(S1), 157187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×