Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-wzw2p Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-30T23:13:47.142Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 7 - Trust and Reputation

How Knowledge about Others Shapes Our Decisions

from Part II - Neuropsychological Level of Trust

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 December 2021

Frank Krueger
Affiliation:
George Mason University, Virginia
Get access

Summary

Forming impressions of trustworthiness about social partners is critical to making adaptive decisions in novel social environments and maintaining healthy interpersonal relationships. However, such impressions can also be biased and lead to maladaptive or harmful trust decisions. In this chapter, we discuss the neurobiological underpinnings of how initial impressions of one’s trustworthiness and the reputation we assign them inform social decision making. We first focus on rapid initial impressions of perceived trustworthiness and how they can be biased by social and contextual information. Next, we review how trustworthiness reputations are learned through novel social interactions and are shaped by prior knowledge, particularly in the context of established interpersonal relationships. Here, we focus primarily on the role of corticostriatal neural systems. Finally, we present emerging avenues of research on how impressions of trustworthiness and acquired reputations guide real-world decision making and are impacted by adverse environments.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2021

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abrahao, B., Parigi, P., Gupta, A., & Cook, K. S. (2017). Reputation offsets trust judgments based on social biases among Airbnb users. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(37), 98489853. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1604234114Google Scholar
Adolphs, R. (2010). What does the amygdala contribute to social cognition?. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1191(1), 4261. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05445.xGoogle Scholar
Adolphs, R., & Spezio, M. (2006). Role of the amygdala in processing visual social stimuli. In Progress in brain research (Vol. 156, pp. 363378). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079–6123(06)56020-0Google Scholar
Alexander, J. K., Hillier, A., Smith, R. M., Tivarus, M. E., & Beversdorf, D. Q. (2007). Beta-adrenergic modulation of cognitive flexibility during stress. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(3), 468478. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.3.468Google Scholar
Ames, D. L., & Fiske, S. T. (2013). Outcome dependency alters the neural substrates of impression formation. NeuroImage, 83, 599608. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.07.001Google Scholar
Amodio, D. M., & Frith, C. D. (2006). Meeting of minds: The medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7(4), 268277. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1884Google Scholar
Arnsten, A. F. T., Wang, M. J., & Paspalas, C. D. (2012). Neuromodulation of thought: Flexibilities and vulnerabilities in prefrontal cortical network synapses. Neuron, 76(1), 223239. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2012.08.038Google Scholar
Balas, B., & Pacella, J. (2017). Trustworthiness perception is disrupted in artificial faces. Computers in Human Behavior, 77, 240248. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.08.045Google Scholar
Baron, S. G., Gobbini, M. I., Engell, A. D., & Todorov, A. (2011). Amygdala and dorsomedial prefrontal cortex responses to appearance-based and behavior-based person impressions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6(5), 572581. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsq086Google Scholar
Bartra, O., McGuire, J. T., & Kable, J. W. (2013). The valuation system: A coordinate-based meta-analysis of BOLD fMRI experiments examining neural correlates of subjective value. NeuroImage, 76, 412427. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.02.063Google Scholar
Bault, N., Pelloux, B., Fahrenfort, J. J., Ridderinkhof, K. R., & Van Winden, F. (2015). Neural dynamics of social tie formation in economic decision making. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(6), 877884. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsu138CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumgartner, T., Heinrichs, M., Vonlanthen, A., Fischbacher, U., & Fehr, E. (2008). Oxytocin shapes the neural circuitry of trust and trust adaptation in humans. Neuron, 58(4), 639650. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.009Google Scholar
Bellucci, G., Chernyak, S. V., Goodyear, K., Eickhoff, S. B., & Krueger, F. (2017). Neural signatures of trust in reciprocity: A coordinate-based meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 38(3), 12331248. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23451Google Scholar
Bellucci, G., Hahn, T., Deshpande, G., & Krueger, F. (2018). Functional connectivity of specific resting-state networks predicts trust and reciprocity in the trust game. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 19(1), 165176. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-018-00654-3Google Scholar
Bellucci, G., Molter, F., & Park, S. Q. (2019). Neural representations of honesty predict future trust behavior. Nature Communications, 10(1), Article 5184. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-019-13261-8Google Scholar
Benuzzi, F., Pugnaghi, M., Meletti, S., et al. (2007). Processing the socially relevant parts of faces. Brain Research Bulletin, 74(5), 344356. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresbull.2007.07.010Google Scholar
Berg Nødtvedt, K., Sjåstad, H., Skard, S., Thorbjørnsen, H., & Van Bavel, J. (2020). Racial bias in the sharing economy and the role of trust and self-congruence. Social Science Research Network. https://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3434463Google Scholar
Berger, J., Heinrichs, M., von Dawans, B., Way, B. M., & Chen, F. S. (2016). Cortisol modulates men’s affiliative responses to acute social stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 63, 19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.09.004Google Scholar
Bhanji, J. P., & Delgado, M. R. (2014). Perceived control influences neural responses to setbacks and promotes persistence. Neuron, 83(6), 13691375. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2014.08.012Google Scholar
Bogdan, R., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2006). Acute stress reduces reward responsiveness: Implications for depression. Biological Psychiatry, 60(10), 11471154. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.037Google Scholar
Bonnefon, J.-F., Hopfensitz, A., & De Neys, W. (2013). The modular nature of trustworthiness detection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(1), 143150. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028930Google Scholar
Brambilla, M., Biella, M., & Freeman, J. B. (2018). The influence of visual context on the evaluation of facial trustworthiness. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 78, 3442. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2018.04.011Google Scholar
Brudner, E. G., Khalil, A., & Delgado, M. R. (2019, August). The upside of social media: The influence of peer ‘likes’ on adaptive social behavior. Flux Congress, New York, NY.Google Scholar
Buchanan, T. W., & Passarelli, T. O. (2020). How do stress and social closeness impact prosocial behavior? Experimental Psychology, 67(2), 123131. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000482Google Scholar
Bzdok, D., Langner, R., Caspers, S., et al. (2011). ALE meta-analysis on facial judgments of trustworthiness and attractiveness. Brain Structure and Function, 215(3–4), 209223. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-010-0287-4Google Scholar
Campellone, T. R., & Kring, A. M. (2013). Who do you trust? The impact of facial emotion and behaviour on decision making. Cognition & Emotion, 27(4), 603620. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2012.726608CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chang, L. J., Doll, B. B., van’t Wout, M., Frank, M. J., & Sanfey, A. G. (2010). Seeing is believing: Trustworthiness as a dynamic belief. Cognitive Psychology, 61(2), 87105. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.03.001Google Scholar
Chang, S.-A. A., & Baskin-Sommers, A. (2020). Living in a disadvantaged neighborhood affects neural processing of facial trustworthiness. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, Article 409. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00409Google Scholar
Charness, G., Du, N., & Yang, C.-L. (2011). Trust and trustworthiness reputations in an investment game. Games and Economic Behavior, 72(2), 361375. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.geb.2010.09.002Google Scholar
Chein, J., Albert, D., O’Brien, L., Uckert, K., & Steinberg, L. (2011). Peers increase adolescent risk taking by enhancing activity in the brain’s reward circuitry. Developmental Science, 14(2), F1F10. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01035.xGoogle Scholar
Corral-Frías, N. S., Nikolova, Y. S., Michalski, L. J., Baranger, D. A. A., Hariri, A. R., & Bogdan, R. (2015). Stress-related anhedonia is associated with ventral striatum reactivity to reward and transdiagnostic psychiatric symptomatology. Psychological Medicine, 45(12), 26052617. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291715000525Google Scholar
Daw, N. D., O’Doherty, J. P., Dayan, P., Seymour, B., & Dolan, R. J. (2006). Cortical substrates for exploratory decisions in humans. Nature, 441(7095), 876879. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature04766Google Scholar
Declerck, C. H., Boone, C., Pauwels, L., Vogt, B., & Fehr, E. (2020). A registered replication study on oxytocin and trust. Nature Human Behaviour, 4(6), 646655. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-0878-xGoogle Scholar
Delgado, M. R., Frank, R. H., & Phelps, E. A. (2005). Perceptions of moral character modulate the neural systems of reward during the trust game. Nature Neuroscience, 8(11), 16111618. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1575Google Scholar
De Neys, W., Hopfensitz, A., & Bonnefon, J. F. (2017). Split-second trustworthiness detection from faces in an economic game. Experimental Psychology, 64(4), 231239. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000367Google Scholar
Dillon, D. G., Holmes, A. J., Birk, J. L., Brooks, N., Lyons-Ruth, K., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2009). Childhood adversity is associated with left basal ganglia dysfunction during reward anticipation in adulthood. Biological Psychiatry, 66(3), 206213. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.02.019Google Scholar
Ditzen, B., & Heinrichs, M. (2014). Psychobiology of social support: The social dimension of stress buffering. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 32(1), 149162. https://doi.org/10.3233/RNN-139008Google Scholar
Duncko, R., Johnson, L., Merikangas, K., & Grillon, C. (2009). Working memory performance after acute exposure to the cold pressor stress in healthy volunteers. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 91(4), 377381. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2009.01.006Google Scholar
Eisenberger, N. I., Taylor, S. E., Gable, S. L., Hilmert, C. J., & Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Neural pathways link social support to attenuated neuroendocrine stress responses. NeuroImage, 35(4), 16011612. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.01.038Google Scholar
Elzinga, B. M., & Roelofs, K. (2005). Cortisol-induced impairments of working memory require acute sympathetic activation. Behavioral Neuroscience, 119(1), 98103. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.119.1.98Google Scholar
Engell, A. D., Haxby, J. V., & Todorov, A. (2007). Implicit trustworthiness decisions: Automatic coding of face properties in the human amygdala. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(9), 15081519. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.9.1508Google Scholar
Engelmann, J. B., Meyer, F., Ruff, C. C., & Fehr, E. (2019). The neural circuitry of affect-induced distortions of trust. Science Advances, 5(3), Article eaau3413. https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.aau3413Google Scholar
Everett, J. A. C., Pizarro, D. A., & Crockett, M. J. (2016). Inference of trustworthiness from intuitive moral judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(6), 772787. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000165Google Scholar
Ewing, L., Caulfield, F., Read, A., & Rhodes, G. (2015). Perceived trustworthiness of faces drives trust behaviour in children. Developmental Science, 18(2), 327334. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12218Google Scholar
Falvello, V., Vinson, M., Ferrari, C., & Todorov, A. (2015). The robustness of learning about the trustworthiness of other people. Social Cognition, 33(5), 368386. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2015.33.5.368CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fareri, D. S., Chang, L. J., & Delgado, M. R. (2012). Effects of direct social experience on trust decisions and neural reward circuitry. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 6(148), 117. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2012.00148CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fareri, D. S., Chang, L. J., & Delgado, M. R. (2015). Computational substrates of social value in interpersonal collaboration. Journal of Neuroscience, 35(21), 81708180. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.4775-14.2015Google Scholar
Fareri, D. S., Smith, D. V., & Delgado, M. R. (2020). The influence of relationship closeness on default-mode network connectivity during social interactions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 15(3), 261271. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsaa031Google Scholar
FeldmanHall, O., Dunsmoor, J. E., Tompary, A., Hunter, L. E., Todorov, A., & Phelps, E. A. (2018). Stimulus generalization as a mechanism for learning to trust. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(7), E1690E1697. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1715227115CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
FeldmanHall, O., Raio, C. M., Kubota, J. T., Seiler, M. G., & Phelps, E. A. (2015). The effects of social context and acute stress on decision making under uncertainty. Psychological Science, 26(12), 19181926. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615605807CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferrari, C., Lega, C., Vernice, M., et al. (2014). The dorsomedial prefrontal cortex plays a causal role in integrating social impressions from faces and verbal descriptions. Cerebral Cortex, 26(1), 156165. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhu186CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fett, A. K. J., Gromann, P. M., Giampietro, V., Shergill, S. S., & Krabbendam, L. (2014). Default distrust? An fMRI investigation of the neural development of trust and cooperation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(4), 395402. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nss144Google Scholar
Fiske, S. T., & Dupree, C. (2014). Gaining trust as well as respect in communicating to motivated audiences about science topics. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(Suppl. 4), 1359313597. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1317505111Google Scholar
Fouragnan, E., Chierchia, G., Greiner, S., Neveu, R., Avesani, P., & Coricelli, G. (2013). Reputational priors magnify striatal responses to violations of trust. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(8), 36023611. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.3086-12.2013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, J. B., Stolier, R. M., Ingbretsen, Z. A., & Hehman, E. A. (2014). Amygdala responsivity to high-level social information from unseen faces. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(32), 1057310581. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.5063-13.2014Google Scholar
Friston, K. J. (2011). Functional and effective connectivity: A review. Brain Connectivity, 1(1), 1336. https://doi.org/10.1089/brain.2011.0008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fujino, J., Tei, S., Itahashi, T., et al. (2020). Role of the right temporoparietal junction in intergroup bias in trust decisions. Human Brain Mapping, 41(6), 16771688. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24903Google Scholar
Gheorghiu, A. I., Callan, M. J., & Skylark, W. J. (2017). Facial appearance affects science communication. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(23), 59705975. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1620542114Google Scholar
Gluckman, M., & Johnson, S. P. (2013). Attentional capture by social stimuli in young infants. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, Article 527. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00527CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 14641480. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464Google Scholar
Hackel, L. M., Doll, B. B., & Amodio, D. M. (2015). Instrumental learning of traits versus rewards: Dissociable neural correlates and effects on choice. Nature Neuroscience, 18(9), 12331235. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4080CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hackel, L. M., Wills, J. A., & Van Bavel, J. J. (2020). Shifting prosocial intuitions: Neurocognitive evidence for a value-based account of group-based cooperation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 15(4), 371381. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsaa055CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heatherton, T. F. (2011). Neuroscience of self and self-regulation. Annual Review of Psychology, 62(1), 363390. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.121208.131616Google Scholar
Hetherington, M. J. (1999). The effect of political trust on the presidential vote, 1968–96. American Political Science Review, 93(2), 311326. https://doi.org/10.2307/2585398CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, B. L., Ambady, N., & Zaki, J. (2017). Trusting outgroup, but not ingroup members, requires control: Neural and behavioral evidence. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(3), 372381. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw139CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, B. L., & Zaki, J. (2015). The neuroscience of motivated cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(2), 6264. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.12.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, B. L., Zaki, J., & Ambady, N. (2017). Motivation alters impression formation and related neural systems. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(1), 4960. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw147Google Scholar
Huijsmans, I., Ma, I., Micheli, L., Civai, C., Stallen, M., & Sanfey, A. G. (2019). A scarcity mindset alters neural processing underlying consumer decision making. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(24), 1169911704. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1818572116Google Scholar
Ide, J. S., Nedic, S., Wong, K. F., et al. (2018). Oxytocin attenuates trust as a subset of more general reinforcement learning, with altered reward circuit functional connectivity in males. NeuroImage, 174, 3543. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.02.035Google Scholar
Jachimowicz, J. M., Chafik, S., Munrat, S., Prabhu, J. C., & Weber, E. U. (2017). Community trust reduces myopic decisions of low-income individuals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(21), 54015406. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1617395114CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jaeger, B., Todorov, A. T., Evans, A. M., & Van Beest, I. (2020). Can we reduce facial biases? Persistent effects of facial trustworthiness on sentencing decisions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 90, Article 104004. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2020.104004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jirsaraie, R. J., Ranby, K. W., & Albeck, D. S. (2019). Early life stress moderates the relationship between age and prosocial behaviors. Child Abuse & Neglect, 94, Article 104029. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2019.104029Google Scholar
Keres, A., & Chartier, C. R. (2016). The biasing effects of visual background on perceived facial trustworthiness. Psi Chi Journal of Psychological Research, 21(3), 170175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
King-Casas, B. (2005). Getting to know you: Reputation and trust in a two-person economic exchange. Science, 308(5718), 7883. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1108062CrossRefGoogle Scholar
King-Casas, B., Tomlin, D., Anen, C., Camerer, C. F., Quartz, S. R., & Montague, P. R. (2005). Getting to know you: Reputation and trust in a two-person economic exchange. Science, 308(5718), 7883. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1108062Google Scholar
Kliemann, D., Young, L., Scholz, J., & Saxe, R. (2008). The influence of prior record on moral judgment. Neuropsychologia, 46(12), 29492957. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.06.010Google Scholar
Kosfeld, M., Heinrichs, M., Zak, P. J., Fischbacher, U., & Fehr, E. (2005). Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature, 435(7042), 673676. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature03701Google Scholar
Krosch, A. R., & Amodio, D. M. (2014). Economic scarcity alters the perception of race. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(25), 90799084. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1404448111Google Scholar
Krosch, A. R., & Amodio, D. M. (2019). Scarcity disrupts the neural encoding of Black faces: A socioperceptual pathway to discrimination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 117(5), 859875. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000168Google Scholar
Krumhuber, E., Manstead, A. S. R., Cosker, D., Marshall, D., Rosin, P. L., & Kappas, A. (2007). Facial dynamics as indicators of trustworthiness and cooperative behavior. Emotion, 7(4), 730735. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.730CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kubota, J. T., Li, J., Bar-David, E., Banaji, M. R., & Phelps, E. A. (2013). The price of racial bias: Intergroup negotiations in the ultimatum game. Psychological Science, 24(12), 24982504. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613496435CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 108(3), 480498. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.108.3.480Google Scholar
Lambert, B., Declerck, C. H., Emonds, G., & Boone, C. (2017). Trust as commodity: Social value orientation affects the neural substrates of learning to cooperate. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(4), 609617. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw170Google Scholar
Li, D., Meng, L., & Ma, Q. (2017). Who deserves my trust? Cue-elicited feedback negativity tracks reputation learning in repeated social interactions. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11, Article 307. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00307Google Scholar
Li, H., Song, Y., & Xie, X. (2019). Altruistic or selfish? Responses when safety is threatened depend on childhood socioeconomic status. European Journal of Social Psychology, 50(5), 10011016. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2651Google Scholar
Li, J., Delgado, M. R., & Phelps, E. A. (2011). How instructed knowledge modulates the neural systems of reward learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(1), 5560. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1014938108Google Scholar
Luethi, M., Meier, B., & Sandi, C. (2009). Stress effects on working memory, explicit memory, and implicit memory for neutral and emotional stimuli in healthy men. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 2(5). https://doi.org/10.3389/neuro.08.005.2008Google Scholar
Margittai, Z., Strombach, T., Van Wingerden, M., Joëls, M., Schwabe, L., & Kalenscher, T. (2015). A friend in need: Time-dependent effects of stress on social discounting in men. Hormones and Behavior, 73, 7582. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.05.019Google Scholar
Marzi, T., Righi, S., Ottonello, S., Cincotta, M., & Viggiano, M. P. (2014). Trust at first sight: Evidence from ERPs. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(1), 6372. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nss102Google Scholar
Mather, M., & Lighthall, N. R. (2012). Risk and reward are processed differently in decisions made under stress. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 3641. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411429452CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mende-Siedlecki, P., Said, C. P., & Todorov, A. (2013). The social evaluation of faces: A meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(3), 285299. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsr090Google Scholar
Mende-Siedlecki, P., Verosky, S. C., Turk-Browne, N. B., & Todorov, A. (2013). Robust selectivity for faces in the human amygdala in the absence of expressions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25(12), 20862106. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00469Google Scholar
Morelli, S. A., Leong, Y. C., Carlson, R. W., Kullar, M., & Zaki, J. (2018). Neural detection of socially valued community members. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(32), 81498154. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1712811115Google Scholar
Montague, P. R., Berns, G. S., Cohen, J. D., et al. (2002). Hyperscanning: Simultaneous fMRI during linked social interactions. NeuroImage, 16(4), 11591164. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1150Google Scholar
O’Doherty, J., Dayan, P., Schultz, J., Deichmann, R., Friston, K., & Dolan, R. J. (2004). Dissociable roles of ventral and dorsal striatum in instrumental conditioning. Science, 304(5669), 452454. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1094285Google Scholar
O’Doherty, J. P., Lee, S. W., & McNamee, D. (2015). The structure of reinforcement-learning mechanisms in the human brain. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 1, 94100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2014.10.004Google Scholar
Oh, D., Shafir, E., & Todorov, A. (2020). Economic status cues from clothes affect perceived competence from faces. Nature Human Behaviour, 4(3), 287293. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-019-0782-4Google Scholar
Olcaysoy Okten, I., Magerman, A., & Forbes, C. E. (2020). Behavioral and neural indices of trust formation in cross-race and same-race interactions. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 13(2), 100125. https://doi.org/10.1037/npe0000127Google Scholar
Olivola, C. Y., & Todorov, A. (2010). Elected in 100 milliseconds: Appearance-based trait inferences and voting. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 34(2), 83110. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-009-0082-1Google Scholar
Oosterhof, N. N., & Todorov, A. (2009). Shared perceptual basis of emotional expressions and trustworthiness impressions from faces. Emotion, 9(1), 128133. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014520Google Scholar
Park, B., Fareri, D., Delgado, M., & Young, L. (2020). The role of right temporoparietal junction in processing social prediction error across relationship contexts. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsaa072Google Scholar
Park, B., & Young, L. (2020). An association between biased impression updating and relationship facilitation: A behavioral and fMRI investigation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 87, Article 103916. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2019.103916Google Scholar
Phan, K. L., Sripada, C. S., Angstadt, M., & McCabe, K. (2010). Reputation for reciprocity engages the brain reward center. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(29), 1309913104. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1008137107Google Scholar
Plessow, F., Fischer, R., Kirschbaum, C., & Goschke, T. (2011). Inflexibly focused under stress: Acute psychosocial stress increases shielding of action goals at the expense of reduced cognitive flexibility with increasing time lag to the stressor. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(11), 32183227. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00024Google Scholar
Plessow, F., Kiesel, A., & Kirschbaum, C. (2012). The stressed prefrontal cortex and goal-directed behaviour: Acute psychosocial stress impairs the flexible implementation of task goals. Experimental Brain Research, 216(3), 397408. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2943-1Google Scholar
Porcelli, A. J., & Delgado, M. R. (2017). Stress and decision making: Effects on valuation, learning, and risk-taking. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 14, 3339. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.11.015Google Scholar
Porcelli, A. J., Lewis, A. H., & Delgado, M. R. (2012). Acute stress influences neural circuits of reward processing. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 6(157). https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2012.00157CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Radell, M. L., Sanchez, R., Weinflash, N., & Myers, C. E. (2016). The personality trait of behavioral inhibition modulates perceptions of moral character and performance during the trust game: Behavioral results and computational modeling. PeerJ, 4, Article e1631. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.1631Google Scholar
Raio, C. M., Orederu, T. A., Palazzolo, L., Shurick, A. A., & Phelps, E. A. (2013). Cognitive emotion regulation fails the stress test. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(37), 1513915144. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1305706110Google Scholar
Raposa, E. B., Laws, H. B., & Ansell, E. B. (2015). Prosocial behavior mitigates the negative effects of stress in everyday life. Clinical Psychological Science, 4(4), 691698. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702615611073Google Scholar
Rescorla, R., & Wagner, A. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In Black, A. H. & Prokasy, W. F. (Eds)., Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory (Vol. 2, pp. 6499). Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Rezlescu, C., Duchaine, B., Olivola, C. Y., & Chater, N. (2012). Unfakeable facial configurations affect strategic choices in trust games with or without information about past behavior. PLoS ONE, 7(3), Article e34293. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0034293Google Scholar
Roozendaal, B., McReynolds, J. R., Van der Zee, E. A., Lee, S., McGaugh, J. L., & McIntyre, C. K. (2009). Glucocorticoid effects on memory consolidation depend on functional interactions between the medial prefrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(45), 1429914308. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3626-09.2009Google Scholar
Rule, N. O., Freeman, J. B., Moran, J. M., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Adams, R. B., & Ambady, N. (2010). Voting behavior is reflected in amygdala response across cultures. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 5(2–3), 349355. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsp046CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rule, N. O., Krendl, A. C., Ivcevic, Z., & Ambady, N. (2013). Accuracy and consensus in judgments of trustworthiness from faces: Behavioral and neural correlates. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(3), 409426. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031050Google Scholar
Said, C. P., Baron, S. G., & Todorov, A. (2009). Nonlinear amygdala response to face trustworthiness: Contributions of high and low spatial frequency information. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(3), 519528. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21041Google Scholar
Salam, A. P., Rainford, E., Van Vugt, M., & Ronay, R. (2017). Acute stress reduces perceived trustworthiness of male racial outgroup faces. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology, 3(4), 282292. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40750-017-0065-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Santos, S., Almeida, I., Oliveiros, B., & Castelo-Branco, M. (2016). The role of the amygdala in facial trustworthiness processing: A systematic review and meta-analyses of fMRI Studies. PLoS ONE, 11(11), Article e0167276. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0167276Google Scholar
Schilke, O., Reimann, M., & Cook, K. S. (2013). Effect of relationship experience on trust recovery following a breach. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(38), 1523615241. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1314857110Google Scholar
Schoofs, D., Wolf, O. T., & Smeets, T. (2009). Cold pressor stress impairs performance on working memory tasks requiring executive functions in healthy young men. Behavioral Neuroscience, 123(5), 10661075. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016980Google Scholar
Schultz, W., Dayan, P., & Montague, P. R. (1997). A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science, 275(5306), 15931599. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.275.5306.1593Google Scholar
Schweda, A., Faber, N. S., Crockett, M. J., & Kalenscher, T. (2019). The effects of psychosocial stress on intergroup resource allocation. Scientific Reports, 9(1), Article 18620. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-54954-wCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seeley, W. W., Menon, V., Schatzberg, A. F., et al. (2007). Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(9), 23492356. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.5587-06.2007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shteingart, H., & Loewenstein, Y. (2014). Reinforcement learning and human behavior. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 25, 9398. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2013.12.004Google Scholar
Sofer, C., Dotsch, R., Oikawa, M., Oikawa, H., Wigboldus, D. H. J., & Todorov, A. (2017). For your local eyes only: Culture-specific face typicality influences perceptions of trustworthiness. Perception, 46(8), 914928. https://doi.org/10.1177/0301006617691786Google Scholar
Stanley, D. A. (2016). Getting to know you: General and specific neural computations for learning about people. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(4), 525536. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsv145Google Scholar
Stanley, D. A., Sokol-Hessner, P., Banaji, M. R., & Phelps, E. A. (2011). Implicit race attitudes predict trustworthiness judgments and economic trust decisions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(19), 77107715. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1014345108Google Scholar
Stanley, D. A., Sokol-Hessner, P., Fareri, D. S., et al. (2012). Race and reputation: Perceived racial group trustworthiness influences the neural correlates of trust decisions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1589), 744753. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0300Google Scholar
Steinbeis, N., Engert, V., Linz, R., & Singer, T. (2015). The effects of stress and affiliation on social decision making: Investigating the tend-and-befriend pattern. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 62, 138148. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.08.003Google Scholar
Sutton, R. S., & Barto, A. G. (1998). Reinforcement learning: An introduction. The MIT Press. https://muse.jhu.edu/book/60836Google Scholar
Suzuki, A., Ito, Y., Kiyama, S., et al. (2016). Involvement of the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex in learning others’ bad reputations and indelible distrust. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, Article 28. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00028Google Scholar
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In Worchel, S. and Austin, W. G. (Eds.), Psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 724). Hall Publishers.Google Scholar
Tashjian, S. M., Guassi Moreira, J. F., & Galván, A. (2019). Multivoxel pattern analysis reveals a neural phenotype for trust bias in adolescents. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 31(11), 17261741. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01448Google Scholar
Taylor, S. E. (2006). Tend and befriend: Biobehavioral bases of affiliation under stress. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(6), 273277. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00451.xGoogle Scholar
Taylor, S. E., & Master, S. L. (2011). Social responses to stress: The tend-and-befriend model. In Contrada, R. J. & Baum, A. (Eds.), The handbook of stress science: Biology, psychology, and health (pp. 101109). Springer.Google Scholar
Theeuwes, J., & Van der Stigchel, S. (2006). Faces capture attention: Evidence from inhibition of return. Visual Cognition, 13(6), 657665. https://doi.org/10.1080/13506280500410949Google Scholar
Todorov, A. (2012). The role of the amygdala in face perception and evaluation. Motivation and Emotion, 36(1), 1626. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-011-9238-5Google Scholar
Todorov, A., Baron, S. G., & Oosterhof, N. N. (2008). Evaluating face trustworthiness: A model based approach. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 3(2), 119127. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsn009Google Scholar
Todorov, A., Mandisodza, A. N., Goren, A., & Hall, C. C. (2005). Inferences of competence from faces predict election outcomes. Science, 308(5728), 16231626. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1110589Google Scholar
Todorov, A., Mende-Siedlecki, P., & Dotsch, R. (2013). Social judgments from faces. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23(3), 373380. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2012.12.010Google Scholar
Todorov, A., Olivola, C. Y., Dotsch, R., & Mende-Siedlecki, P. (2015). Social attributions from faces: Determinants, consequences, accuracy, and functional significance. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 519545. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143831Google Scholar
Todorov, A., Pakrashi, M., & Oosterhof, N. N. (2009). Evaluating faces on trustworthiness after minimal time exposure. Social Cognition, 27(6), 813833. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2009.27.6.813CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Utevsky, A. V., Smith, D. V., Young, J. S., & Huettel, S. A. (2017). Large-scale network coupling with the fusiform cortex facilitates future social motivation. ENeuro, 4(5). https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0084-17.2017Google Scholar
Van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2012). Differential susceptibility experiments: Going beyond correlational evidence: Comment on beyond mental health, differential susceptibility articles. Developmental Psychology, 48(3), 769774. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027536Google Scholar
Van’t Wout, M., & Sanfey, A. G. (2008). Friend or foe: The effect of implicit trustworthiness judgments in social decision making. Cognition, 108(3), 796803. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.07.002Google Scholar
Vanyukov, P. M., Hallquist, M. N., Delgado, M., Szanto, K., & Dombrovski, A. Y. (2019). Neurocomputational mechanisms of adaptive learning in social exchanges. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 19(4), 985997. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-019-00697-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vieira, J., Schellhaas, S., Enström, E., & Olsson, A. (2020). Help or flight? Increased threat imminence promotes defensive helping in humans. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/bckn3Google Scholar
von Dawans, B., Ditzen, B., Trueg, A., Fischbacher, U., & Heinrichs, M. (2019). Effects of acute stress on social behavior in women. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 99, 137144. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.08.031Google Scholar
von Dawans, B., Fischbacher, U., Kirschbaum, C., Fehr, E., & Heinrichs, M. (2012). The social dimension of stress reactivity: Acute stress increases prosocial behavior in humans. Psychological Science, 23(6), 651660. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611431576Google Scholar
Wardle, M. C., Fitzgerald, D. A., Angstadt, M., Sripada, C. S., McCabe, K., & Luan Phan, K. (2013). The caudate signals bad reputation during trust decisions. PLoS ONE, 8(6), Article e68884. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0068884Google Scholar
Williams, K. D., Cheung, C. K. T., & Choi, W. (2000). Cyberostracism: Effects of being ignored over the Internet. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(5), 748762. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.5.748Google Scholar
Wilson, J. P., & Rule, N. O. (2015). Facial trustworthiness predicts extreme criminal-sentencing outcomes. Psychological Science, 26(8), 13251331. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615590992CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winston, J. S., Strange, B. A., O’Doherty, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2002). Automatic and intentional brain responses during evaluation of trustworthiness of faces. Nature Neuroscience, 5(3), 277283. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn816Google Scholar
Wu, C.-T., Fan, Y.-T., Du, Y.-R., et al. (2018). How do acquired political identities influence our neural processing toward others within the context of a trust game? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12, Article 23. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00023Google Scholar
Yan, X., Yong, X., Huang, W., & Ma, Y. (2018). Placebo treatment facilitates social trust and approach behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(22), 57325737. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1800779115Google Scholar
Zaki, J., Kallman, S., Wimmer, G. E., Ochsner, K., & Shohamy, D. (2016). Social cognition as reinforcement learning: Feedback modulates emotion inference. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 28(9), 12701282. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00978Google Scholar
Zarolia, P., Weisbuch, M., & McRae, K. (2017). Influence of indirect information on interpersonal trust despite direct information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 112(1), 3957. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000074Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×