Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-m9kch Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-06T09:29:52.625Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Bibliography

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 August 2010

Richard S. Hallam
Affiliation:
University of Greenwich
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Virtual Selves, Real Persons
A Dialogue across Disciplines
, pp. 314 - 330
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abbey, Ruth. 2000. Charles Taylor. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Abelson, Raziel. 1977. Persons: A study in philosophical psychology. London: Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Althusser, Louis. 1969. For Marx. Trans. Brewster, Ben. London: Allen Lane.Google Scholar
Andrews, , John, D. W. 1989. Integrating visions of reality: Interpersonal diagnosis and the existential vision. American Psychologist, 44: 803–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andrews, Kristin. 2008. It's in your nature: A pluralistic folk psychology. Synthese, 165: 13–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arbib, Michael A. 2005. From monkey-like action recognition to human language: An evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28: 105–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arendt, Hannah. 1978. The life of the mind. Vol. II: Willing. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovitch.Google Scholar
Astington, Janet. 1996. What is theoretical about the child's theory of mind? A Vygotskyan view of its development. In Theories of theories of mind (pp. 184–99). Eds. Carruthers, Peter and Smith, Peter K.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Austin, John L. 1964. Sense and sensibilia. Reconstructed from the manuscript notes by Warnock, G. J.. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Baars, Bernard J. 1997. In the theater of consciousness: The workspace of the mind. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Back, Kurt. 1989. Thriller: The self in modern society. In Texts of identity (pp. 207–36). Eds. Shotter, John and Gergen, Kenneth. London: Sage.Google Scholar
Bargh, John A. and McKenna, Katelyn Y.A.. 2004. The internet and social life. Annual Review of Psychology, 55: 573–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bayne, Timothy. 2006. Phenomenology and the feeling of doing: Wegner on the conscious will. In Does consciousness cause behavior? (pp.169–85). Eds. Pockett, Susan, Banks, William P. and Gallagher, Shaun. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Benediktson, D. Thomas. 2006. The first silent reader of Latin literature. Classical World, 100: 43–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bennett, Max R. and Hacker, Peter. 2003. Philosophical foundations of neuroscience. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bennett, Max R., Dennett, Daniel, Hacker, Peter and Searle, John. 2007. Neuroscience and philosophy: Brain, mind, and language. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Benton, Ted and Craib, Ian. 2001. Philosophy of social science: The philosophical foundations of social thought. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Bermúdez, José L. 1998. The paradox of self-consciousness. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bermúdez, José L. 2003. The domain of folk psychology. In Minds and persons: Royal Institute of Philosophy, Supplement 53 (pp.25–49). Ed. O'Hear, Anthony. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Berrios, German E. and Marková, Ivana S.. 2003. The self and psychiatry: A conceptual history. In The self in neuroscience and psychiatry (pp.9–39). Eds. Kircher, Tilo and David, Anthony. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Beyle, Henri. 1949. Stendahl: Memoirs of an egotist. Trans. Earp, T.W.. London: Turnstile Press.Google Scholar
Bhaskar, Roy. 1979. The Possibility of Naturalism: A Philosophical Critique of the Contemporary Human Sciences. Brighton: Harvester Press.Google Scholar
Blackman, Derek E. 1991. Skinner, B. F. and Mead, G. H.: On biological science and social science. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 55: 251–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blasi, Augusto. 2004. Neither personality nor cognition: An alternative approach to the nature of the self. In Changing conceptions of psychological life. Eds. Chandler, Michael, Chris, Lalonde, and Lightfoot, Cynthia. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Boesch, Christophe. 2005. Joint cooperative hunting among wild chimpanzees: Taking natural observations seriously. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 28: 692–3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bourdieu, Pierre. 1977. Outline of a theory of practice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyer, Pascal. 2000. Evolution of the modern mind and the origins of culture: Religious concepts as a limiting case. In Evolution and the human mind: Modularity, language and meta-cognition (pp.93–112). Eds. Carruthers, Peter and Chamberlain, Andrew. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Boyle, Mary. 2002. Schizophrenia: A scientific delusion? 2nd edn. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Brand, Myles. 1984. Intending and acting: Towards a naturalized action theory. Cambridge, Mass: Bradford, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bratman, Michael E. 2007. Structures of agency: Essays. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braude, Stephen E. 1995. First person plural: Multiple personality and the philosophy of mind. Lanham, Md.: Rowman & Littlefield.Google Scholar
Brockmeier, Jens. 2005. The text of the mind. In Mind as a scientific object (pp.432–49). Eds. Erneling, Christina E. and Johnson, David M.. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, Jerome. 1992. Acts of meaning: Four lectures on mind and culture. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, Jerome. 1997. A narrative model of self-construction. In The self across psychology: Self-recognition, self-awareness, and the self-concept (pp.145–61). Eds. Snodgrass, Joan G. and Thompson, Robert L.. New York: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
Burkitt, Ian. 1991. Social selves: Theories of the social formation of personality. London: Sage.Google Scholar
Byrne, Richard W. 1999. Human cognitive evolution. In The descent of mind: Psychological perspectives on hominid evolution (pp.71–87). Eds. Corballis, Michael C. and Lea, Stephen E.G.. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Byrne, Richard W. and Whiten, Andrew. 1988. Tactical deception of familiar individuals in baboons. In Machiavellian intelligence: Social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes and humans (pp.205–10). Eds. Byrne, Richard W. and Whiten, Andrew. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Calvert-Minor, Chris. 2008. The ‘strong programme’, normativity, and social causes. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 38: 1–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cannon, Betty. 2003. Sartre's contribution to psychoanalysis. In Understanding experience: Psychotherapy and post-modernism (pp.27–51). Ed. Frie, Roger. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Carpendale, Jeremy and Lewis, Charlie. 2004. Constructing an understanding of mind: The development of children's social understanding within social interaction. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27: 79–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carr, David. 1986. Time, narrative and history. Bloomington, Ind.: Indiana University PressGoogle Scholar
Carr, David. 1997. Narrative and the real world: An argument for continuity. In Memory, identity, community: The idea of narrative in the human sciences (pp.7–25). Eds. Hinchman, Lewis P. and Hinchman, Sandras K.. Albany, NY: SUNY Press.Google Scholar
Carruthers, Peter. 2004. The nature of the mind: An introduction. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Cazeneuve, Jean. 1972. Lucien Levy-Bruhl. Trans. Riviere, P.. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Cheney, Dorothy L. and Seyfarth, Robert M.. 1990. How monkeys see the world. Chicago, Ill.: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Clark, Andy. 2008. Memento's revenge: The extended mind, re-visited. Available at http://consc.net/mindpapers/2.2i (accessed 28 December 2008).
Clark, Andy and Chalmers, David J.. 1998. The extended mind. Analysis, 58: 7–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Claxton, Guy. 2005. The wayward mind: An intimate history of the unconscious. London: Little, Brown.Google Scholar
Collingwood, Robin G. 1961a. Human nature and human history. In The idea of history (pp.205–31). Oxford: Oxford University Press. First published in 1936.Google Scholar
Collingwood, Robin G. 1961b. The idea of history. Oxford: Oxford University Press. First published in 1946.Google Scholar
Cooney, Jeffrey W. and Gazzaniga, Michael S.. 2003. Neurological disorders and the structure of human consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Science, 7: 161–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooper, Anthony Ashley, Third Earl of Shaftesbury. 1981. Complete works, selected letters, and posthumous writings. In English with parallel German translations. Ed. and trans. Hemmerich, Gerd and Benda, Wolfram. Stuttgart: Frommann-Holzboog.Google Scholar
Costall, Alan and Leudar, Ivan. 2004. Where is the ‘theory’ in theory of mind. Theory and Psychology, 14: 623–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coulter, Jeff. 1983. Rethinking cognitive theory. New York: St Martin's Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coulter, Jeff. 1999. Discourse and mind. Human Studies, 22: 163–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coulter, Jeff and Sharrock, Wes. 2007. Brain, mind and human behavior in contemporary cognitive science. New York: Edwin Mellen Press.Google Scholar
Crabbe, M. James C. 1999. From soul to self. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Craik, Fergus I.M., Moroz, Tara M., Moscovitch, Morris, Stuss, Donald, Winocur, Gordon, Tulving, Endel and Kapur, Shitij. 1999. In search of the self: A positron emission tomography study. Psychological Science, 10: 26–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cushman, Phillip. 1990. Why the self is empty: Toward a historically situated psychology. American Psychologist, 45: 599–611.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Damasio, Antonio. 2000. The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness. London: Heinemann.Google Scholar
Danziger, Kurt. 1997. Naming the mind: How psychology found its language. London: Sage.Google Scholar
Davidson, Donald. 1994. Donald Davidson: Reply to Ralf Stoecker. In Reflecting Davidson (pp.287–90). Ed. Stoecker, Ralph. New York: de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Davidson, Donald. 2001. Actions, reasons, and causes. In Essays on Actions and Events (pp.3–19). Oxford: Oxford University Press. First published 1963.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davison, Kenneth. 1964. Episodic depersonalisation. British Journal of Psychiatry, 110: 505–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deacon, Terrence, W. 1997. The symbolic species: The co-evolution of language and the brain. New York: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
Deikman, Arthur J. 1999. ‘I’ = awareness. In Models of the self (pp.421–7). Eds. Gallagher, Shaun and Shear, Jonathan. Exeter: Imprint Academic.Google Scholar
Dennett, Daniel C. 1987. The intentional stance. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dennett, Daniel C. 1991. Consciousness explained. Boston, Mass.: Little, Brown.Google Scholar
Dennett, Daniel C. 2003. Freedom evolves. New York: Penguin Viking.Google Scholar
Descartes, René. 1901. The method, meditations and philosophy of Descartes. Trans. Veitch, John. New York: Tudor Publishing Co.Google Scholar
Descartes, René. 2005. Descartes's meditations: A trilingual HTML edition. Eds. Manley, David B. and Taylor, Charles S.. Available online at www.wright.edu/cola/descartes/mede.html (accessed 28 December 2008).
Donald, Merlin. 1991. Origins of the modern mind: Three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Donald, Merlin. 2001. A mind so rare: The evolution of human consciousness. New York: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
Donald, Merlin. 2004. The virtues of rigorous interdisciplinarity. In The development of the mediated mind: Sociocultural context and cognitive development (Chapter 12). Eds. Lacariello, John, Hudson, Judith A., Fivush, Robyn and Bauer, Patricia J.. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Dreyfus, Hubert L. 1989. Alternative philosophical conceptualisation of psychopathology. In Phenomenology and beyond: The self and its language (pp.41–50). Eds. Durfee, Harald A. and Rodier, Daid F.T.. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Dror, Itiel E. and Thomas, Robin D.. 2005. The cognitive neuroscience laboratory: A framework for the science of mind. In The mind as a scientific object (pp.283–92). Eds. Erneling, Christina and Johnson, David M.. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dunbar, Robin. 2000. On the origin of the human mind. In Evolution and the human mind: Modularity, language and meta-cognition (pp.238–53). Eds. Carruthers, Peter and Chamberlain, Andrew. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Edwards, Derek and Potter, Jonathan. 1992. Discursive psychology. London: Sage.Google Scholar
Elder-Vass, Dave. 2006. A method for social ontology. Paper presented to IACR Annual Conference, Toronto, August 2006.Google Scholar
Elder-Vass, Dave. 2007. For emergence: Refining Archer's account of social structure. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 37: 25–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elliot, Anthony. 2001. Concepts of the self. Cambridge, Mass.: Blackwell, Polity Press.Google Scholar
Emirbayer, Mustafa and Mische, Ann. 1998. What is agency?American Journal of Sociology, 103: 962–1023.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Epstein, Seymour. 1973. The self-concept revisited or a theory of a theory. American Psychologist, 28: 404–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Erneling, Christina. 2005. Introduction: Is the ‘mind’ just another name for what the brain does? In The mind as a scientific object (pp.247–50). Eds. Erneling, Christina and Johnson, David M.. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ey, Henri. 1978. Consciousness: A phenomenological study of being conscious and becoming conscious. Trans. Flodsrom, John H.. Bloomington, Ind.: Indiana University Press. First published 1963.Google Scholar
Fajans, Jane. 1985. The person in social context: The social character of Baining ‘psychology’. In Person, self and experience: Exploring Pacific ethnopsychologies (pp.367–97). Eds. White, Geoffrey M. and Kirkpatrick, John. Berkeley, Calif.: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Feest, Uljana. 2003. Functional analysis and the autonomy of psychology. Philosophy of Science, 70: 937–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feinberg, Todd E. 2002. Altered egos: How the brain creates the self. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fernyough, Charles. 2004. More than a context for learning? The epistemic triangle and the dialogic mind. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27: 104–5.Google Scholar
Flavell, John H., Green, Frances L., Flavell, Eleanor R. and Lin, Nancy T.. 1999. Development of children's knowledge about unconsciousness. Child development, 70: 396–412.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foster, Mary L. 1994. Symbolism: The foundation of culture. In Companion encyclopedia of anthropology: Humanity, culture and social Life (pp.366–395). Ed. Ingold, Tim. London: RoutledgeGoogle Scholar
Foucault, Michel. 1977. Discipline and punish. London: Allen Lane.Google Scholar
Foucault, Michel. 1984. What is enlightenment? In The Foucault reader. Ed. Rabinow, Paul. New York: Pantheon.Google Scholar
Foucault, Michel. 2004. The order of things. London: Routledge. First published 1966.Google Scholar
Fragaszy, Dorothy and Perry, Susan. 2003. Towards a biology of traditions. In Traditions in nonhuman animals: Models and evidence (pp.1–32). Eds. Fragaszy, Dorothy and Perry, Susan. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frankfurt, Harry G. 1988. Freedom of the will and the concept of the person. In The importance of what we care about (pp.11–25). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fraser, Ian. 2007. Dialectics of the self: Transcending Charles Taylor. Exeter: Imprint Academic.Google Scholar
Frith, Chris. 2007. Making up the mind: How the brain creates our mental world. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Gallagher, Shaun. 2000. Philosophical conceptions of the self: Implications for cognitive science. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4: 14–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallagher, Shaun. 2005. How the body shapes the mind. Oxford: Clarendon Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gander, Eric M. 2003. On our minds: How evolutionary psychology is reshaping the nature versus nurture debate. Baltimore, Md.: Johns Hopkins.Google Scholar
Geertz, Clifford. 1983. ‘From the native's point of view’: On the nature of anthropological understanding. In Local knowledge: Further essays in interpretive anthropology (pp. 55–71). New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gergely, György. 2004. The development of understanding self and agency. In Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (pp.26–46). Ed. Goswami, Usha. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Gergen, Kenneth J. 1988. If persons are texts. In Hermeneutics and psychological theory (pp.28–51). Eds. Messer, Stanley B., Sass, Louis A. and Woolfolk, Robert L.. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.Google Scholar
Gergen, Kenneth J. 1999. An invitation to social construction. Thousand Oaks, Calif.: Sage.Google Scholar
Giddens, Anthony. 1979. Central problems in social theory: Action, structure and contradiction in social analysis. London: Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giddens, Anthony. 1991. Modernity and self-identity: Self and society in the late modern age. Palo Alto, Calif.: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Glas, Gerrit. 2004. Idem, ipse, and loss of the self. Philosophy, Psychiatry and Psychology, 10: 347–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gordon, Robert M. 1998. The prior question: Do human primates have a theory of mind. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21: 120–1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gräslund, Bo. 2005. Early humans and their world. London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gray, Jeffrey. 2004. Consciousness of self: The point of view. In Consciousness: Creeping up on the hard problem (pp.261–6). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Greenspan, Stanley I. and Shanker, Stuart G.. 2006. The first idea: How symbols, language, and intelligence evolved from our primate ancestors to modern humans. Cambridge, Mass.: Da Capo Press.Google Scholar
Hacker, , Peter, M. S. 2001. Wittgenstein and the autonomy of humanistic understanding. In Wittgenstein, theory and the arts (pp.39–74). Ed. Allen, Richard and Turvey, Malcolm. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Hacking, Ian. 1995. Rewriting the soul: Multiple personality and the science of memory. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Hacking, Ian. 1999. The social construction of what?Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Hall, Stuart. 2000. Who needs identity? In Identity: A reader. Eds. Gay, Paul du, Evans, Jessica and Redman, Peter. London: Sage.Google Scholar
Hallam, Richard S. and O'Connor, Kieron. 2002. A dialogical approach to obsessions. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 75: 333–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hallowell, A. Irving. 1967. Culture and experience. New York: Schocken Books.Google Scholar
Harré, Rom. 1986. Emotion talk across times: Accidie and melancholy in the psychological context. In The Social construction of emotions (pp.220–7). Ed. Harré, Rom. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Harré, Rom. 1997a. Forward to Aristotle: The case for a hybrid ontology. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 27: 173–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harré, Rom. 1997b. Pathological autobiographies. Philosophy, Psychiatry, and Psychology, 4: 99–109.Google Scholar
Harré, Rom. 1998. The singular self, London: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Harré, Rom. 2002. Public sources of the personal mind. Theory and Psychology, 12: 611–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harré, Rom. 2005. Transcending the emergence/reduction distinction: The case of biology. Royal Institute of Philosophy Supplements, 80: 1–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harré, Rom and Langenhove, Luk. 1992. Varieties of positioning. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 20: 393–407.Google Scholar
Harré, Rom and Langenhove, Luk. 1999. Positioning theory: Moral contexts of intentional action. Malden, Mass.: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Harris, Grace G. 1989. Concepts of individual, self, and person in description and analysis. American Anthropologist, 91: 599–612.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haskell, Robert E. 1993. Vico and Jaynes: Neurocultural and cognitive operations in the origin of consciousness. New Vico Studies, 11: 24–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hawkes, Terence. 1972. Metaphor. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Hayes, Steven C., Barnes-Holmes, Dermot and Roche, Bryan. 2001. Relational frame theory. New York: Kluwer/Plenum.Google Scholar
Heelas, Paul and Lock, Andrew. 1981. Indigenous psychologies: The anthropology of the self. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Held, Barbara S. 2002. What follows? Mind dependence, fallibility and transcendence according to (strong) constructionism's realist and quasi-realist critics. Theory and Psychology, 12: 651–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hermans, Hubert. 2001. The dialogical self: Toward a theory of personal and cultural positioning. Culture and Psychology, 7: 243–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hermans, , Hubert, J. M. and J.G. Kempen, Harry. 1993. The dialogical self: Meaning as movement. San Diego, Calif.: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Heyes, Celia M. 1998. Theory of mind in non-human primates. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21: 101–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hirst, Paul. 1979. On law and ideology. Basingstoke: Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobson, R. Peter. 2000. The grounding of symbols: A social developmental account. In Children's reasoning and the mind (pp.11–35). Eds. Mitchell, Peter and Riggs, Kevin. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Holden, Constance. 2004. The origin of speech. Science, 303 (5662): 1316–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holland, Dorothy. 1997. Selves as cultured: As told by an anthropologist who lacks a soul. In Self and identity: Fundamental issues (pp.160–90). Eds. Ashmore, Richard D. and Jussim, Lee. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hood, Thomas. 1900. The poetical works of Thomas Hood. London: George Routledge.Google Scholar
Horner, Victoria and Whiten, Andrew. 2005. Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens). Animal Cognition, 8: 164–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howie, Dorothy and Peters, Michael. 1996. Positioning theory: Vygotsky, Wittgenstein and social construction psychology. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 26: 51–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humphrey, Nicholas. 1993. The inner eye. London: Vintage.Google Scholar
Humphrey, Nicholas. 2002. The mind made flesh. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hundert, Ed J. 1997. The European enlightenment and the history of the self. In Rewriting the self: Histories from the Renaissance to the present (pp.72–83). Ed. Porter, Roy. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Hutto, Daniel D. 2008. Folk psychological narratives: The sociocultural basis of understanding reasons. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ingold, Tim. 1994. Introduction to humanity. In Companion encyclopedia of anthropology: Humanity, culture and social life (pp.3–13). Ed. Ingold, Tim. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, Ray. 2002. Foundations of language: Brain, meaning, grammar, evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacob, Pierre. 2003. Intentionality. In The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy (Fall 2003 Edition). Ed. Zalta, Edward N.. Available online at http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2003/entries/intentionality.
James, William. 1890. The principles of psychology. Vol. I. London: Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, William. 1961. Psychology: Briefer course. Ed. Allport, Gordon. New York: Harper & Row. First published 1892.Google Scholar
Jaynes, Julian. 1976. The origin of consciousness in the breakdown of the bicameral mind. London: Allen Lane.Google Scholar
Johnson, David M. 2005. Mind, brain and the upper paleolithic. In The mind as a scientific object (pp.499–510). Eds. Erneling, Christina E. and Johnson, David M.. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Jones, Raya A. 1997. The presence of self in the person: Reflexive positioning and personal construct psychology. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 27: 453–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jopling, David A. 1997. A ‘self of selves?’ In The conceptual self in context: Culture, experience, self-understanding (pp.249–67). Eds. Neisser, Ulric and Jopling, David A.. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Jopling, David A. 2000. Self-knowledge and the self. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Josephs, Ingrid E. 1998. Constructing one's self in the city of the silent: Dialogue, symbols, and the role of ‘as-if’ in self-development. Human Development, 41: 180–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaye, Kenneth. 1982. The mental and social life of babies: How parents create persons. Chicago, Ill.: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Kelly, George. 1955. The psychology of personal constructs. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Kendall, Gavin and Wickham, Gary. 1999. Using Foucault's methods. London: Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kihlstrom, John F., Marchese-Foster, Lori A., and Klein, Stanley B.. 1997. Situating the self in interpersonal space. In The conceptual self in context: Culture, experience, self-understanding (pp.154–75). Eds. Neisser, Ulric and Jopling, David A.. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kinsbourne, Marcel. 2005. A continuum of self-consciousness that emerges in phylogeny and ontogeny. In The missing link in cognition (pp.142–56). Eds. Terrace, Herbert S. and Metcalfe, Janet. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kirsch, Irving. 1998. Volition as a believed-in imagining. In Believed-in imaginings: The narrative construction of reality (pp.157–68). Eds. Rivera, Joseph H. and Sarbin, Theodore R.. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Knight, Chris. 2000. Play as a precursor of phonology and syntax. In The evolutionary emergence of language: social function and the origins of linguistic form (pp.99–129). Eds. Knight, Chris, Studdert-Kennedy, Michael and Hurford, James. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, Deanna. 2000. Theory of mind, metacognition, and reasoning: A lifespan perspective. In Children's reasoning and the mind (pp.301–26). Eds. Mitchell, Peter and Riggs, Kevin. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, George. 1997. The internal structure of the self. In The conceptual self in context: Culture, experience, self-understanding (pp.92–113). Eds. Neisser, Ulric and Jopling, David A.. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, George and Johnson, Mark. 1980. Metaphors we live by. Chicago, Ill.: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Landrine, Hope. 1992. Clinical implications of cultural differences: The referential versus the indexical self. Clinical Psychology Review, 12 (4): 401–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langenhove, Luk and Harré, Rom. 1999. Reflexive positioning: autobiography. In Positioning theory: Moral contexts of intentional action. Eds. Harré, Rom and Langenhove, Luk. Malden, Mass.: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Langer, Ellen J. 1989. Mindfulness. Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Leahey, Thomas. 2003. Mind as a scientific object: A historical-philosophical exploration. In Mind as a scientific object (pp.35–78). Eds. Erneling, Christina E. and Johnson, David M.. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Leary, Mark R. 2007. The curse of the self: Self-awareness, egotism, and the quality of human life. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
LeDoux, Joseph. 2002. Synaptic self: How our brains become who we are. New York: Penguin, Viking.Google Scholar
LePan, Don. 1989. The cognitive revolution in western culture. London: Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leudar, Ivan and Thomas, Philip. 2000. Voices of reason, voices of insanity: Studies of verbal hallucinations. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Lévi-Strauss, Claude. 1963. Structural anthropology. Trans. Jacobson, Claire and Schoepf, Brooke Grundfest. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Levy, Robert I. 1989. The quest for mind in different times and different places. In Social history and issues in human consciousness (pp.3–28). Eds. Barnes, Andrew E. and Stearns, Peter N.. New York: New York University Press.Google Scholar
Lewis, C.S. 1960. Studies in words. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Libet, Benjamin. 2000. Do we have free will? In The volitional brain: Towards a neuroscience of free will (pp.47–58). Eds. Libet, Benjamin, Freeman, Anthony and Sutherland, Keith. Exeter: Imprint Academic.Google Scholar
Lieberman, Phillip. 1994. The origins and evolution of language. In Companion encyclopedia of anthropology: Humanity, culture and social life (pp.108–132). Ed. Ingold, Tim. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Little, Todd, Hawley, Patricia H., Henrich, Christopher C. and Marsland, Katherine W.. 2002. Three views of the agentic self: A developmental synthesis. In Handbook of self-regulation research (pp.389–404). Eds. Deci, Edward L. and Ryan, Richard M.. Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.Google Scholar
Locke, John. 1959. An essay concerning human understanding. Ed. and Abr, . Wilburn, Raymond. London: Dent & Sons. First published 1700.Google Scholar
Lodge, David. 2003. Consciousness and the novel. London: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Logan, Richard D. 1987. Historical change in prevailing sense of self. In Self and identity: Psychosocial perspectives (pp.13–26). Eds. Yardley, Krysia and Honess, Terry. Chichester: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Loevinger, Jane. 1976. Ego development. San Francisco, Calif.: Jossey Bass.Google Scholar
Lukes, Steven. 1973. Individualism. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Lukes, Steven. 1985. Conclusion. In The category of the person: Anthropology, philosophy, history (pp.282–301). Eds. Carrithers, Michael, Collins, Steven and Lukes, Steven. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lyons, John O. 1978. The invention of the self: The hinge of consciousness in the eighteenth century. Carbondale, Ill.: Southern Illinois University Press.Google Scholar
Macdonald, Paul S. 2003. History of the concept of mind. Aldershot: Ashgate.Google Scholar
MacIntyre, Alasdair. 1981. After virtue. Notre Dame, Ind.: University of Notre Dame Press.Google Scholar
MacKinnon, Richard C. 1997. Punishing the persona: Correctional strategies for the virtual offender. In Virtual culture: Identity and communication in cybersociety (pp.206–35). Ed. Jones, Steven. Thousand Oaks, Calif.: Sage.
MacWhinney, Brian. 2004. Language evolution and human development. In Origins of the social mind: Evolutionary psychology and child development (pp.383–410). Eds. Ellis, Bruce J. and Bjorklund, David F.. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Maddocks, Fiona. 2001. Hildegard of Bingen. London: Headline Publishing.Google Scholar
Malle, Bertram F. 2004. How the mind explains behavior: Folk explanations, meaning, and social interaction. Cambridge, Mass.: Bradford Books, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Malle, Bertram F. 2006. Of windmills and straw men: Folk assumptions of mind and action. In Does consciousness cause behavior? (pp.207–31). Eds. Pockett, Susan, Banks, William P. and Gallagher, Shaun. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Mallon, Ronald and Stich, Stephen P.. 2000. The odd couple: The compatibility of social construction and evolutionary psychology. Philosophy of Science, 67: 133–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mancuso, James C. and Sarbin, Theodore R.. 1983. The self-narrative in the enactment of roles. In Studies in social identity (pp.233–53). Eds. Sarbin, Theodore R. and Scheibe, Karl E.. Westport, Conn.: Praeger.Google Scholar
Manicas, Peter T. 2006. A realist philosophy of science: Explanation and understanding. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marková, Ivana. 1987. Human awareness. London: Hutchinson.Google Scholar
Marková, Ivana. 2000. The individual and society in psychological theory. Theory and psychology, 10: 107–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marková, Ivana. 2003. Dialogicality and social representations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Marks, Isaac. 1987. Fears and phobias. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Markus, Hazel. 1977. Self-schemata and processing information about the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35: 63–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markus, Hazel. 1986. Possible selves. American Psychologist, 41: 954–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markus, Hazel R. and Kitayama, Shinobu. 2002. Models of agency: Sociocultural diversity in the construction of action. In The annual Nebraska symposium on motivation: Cross-cultural differences in perspectives on self (V. 49) (pp.1–57). Eds. Murphy-Berman, Virginia and Berman, John J.. Lincoln, Nebr.: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Martin, Jack and Sugarman, Jeff H.. 2003. A theory of personhood for psychology. In About psychology: Essays at the crossroads of history, theory, and philosophy (pp.73–87). Eds. Hill, Darryl B. and Kral, Michael. Albany, NY: SUNY Press.Google Scholar
Martin, Raymond, and Barresi, John. 2000. Naturalization of the soul: Self and personal identity in the eighteenth century. London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, Raymond, 2006. The rise and fall of soul and self: An intellectual history of personal identity. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Martin, Rux. 1988. Truth, power, self: An interview with Michel Foucault, October 25 1982. In Foucault, Michel, Technologies of the self: A seminar with Michel Foucault (pp.9–15). Eds. Martin, Luther H., Gutman, Huck and Hutton, Patrick H.. Amherst, Mass.: University of Massachusetts Press.Google Scholar
Mascolo, Michael F. and Fischer, Kurt W.. 1998. The development of self through the coordination of component systems. In Self-awareness: Its nature and development (pp.332–83). Eds. Ferrari, Michael and Sternberg, Robert J.. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Maturana, Humberto and Varela, Francisco. 1980. Autopoiesis and cognition: The realization of the living. In Boston Studies in the Philosophy of Science Vol. 42. Eds. Cohen, Robert S. and Wartofsky, Marx W.. Dordrecht: D. Reidel Publishing Co.Google Scholar
Mauss, Marcel. 1985. A category of the human mind: The notion of person: the notion of self. In The category of the person (pp.1–25). Eds. Carrithers, Malcolm, Collins, Steven and Lukes, Steven. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. First published 1938.Google Scholar
McAdams, Dan P. 1996. Personality, modernity, and the storied self: A contemporary framework for studying persons. Psychological Inquiry, 7: 295–321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCall, Catherine. 1990. Concepts of person: An analysis of concepts of person, self, and human being. Aldershot: Avebury.Google Scholar
Mead, George H. 1962. Mind, self and society. Chicago, Ill.: University of Chicago Press. First published 1934.Google Scholar
Melchior-Bonnet, Sabine. 2002. The mirror: A history. Trans. Jewett, Katherine H.. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Melser, Derek. 2004. The act of thinking. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Merleau-Ponty, Maurice. 1966. Phenomenology of perception. London: Routledge Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Messari, Sophia and Hallam, Richard. 2003. CBT for psychosis: A qualitative analysis of client's accounts. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 42: 171–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Michel, A. Alexandra and Wortham, Stanton E.F.. 2002. Clearing away the self. Theory and Psychology, 12: 625–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Millikan, Ruth. 2004. Existence proof for a viable externalism. In The externalist challenge: New studies on cognition and intentionality (pp.227–38). Ed. Schantz, Richard. New York: Walter de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Mineka, Susan and Cook, Michael. 1988. Social learning and the acquisition of snake fear. In Social learning: Psychological and biological perspectives (pp.51–74). Eds. Zentall, Thomas R. and Galef, Bennett G.. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Mitchell, Robert W. 1994. Multiplicities of self. In Self-awareness in animals and humans: Developmental perspectives (pp.81–107). Eds. Parker, Sue Taylor, Mitchell, Robert W. and Boccia, Maria L.. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Mithen, Steven. 2000. Mind, brain and material culture: an archaeological perspective. In Evolution and the human mind: Modularity, language and meta-cognition. Eds. Carruthers, Peter and Chamberlain, Andrew. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Moeller, Hans-Georg. 2006. Luhmann explained: From souls to systems. Chicago, Ill.: Open Court.Google Scholar
Mühlhäusler, Peter and Harré, Rom. 1993. Pronouns and people. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Neisser, Ulric. 1997. Concepts and self-concepts. In The conceptual self in context: Culture, experience, self-understanding (pp.3–12). Eds. Neisser, Ulric and Jopling, David A.. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, Katherine. 2001. From the experiencing I to the continuing me. In The self in time: Developmental perspectives (pp.15–34). Eds. Moore, Chris, Lemon, Karen and Skene, Karen. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Nelson, Katherine. 2003. Self and social functions: Individual autobiographical memory and collective memory. Memory, 11: 125–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, Katherine. 2004. Evolution and development of human memory systems. In Origins of the social mind: Evolutionary psychology and child development (pp.354–82). Eds. Ellis, Bruce J. and Bjorklund, David F.. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, Katherine. 2005. Emerging levels of consciousness in early human development. In The missing link in cognition: Origins of self-reflective consciousness (pp.116–41). Eds. Terrace, Herbert S. and Metcalfe, Janet. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Nichols, Shaun. 2008. Folk psychology. In Encylopedia of cognitive science. London: Nature Publishing Group. Available online at www.dingo.sbs.arizona.edu/~snichols/Papers/FolkPsychologyECS.pdf (accessed 28 December 2008).Google Scholar
Nienkamp, Jean. 2001. Internal rhetorics: Toward a history and theory of self-persuasion. Carbondale, Ill.: Southern Illinois University Press.Google Scholar
Nightingale, David J. and Cromby, John. 1999. Reconstructing social constructionism. In Social constructionist psychology: A critical analysis of theory and practice (pp.207–24). Eds. Nightingale, David J. and Cromby, John. Buckingham: Open University Press.Google Scholar
Nisbett, Richard and Wilson, Timothy D.. 1977. Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84: 231–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Connor, Kieron P. and Hallam, Richard S.. 2000. Sorcery of the self: The magic of you. Theory and Psychology, 10: 238–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Connor, Kieron, Aardema, Frederick and Pelissier, Marie-Claude. 2005. Beyond reasonable doubt: Reasoning processes in obsessive-compulsive disorder and related disorders. Chichester: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Oshima-Takane, Yuriko. 1999. The learning of first and second person pronouns in English. In Language, logic, and concept: Essays in memory of John Macnamara (pp.373–409). Eds. Jackendoff, Ray, Bloom, Paul and Wynn, Karen. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Parfit, Derek. 1984. Reasons and persons. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Parker, Ian. 1992. Discourse dynamics: Critical analysis for social and individual psychology. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Parker, Sue T., Mitchell, Robert W. and Boccia, Maria L., Eds. 2006. Self-awareness in animals and humans: Developmental perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Pennisi, Elizabeth. 2004. The first language?Science, 303: 1319–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petitto, Laura A. 1987. On the autonomy of language and gesture: Evidence from the acquisition of personal pronouns in American Sign Language. Cognition, 27: 1–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petrarca, Francesco. 1992. Secretum (Il mio segreto). Milan: Tursia.Google Scholar
Mirandola, Pico della, Giovanni, . 1960. Oration on the dignity of man. Washington, DC: H. Regnery.Google Scholar
Plotkin, Henry. 2002. The imagined world made real: Towards a natural science of culture. London: Allen Lane.Google Scholar
Polkinghorne, Donald. 2004. Ricoeur, narrative and personal identity. In Changing conceptions of psychological life (pp.27–48). Eds. Chandler, Michael, Lalonde, Chris and Lightfoot, Cynthia. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Popper, Karl R. and Eccles, John C.. 1977. The self and its brain. New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Potter, Jonathan and Wetherell, Margaret. 1987. Discourse and social psychology. London: Sage.Google Scholar
Potter, Jonathan and Edwards, Derek. 2003. Rethinking cognition: On Coulter on discourse and mind. Human Studies, 26: 165–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Povinelli, Daniel J. and Barth, Jochen. 2005. Reinterpreting behavior: A human specialization?Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28: 712–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prinz, Wolfgang. 2006. Free will as a social institution. In Does consciousness cause behavior? (pp.257–76). Eds. Pockett, Susan, Banks, William P. and Gallagher, Shaun. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Rabinow, Paul and Rose, Nikolas. 2003. Introduction: Foucault today. In The essential Foucault (pp. vii–xxxxv). Eds. Rabinow, Paul and Rose, Nikolas. New York: The New Press.Google Scholar
Ramachandran, Vilayanur. 2006. Mirror neurons and the brain in a vat. Available online at www.edge.org/3rd_culture/ramachandran06/ramachandran06_index.html (accessed 28 December 2008).
Ratcliffe, Matthew. 2006. Rethinking commonsense psychology: A critique of folk psychology, theory of mind, and simulation. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Read, Kenneth E. 1955. Morality and the concept of the person among the Gahuku-Gama. Oceania, 25: 233–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reddy, Michael J. 1993. The conduit metaphor: A case of frame conflict in our language about language. In Metaphor and thought (pp.164–201). Ed. Ortony, Andrew. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. First published 1979.Google Scholar
Reddy, Vasudevi and Morris, Paul. 2004. Participants don't need theories: Knowing minds in engagement. Theory and Psychology, 14: 647–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Revonsuo, Antti. 2005. Inner presence: Consciousness as a biological phenomenon. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Richards, Graham. 1989. On psychological language and the physiomorphic basis of human nature. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Ricoeur, Paul. 1991. Narrative identity. Philosophy Today, 35: 73–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rizzolatti, Giacomo, Fadiga, Luciano, Gallese, Vittorio and Fogassi, Leonardo. 1996. Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Cognitive Brain Research, 3: 131–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roessler, Johannes. 2003. Intentional action and self-awareness. In Agency and self-awareness: Issues in philosophy and psychology (pp.383–405). Eds. Roessler, Johannes and Elan, Naomi, New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rorty, Amélie O. 1988. Mind in action: Essays in the philosophy of mind. Boston, Mass: Beacon Press.Google Scholar
Rose, Nikolas S. 1990. Governing the soul: The shaping of the private self. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Rose, Nikolas S. 1998. Inventing our selves: Psychology, power, and personhood. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ryle, Gilbert. 1949. The concept of mind. London: Hutchinson.Google Scholar
Sarbin, Theodore. 1968. Ontology recapitulates philology: The mythic nature of anxiety. American Psychologist, 23: 411–18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sarbin, Theodore. 1998. Believed in imaginings: A narrative approach. In Believed-in imaginings: The narrative construction of reality (pp.15–30). Eds. Rivera, Joseph and Sarbin, Theodore R.. Washington, DC: APA Publications.Google Scholar
Sarbin, Theodore R. and Scheibe, Karl E.. 1983. A model of social identity. In Studies in social identity (pp.5–28). Eds. Sarbin, Theodore R. and Scheibe, Karl E.. New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
Sartre, Jean-Paul. 1992. The transcendence of the ego. New York: Hill & Wang. First published 1936.Google Scholar
Sass, Louis A. 1997. The consciousness machine: Self and subjectivity in schizophrenia and modern culture. In The conceptual self in context: Culture, experience, self-understanding (pp.203–32). Eds. Neisser, Ulric and Jopling, David A.. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Savage-Rumbaugh, Sue, Shanker, Stuart and Taylor, Talbot J.. 1998. Apes, language and the human mind. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sawyer, R. Keith. 2003a. Non-reductive individualism. Part II: Social causation. Philosophy of the Social Sciences, 33: 202–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sawyer, R. Keith. 2003b. Emergence and complexity: A new approach to social systems theory. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Sociological Association. Atlanta, Ga. Available online at www.allacademic.com/meta/p106832_index.html (accessed 21 July 2008).
Sawyer, R. Keith. 2004. The mechanisms of emergence. Philosophy of the Social Sciences, 34: 260–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scheibe, Karl E. 1998. Self studies: The psychology of self and identity. New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
Seale, Clive. 1999. The quality of qualitative research. London: Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sebanz, Natalie. 2007. The emergence of self: Sensing action through joint agency. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 14: 234–51.Google Scholar
Sedikides, Constantine and Skowronski, John J.. 2003. Evolution of the symbolic self. In Handbook of self and identity (pp.595–609). Eds. Leary, Mark R., Tangney, June P.. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Shear, Jonathan. 1999. Experiential clarification of the problem of self. In Models of the self (pp.401–20). Eds. Gallagher, Shaun and Shear, Jonathan. Exeter: Imprint Academic.Google Scholar
Shotter, John. 1997. The social construction of our inner selves. Journal of Constructivist Psychology, 10: 7–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shotter, John. 2001. Towards a third revolution in psychology: From inner mental representations to dialogically structured social practices. In Jerome Bruner: Language, culture and self (pp.167–83). Eds. Bakhurst, David and Shanker, Stuart G.. London: Sage.Google Scholar
Siegel, Jerrold. 2005. The idea of the self. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. 1957. Verbal learning. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. 1976. About behaviorism. London: Vintage Books.Google Scholar
Smith, Brewster M. 2002. Self and identity in historical/sociocultural context: ‘Perspectives on selfhood’ revisited. In Self and identity: Personal, social, and symbolic (pp.229–43). Eds. Kashima, Yoshihisa, Foddy, Margaret and Platow, Michael J.. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Sodian, Beate. 2006. Theory-of-mind: The case for conceptual development. In Young children's cognitive development: Interrelationships among executive functioning, working memory, verbal ability, and theory of mind (pp.95–130). Eds. Schneider, Wolfgang, Schumann-Hengsteler, Ruth and Sodian, Beate. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Sperber, Dan. 1996. Explaining culture: A naturalistic approach. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Spiro, Melford E. 1993. Is the Western conception of the person ‘peculiar’ within the context of world cultures?Ethos, 21: 107–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sprague, Elmer. 1999. Persons and their minds. Boulder, Col.: Westview Press.Google Scholar
Sterelny, Kim. 2004. Externalism, epistemic artefacts and the extended mind. In The externalist challenge: New studies on cognition and intentionality (pp.239–54). Ed. Schantz, Richard. New York: De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Stich, Stephen and Nichols, Shaun. 2002. Folk psychology. In The Blackwell guide to philosophy of mind (pp.235–55). Eds. Stich, Stephen P. and Warfield, Ted A.. Oxford: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strawson, Peter F. 1959. Individuals: An essay in descriptive metaphysics. London: Methuen.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Studdert-Kennedy, Michael. 2000. Evolutionary implications of the particulate principle: Imitation and dissociation of phonetic forms from semantic function. In The evolutionary emergence of language: social function and the origins of linguistic form (pp.161–76). Eds. Knight, Chris, Studdert-Kennedy, Michael and Hurford, James. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Suddendorf, Thomas and Whiten, Andrew. 2003. Reinterpreting the mentality of apes. In From mating to mentality: Evaluating evolutionary psychology (pp.173–96). Eds. Fitness, Julie and Sterelny, Kim. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Tauber, Alfred I. 2005. The reflexive project: Reconstructing the moral agent. History of the Human Sciences, 18: 49–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, Charles. 1977. What is human agency? In The self: Psychological and philosophical issues (pp.103–35). Ed. Mischel, Theodore. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Taylor, Charles. 1988. Wittgenstein, empiricism, and the question of the ‘inner’: Commentary on Kenneth Gergen. In Hermeneutics and psychological theory: Interpretive perspectives on personality, psychotherapy, and psychopathology (pp.52–61). Eds. Messer, Stanley B., Sass, Louis A. and Woolfolk, Robert L.. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.Google Scholar
Taylor, Charles. 1989. Sources of the self: The making of the modern identity. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Teichert, Dieter. 2004. Narrative, identity and the self. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 11: 175–91.Google Scholar
Terrace, Herbert S. 2005. Metacognition and the evolution of language. In The missing link in cognition: Origins of self-reflective consciousness (pp.84–115). Eds. Terrace, Herbert S. and Metcalfe, Janet. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tollefsen, Deborah P. 2002. Collective intentionality and the social sciences. Philosophy of the social sciences, 32: 25–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, Michael, Carpenter, Malinder, Call, Josep, Behne, Tanya and Moll, Henrike. 2005. Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28: 675–735.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tulving, Endel. 2005. Episodic memory and autonoesis: Uniquely human? In The missing link in cognition: Origins of self-reflective consciousness (pp.3–56). Eds. Terrace, Herbert S. and Metcalfe, Janet. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Velmans, Max. 2000. Understanding consciousness. London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vico, Giambattista. 2000. New science. 3rd edn. Trans. Marsh, David. Ed. Grafton, Anthony. London: Penguin Classics. First published 1725.Google Scholar
Vovelle, Michel. 1990. Ideologies and mentalities. Trans. O, E.'Flaherty, . Cambridge: Polity Press.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, Lev S. 1962. Thought and language. New York: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Warner, C. Terry. 1986. Anger and similar delusions. In The social construction of emotions (pp.135–66). Ed. Harré, Rom. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Webb, Keith. 1995. An Introduction to problems in the philosophy of social sciences. London: Continuum/Pinter.Google Scholar
Wegner, Daniel M. 2002. The illusion of conscious will. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Weintraub, Karl J. 1978. The value of the individual: Self and circumstance in autobiography. Chicago, Ill.: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
White, Hayden V. 1978. Foucault decoded. In Tropics of discourse: Essays in culturalcriticism (pp.230–60). Baltimore, Md: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Whiten, Andrew and Byrne, Richard W.. 1997. Machiavellian intelligence II: Extensions and evaluations. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, Malcolm. 1999. Science and social science: An introduction. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Williams, Meredith. 1985. Wittgenstein's rejection of scientific psychology. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 15: 203–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, Richard. 1992. The human context of agency. American Psychologist, 47: 752–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wittgenstein, Ludwig. 1953. Philosophical investigations. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Wittgenstein, Ludwig. 1980. Remarks on the philosophy of psychology. Vol. I. Eds. E.M. Anscombe, Gertrude and Wright, Georg H.. Trans. Anscombe, G. E. M.. Volume II. Eds Wright, Georg H. and Nyman, Heikki. Trans. Luckhardt, C. Grant and Maximilian, A. E.Aue. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Young, Jeff, Klosko, Janet S. and Weishaar, Marjorie E.. 2003. Schema therapy: A practitioner's guide. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Zahavi, Dan. 2005. Subjectivity and selfhood: Investigating the first-person perspective. Cambridge, Mass.: Bradford Books, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Zelazo, Philip and Sommerville, Jessica. 2001. Levels of consciousness of the self in time. In The Self in time: Developmental perspectives (pp.229–53). Eds. Moore, Chris, Lemmon, Karen and Skene, Karen. New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • Bibliography
  • Richard S. Hallam, University of Greenwich
  • Book: Virtual Selves, Real Persons
  • Online publication: 04 August 2010
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511770449.016
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • Bibliography
  • Richard S. Hallam, University of Greenwich
  • Book: Virtual Selves, Real Persons
  • Online publication: 04 August 2010
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511770449.016
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • Bibliography
  • Richard S. Hallam, University of Greenwich
  • Book: Virtual Selves, Real Persons
  • Online publication: 04 August 2010
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511770449.016
Available formats
×