Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-75dct Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-03T01:11:54.589Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

5 - Evaluation in Spanish-Speaking Mother–Child Narratives: The Social and Sense-Making Function of Internal-State References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Allyssa McCabe
Affiliation:
University of Massachusetts, Lowell
Alison L. Bailey
Affiliation:
University of California, Los Angeles
Gigliana Melzi
Affiliation:
New York University
Get access

Summary

Key words: narrative, evaluation, internal states, mother–child conversations, theory-of-mind, social cognition, autobiographical memory, Spanish, Latino children.

ABSTRACT

Young children's sociocognitive skills, such as their understanding of others' feelings and their ability to explain human action in terms of beliefs, emotions, and intentions, has been associated with naturally occurring talk about internal states in everyday family interactions. The frequency with which children engage in conversations about internal states is related to various aspects of sociocognitive development, including emotion recognition, affective perspective-taking, and false-belief understanding. Various individual and contextual factors influence the language used between parents and children during daily family interactions. However, most studies to date have examined the use of internal-state language in English-speaking homes; thus, results might not be generalizable to other cultural groups. The current study addressed this gap in the literature by examining the manner in which Spanish-speaking Peruvian mothers discussed internal states with their preschooler in two narrative contexts: conversations about the personal past and storybook-sharing interactions. The conversations in these two contexts between 32 middle-class mothers and their preschool-aged children (i.e., ages 3 and 5) were analyzed for the frequency and patterns of use of internal-state references. Results show gender and age differences in the use of internal-state references only in the narrative-conversation context. The chapter concludes with a discussion of the theoretical and empirical links between children's narrative competence and their sociocognitive skills.

Type
Chapter
Information
Spanish-Language Narration and Literacy
Culture, Cognition, and Emotion
, pp. 92 - 118
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adams, S., Kuebli, J., Boyle, P. A., & Fivush, R. (1995). Gender differences in parent–child conversations about past emotions: A longitudinal investigation. Sex Roles, 33(5–6), 309–323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alexander, K. J., Miller, P. J., & Hengst, J. A. (2001). Young children's emotional attachments to stories. Social Development, 10(3), 374–398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartsch, K., & Wellman, H. M. (1995). Children talk about the mind. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Berman, R. A., & Slobin, D. I. (1994). Relating events in narrative: A cross-linguistic developmental study. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bird, A., & Reese, E. (2006). Emotional reminiscing and the development of an autobiographical self. Developmental Psychology, 42(4), 613–626.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boland, A. M., Haden, C. A., & Ornstein, P. A. (2003). Boosting children's memory by training mothers in the use of an elaborative conversational style as an event unfolds. Journal of Cognition and Development, 4(1), 39–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, J., Donelan-McCall, N., & Dunn, J. (1996). Why talk about mental states: The significance of children's conversations with friends, siblings, and mothers. Child Development, 67(3), 836–849.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Buckner, J. P., & Fivush, R. (1998). Gender and self in children's autobiographical narratives. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 12(4), 407–429.3.0.CO;2-7>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buckner, J. P., & Fivush, R. (2000). Gendered themes in family reminiscing. Memory, 8(6), 401–412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charman, T., & Shmueli-Goetz, Y. (1998). The relationship between theory of mind, language and narrative discourse: An experimental study. Current Psychology of Cognition, 17(2), 245–271.Google Scholar
Choi, S. H. (1992). Communicative socialization processes: Korea and Canada. In Iwawaki, S., Kashima, Y., & Leung, K. (Eds.), Innovations in cross-cultural psychology. Lisse, Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger Publishers.Google Scholar
Cohen, J. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 20, 37–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Curenton, S. M. (2004). The association between narratives and theory of mind for low-income preschoolers. Early Education and Development, 15(2), 121–145.Google Scholar
Dunn, J., Bretherton, I., & Munn, P. (1987). Conversations about feeling states between mothers and their young children. Developmental Psychology, 23(1), 132–139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, J., & Brown, J. R. (1993). Early conversations about causality: Content, pragmatics and developmental change. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 11(2), 107–123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, J., Brown, J., & Beardsall, L. (1991). Family talk about feeling states and children's later understanding of others' emotions. Developmental Psychology, 27(3), 448–455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, J., Brown, J., Slomkowski, C., Tesla, C., & Youngblade, L. M. (1991). Young children's understanding of other people's feelings and beliefs: Individual differences and their antecedents. Child Development, 62(6), 1352–1366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eder, R. A. (1990). Uncovering young children's psychological selves: Individual and developmental differences. Child Development, 61, 849–863.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, A. R. (1985). Learning to describe past experiences in conversation. Discourse Processes, 8, 177–204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenberg, A. R. (1999). Emotion talk among Mexican American and Anglo American mothers and children from two social classes. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 45(2), 267–284.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, A. R. (2002). Maternal teaching talk within families of Mexican descent: Influences of task and socioeconomic status. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences. Special Issue: Conversations within Mexican-descent families: Diverse contexts for language socialization and learning, 24(2), 206–224.Google Scholar
Ely, R., & McCabe, A. (1993). Remembered voices. Journal of Child Language, 20, 671–696.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farrant, K., & Reese, E. (2000). Maternal style and children's participation in reminiscing: Steppingstones in children's autobiographical memory development. Journal of Cognition and Development, 1(2), 193–225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farrar, M. J., Fasig, L. G., & Welch-Ross, M. K. (1997). Attachment and emotion in autobiographical memory development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 67(3), 389–408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fivush, R. (1991). Gender and emotion in mother-child conversations about the past. Journal of Narrative and Life History, 1(4), 325-341.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fivush, R. (1993). Emotional content of parent–child conversations about the past. In Nelson, C. A. (Ed.), Memory and affect in development: The Minnesota symposia on child psychology (Vol. 26, pp. 39–77). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Fivush, R. (1994). Constructing narrative, emotion, and self in parent–child conversations about the past. In Neisser, U. & Fivush, R. (Eds.), The remembering self: Construction and accuracy in the self-narrative. Emory symposia in cognition (pp. 136–157). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fivush, R. (2001). Owning experience: Developing subjective perspective in autobiographical narratives. In Moore, C. & Lemmon, K. (Eds.), The self in time: Developmental perspectives (pp. 35–52). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Fivush, R., Brotman, M. A., Buckner, J. P., & Goodman, S. H. (2000). Gender differences in parent-child emotion narratives. Sex Roles, 42(3–4), 233–253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fivush, R., & Buckner, J. P. (2003). Creating gender and identity through autobiographical narratives. In Fivush, R. & Haden, C. A. (Eds.), Autobiographical memory and the construction of a narrative self: Developmental and cultural perspectives (pp. 149–167). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Fivush, R., Haden, C. A., & Reese, E. (2006). Elaborating on elaborations: Role of material reminiscing style in cognitive and socioemotional development. Child Development, 77(6), 1568–1588.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fivush, R., & Nelson, K. (2006). Parent–child reminiscing locates the self in the past. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24(1), 235–251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fivush, R., & Wang, Q. (2005). Emotion talk in mother–child conversations of the shared past: The effects of culture, gender, and event valence. Journal of Cognition and Development, 6(4), 489–506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flannagan, D., & Perese, S. (1998). Emotional references in mother–daughter and mother–son dyads' conversations about school. Sex Roles, 39(5–6), 353–367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuller, N. (1997). Identidades masculinas. Lima: Pontificia Universidad Católica del Perú.Google Scholar
Fuller, N. (1998). Dilemas de la femineidad: Mujeres de la clase media en el Perú. Lima: Pontificia Universidad Católica del Perú.Google Scholar
Fung, H., & Cheng, E. C. (2001). Across time and beyond skin: Self and transgression in the everyday socialization of shame among Taiwanese preschool children. Social Development, 10(3), 420–437.Google Scholar
Furrow, D., Moore, C., Davidge, J., & Chiasson, L. (1992). Mental terms in mothers' and children's speech: Similarities and relationships. Journal of Child Language, 19(3), 617–631.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garrett, P. B., & Baquedano-López, P. (2002). Language socialization: Reproduction and continuity, transformation and change. Annual Review of Anthropology, 31, 339–361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guajardo, N. R., & Watson, A. C. (2002). Narrative discourse and theory of mind development. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 163(3), 305–325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Han, J. J., Leichtman, M. D., & Wang, Q. (1998). Autobiographical memory in Korean, Chinese and American children. Developmental Psychology, 34(4), 701–713.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haden, C. A. (1998). Reminiscing with different children: Relating maternal stylistic consistency and sibling similarity in talk about the past. Developmental Psychology, 34(1), 99–114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haden, C. A., & Fivush, R. (1996). Contextual variation in maternal conversational styles. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 42(2), 200–227.Google Scholar
Haden, C. A., Haine, R. A., & Fivush, R. (1997). Developing narrative structure in parent–child reminiscing across the preschool years. Developmental Psychology, 33(2), 295–307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hudson, J. A. (1990). The emergence of autobiographical memory in mother–child conversations. In Fivush, R. & Hudson, J. A. (Eds.), Knowing and remembering in young children (pp. 166–196). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hughes, C., & Dunn, J. (1997). “Pretend you didn't know”: Preschooler's talk about mental states in pretend play. Cognitive Development, 12(4), 381–403.Google Scholar
Hughes, C., & Dunn, J. (1998). Understanding mind and emotion: Longitudinal associations with mental-state talk between young friends. Developmental Psychology, 34(5), 1026–1037.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Instituto Nacional de Estadística e Informática (2007). Compendio estadístico: Logro educacional. Retrieved April 27, 2008 from http://www.inei.gob.pe.
Kleinknecht, E., & Beike, D. R. (2004). How knowing and doing inform an autobiography: Relations among preschoolers' theory of mind, narrative, and event memory skills. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18(6), 745–764.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labov, W., & Waletzky, J. (1967). Narrative analysis: Oral versions of personal experience. In Helm, J. (Ed.), Essays on the verbal and visual art. Seattle: University of Washington Press.Google Scholar
Leichtman, M. D., Wang, Q., & Pillemer, D. B. (2003). Cultural variations in interdependence and autobiographical memory: Lessons from Korea, China, India and the United States. In Fivush, R. & Haden, C. A. (Eds.), Autobiographical memory and the construction of a narrative self: Developmental and cultural perspectives (pp. 73–97). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Lewis, C., Freeman, N. H., Hagestadt, C., & Douglas, H. (1994). Narrative access and production in preschoolers' false belief reasoning. Cognitive Development, 9, 397–424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Mayer, M. (1969). Frog, where are you?New York: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
McCabe, A., & Peterson, C. (1991). Getting the story: A longitudinal study of parental styles in eliciting narratives and developing narrative skill. In McCabe, A. & Peterson, C. (Eds.), Developing narrative structure (pp. 217–253). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Melzi, G., & Caspe, M. (2005). Variations in maternal narrative styles during book-reading interactions. Narrative Inquiry, 15(1), 101–125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Melzi, G., & Fernández, C. (2004). Talking about past emotions: Conversations between Peruvian mothers and their preschool children. Sex Roles, 50(9–10), 153–177.Google Scholar
Melzi, G., Fernández, C., Caspe, M., & Dale, K. (2008). Agency and communion in mother–child conversations about the past: Cultural and gender variations. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Melzi, G., Schick, A., & Kennedy, J. (2007, April). Cultural variations in mother–child discourse strategies across narrative context. Poster presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, Boston, MA.
Miller, P., Fung, H., & Mintz, J. (1996). Self-construction through narrative practices: A Chinese and American comparison of early socialization. Ethos, 24(2), 237–280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, P. H., Wiley, A. R., Fung, H., & Liang, C. (1997). Personal storytelling as a medium of socialization in Chinese and American families. Child Development, 68(3), 557–568.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, P. H., Sandel, T. L., Liang, C., & Fung, H. (2001). Narrating transgressions in Longwood: The discourses, meanings, and paradoxes of an American socializing practice. Ethos, 29(2), 159–186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Minami, M., & McCabe, A. (1991). Haiku as a discourse regulation device: A stanza analysis of Japanese children's personal narratives. Language in Society, 20(4), 577–599.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Minami, M., & McCabe, A. (1995). Rice balls and bear hunts: Japanese and North American family narrative patterns. Journal of Child Language, 22(2), 423–445.Google Scholar
Mullen, M. K., & Yi, S. (1995). The cultural context of talk about the past: Implications for the development of autobiographical memory. Cognitive Development, 10(3), 407–419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. (1993). The psychological and social origins of autobiographical memory. Psychological Science, 4(1), 7–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. (2003). Narrative and self, myth and memory: Emergence of the cultural self. In Fivush, R. & Haden, C. A. (Eds.), Autobiographical memory and the construction of a narrative self: Developmental and cultural perspectives (pp. 3–28). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Nelson, K., & Fivush, R. (2004). The emergence of autobiographical memory: A social-cultural developmental theory. Psychological Review, 111(2), 486–511.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ochs, E., & Capps, L. (2001). Living narrative: Creating lives in everyday storytelling. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Peterson, C., Jesso, B., & McCabe, A. (1999). Encouraging narratives in preschoolers: An intervention study. Journal of Child Language, 26(1), 49–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1983). Developmental psycholinguistics: Three ways of looking at a child's narrative. New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reese, E. (2002). Social factors in the development of autobiographical memory: The state of the art. Social Development, 11(1), 124–142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reese, E., & Brown, N. (2000). Reminiscing and recounting in the preschool years. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14(1), 1–17.3.0.CO;2-G>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reese, E., & Cleveland, E. S. (2006). Mother–child reminiscing and children's understanding of mind. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. Special Issue: Parent-Child Discourse and the Early Development of Understanding, 52(1), 17–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reese, E., & Fivush, R. (1993). Parental styles of talking about the past. Developmental Psychology, 29(3), 596–606.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reese, E., Haden, C. A., & Fivush, R. (1993). Mother–child conversations about the past: Relationships of style and memory over time. Cognitive Development, 8(4), 403–430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rudek, D. J., & Haden, C. A. (2005). Mothers' and preschoolers' mental-state language during reminiscing over time. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 51(4), 523–549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schieffelin, B., & Ochs, E. (1986). Language socializationAnnual Review of Anthropology, 15, 163–191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, Q. (2001). “Did you have fun?” American and Chinese mother–child conversations about shared emotional experiences. Cognitive Development, 16(2), 693–715.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, Q. (2003). Emotion situation knowledge in American and Chinese preschool children and adults. Cognition and Emotion, 17(5), 725–746.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, Q. (2004). The emergence of cultural self-constructs: Autobiographical memory and self-description in European American and Chinese children. Developmental Psychology, 40(1), 3-15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, Q., & Fivush, R. (2005). Mother–child conversations of emotionally salient events: Exploring the functions of emotional reminiscing in European American and Chinese families. Social Development, 14(3), 473–495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, Q., Leichtman, M. D., & Davies, K. I. (2000). Sharing memories and telling stories: American and Chinese mothers and their 3-year-olds. Memory, 8(3), 159–177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Welch-Ross, M. K. (1995). An integrative model of the development of autobiographical memory. Developmental Review, 15(3), 338–365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Welch-Ross, M. K. (1997). Mother–child participation in conversation about the past: Relationships to preschoolers' theory of mind. Developmental Psychology, 33(4), 618–629.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Welch-Ross, M. K., Fasig, L. G., & Farrar, M. J. (1999). Predictors of preschoolers' self-knowledge: Reference to emotion and mental states in mother–child conversation about past events. Cognitive Development, 14(3), 401–422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×