Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-dfsvx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T05:58:21.543Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

7 - Seeing actions as hierarchically organized structures: Great ape manual skills

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2009

Andrew N. Meltzoff
Affiliation:
University of Washington
Wolfgang Prinz
Affiliation:
Max-Planck-Institut für psychologische Forschung, Germany
Get access

Summary

The word “imitation” has seen its meaning change many times over the years, a process that has narrowed down its scope onto an ever-decreasing range of referents. Most of these revisions have been driven by debate in the literature concerning nonhuman animals (hereafter, “animals”), but may have implications for those studying imitation in humans, especially at preverbal stages where many of the same issues occur. Before Thorndike's (1898) definition “learning to do an act from seeing it done,” imitation seemed not even to require a causal role for observation – and this sense is preserved in everyday talk, when people say, for instance, “this is a case of nature imitating art.” More recently, the notion of stimulus enhancement was introduced (Spence, 1937): seeing some act done in a particular place, or to some particular object, has the effect of increasing the observer's probability of going to that place or interacting with that object. This, as Spence noted, would make subsequent individual learning very much more efficient, and many behaviors that were formerly (and are still popularly) considered imitation were thereby explained away – as trial and error learning aided by social circumstances (Galef, 1988). Tomasello further drew attention to the possibility of learning about the physical situation as an indirect consequence of another's behavior, introducing the concept of emulation (Tomasello, 1990).

Type
Chapter
Information
The Imitative Mind
Development, Evolution and Brain Bases
, pp. 122 - 140
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2002

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×