Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-p2v8j Total loading time: 0.001 Render date: 2024-05-15T10:21:54.255Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Self- and Teacher-Rated School Adjustment in MZ and DZ Twins

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 August 2014

Siv Fischbein*
Affiliation:
The Department of Educational Research, Stockholm Institute of Education
*
Department of Educational Research, Institute of Education, Box 34 103, S-100 26 Stockholm, Sweden

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

In a Swedish longitudinal twin study, teacher ratings of school adjustment were collected in grades 3 and 6 for approximately 80 pairs of MZ twins, 100 pairs of DZ like-sex twins, and 70 pairs of opposite-sex twins. These same groups of twins then rated their own school adjustment in grades 4 and 6 as seen by the home, the school, their classmates, and themselves.

A comparison of within-pair similarity for the different types of self-ratings tends to show more consistency and a higher concordance for MZ compared to DZ in grade 6 than in grade 4.

The teacher ratings tend to show a larger and more consistent difference between MZ and DZ than the twins' own ratings.

The results reported have certain implications for heritability estimates based on different types of ratings. Ratings by others thus seem to give the highest intraclass correlations, probably due to a certain halo effect. Self ratings, on the other hand, tend to fluctuate more over time at least for children before and at puberty. Also the construction of items seems to influence the magnitude of the correlations.

Type
Twin Development and Temperament
Copyright
Copyright © The International Society for Twin Studies 1984

References

REFERENCES

1.Cattel, RB (1982): The Inheritance of Personality and Ability. New York: Academic Press, Inc.Google Scholar
2.Fischbein, S (1979): Heredity-Environment Influence on Growth and Development During Adolescence. Lund: Liber.Google Scholar
3.Harris, EL, Rose, RJ (1977): Personality Resemblance in Twin Children: Comparison of Self-Descriptions with Mothers' Ratings. Paper presented at the Second International Congress on Twin Studies, 08 29 - September 1 in Washington D.C.Google Scholar
4.Lerner, RM (1982): A “Goodness of Fit” model of person-context interaction. Paper presented at the Conference on “Human Development from a Person-Environment Interaction Perspective”. Stockholm, 06 28 - July 1.Google Scholar
5.Ljung, B -O (1966): Intra-class correlation (in Swed.). Report no 16 from the Department of Educational Research, School of Education, Stockholm.Google Scholar
6.Ljung, B -O, Bergsten-Brucefors, A, Lindgren, G (1974): The secular trend in physical growth in Sweden. Ann Hum Biol 1:245256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7.Ljung, B -O, Fischbein, S, Lindgren, G (1977): A comparison of growth in twins and singleton controls of matched age followed longitudinally from 10 to 18 years. Ann Hum Biol 4:405415.Google Scholar
8.Loehlin, JC, Nichols, RC (1976): Heredity, Environment, and Personality: A Study of 850 Twins. Austin: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
9.Mittler, P (1971): The Study of Twins. Penguin Books Ltd.Google Scholar
10.Nordqvist, T (1977): What others say and you think (in Swed.). Report from the Department of Educational Research at the Stockholm Institute of Education.Google Scholar
11.Plomin, R (1981): Heredity and temperament: A comparison of twin data for self-report questionnaires, parental ratings, and objectively assessed behavior. In Gedda, L, Parisi, P, Nance, WE (eds): Twin Research: Part B, Intelligence, Personality, and Development, New York: Alan R. Liss, p 269278.Google Scholar