Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-5nwft Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-06T19:01:41.538Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Genotype rather than non-genetic behavioural transmission determines the temperament of Merino lambs

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2023

S Bickell
Affiliation:
School of Animal Biology, Faculty of Natural & Agricultural Sciences, The University of Western Australia, 35 Stirling Highway, Crawley, WA 6009, Australia
P Poindron
Affiliation:
Equipe Comportement, Neurobiologie, Université François Rabelais, Nouzilly, France
R Nowak
Affiliation:
Equipe Comportement, Neurobiologie, Université François Rabelais, Nouzilly, France
A Chadwick
Affiliation:
School of Animal Biology, Faculty of Natural & Agricultural Sciences, The University of Western Australia, 35 Stirling Highway, Crawley, WA 6009, Australia
D Ferguson
Affiliation:
CSIRO Livestock Industries, Livestock Welfare, Locked Bag 1 Armidale, NSW 2350, Australia
D Blache*
Affiliation:
School of Animal Biology, Faculty of Natural & Agricultural Sciences, The University of Western Australia, 35 Stirling Highway, Crawley, WA 6009, Australia
*
* Contact for correspondence and requests for reprints: dbla@animals.uwa.edu.au

Abstract

Merino ewes have been selected, over 18 generations, for calm (C) or nervous (N) temperament using using an arena test and an isolation box test. We investigated the relative contributions of genotype versus the post-partum behaviour of the dam on the temperament of the lambs using a cross-fostering procedure. Forty-eight multiparous calm and 52 nervous ewes were artificially inseminated with the semen of a sire of the same temperament. At birth, 32 lambs of a given temperament line were cross fostered to ewes from the other line (16 N × C, 16 C × N), 34 lambs were cross fostered to ewes from the same line (15 C × C, 19 N × N) and 30 lambs were left with their birth mother (15 C, 15 N), to control for the effect of cross fostering. The temperament of the progeny was assessed at two occasions, one week after birth by measuring locomotor activity during an open-field test and at weaning (16 weeks) by measuring locomotor activity during an arena test and agitation score measured during an isolation box test. There was a genotype effect but no maternal or fostering effect on the lamb temperament at one week. This may be because the maternal behaviour of the foster ewes did not differ considerably between the calm and nervous mothers during adoption or within the first week, post partum. Similarly, at weaning, only a genotype effect was found on the locomotor and agitation score. Therefore, it appears that temperament in Merino sheep is mainly determined by the genetic transmission of the trait across generations rather than behaviours learned from the mother.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2009 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alexander, G, Poindron, P, Le Neindre, P, Stevens, D, Levy, F and Bradley, L 1986 Importance of the first hour post-partum for exclusive maternal bonding in sheep. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 16: 295300CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anisman, H, Zaharia, MD, Meaney, MJ and Merali, Z 1998 Do early life events permanently alter behavioral and hormonal responses to stressors? International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience 16: 149164CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barbazanges, A, Piazza, PV, Le Moal, M and Maccari, S 1996 maternal glucocorticoid secretion mediates long term effects of prenatal stress. Journal of Neuroscience 16: 39433949CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beausoleil, NJ, Blache, D, Stafford, KJ, Mellor, DJ and Noble, ADL 2008 Exploring the basis of divergent selection for ‘temperament’ in domestic sheep. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 109: 261274CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickell, S 2005 Sheep selected for a low emotional reactivity are less stressed when faced with a novel object. Honours Thesis, School of Animal Biology, The University of Western Australia, Perth, AustraliaGoogle Scholar
Blache, D and Ferguson, D 2005a Genetic estimates for temperament traits in sheep breeds. Final report AHW 140 p 18. Meat and Livestock Australia: Sydney, AustraliaGoogle Scholar
Blache, D and Ferguson, DM 2005b Increasing sheep meat production efficiency and animal welfare by selection for temperament. Final report SHGEN.025 p 59. Meat and Livestock Australia: Sydney, AustraliaGoogle Scholar
Boissy, A, Bouix, J, Orgeur, P, Poindron, P, Bibe, B and Le Neindre, P 2005 Genetic analysis of emotional reactivity in sheep: effects of genotypes of the lambs and their dams. Genetics Selection Evolution 37: 381401CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braastad, BO 1998 Effects of prenatal stress on behaviour of offspring of laboratory and farmed mammals. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 61: 159180CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broadhurst, PL 1961 Analysis of maternal effects in the inheritance of behaviour. Animal Behaviour 9: 129141CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broom, DM 1986 Indicators of poor welfare. British Veterinary Journal 142: 524CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caldji, C, Diorio, J and Meaney, MJ 2000 Variations in maternal care in infancy regulate the development of stress reactivity. Biological Psychiatry 48: 11641174Google ScholarPubMed
Caldji, C, Tannenbaum, B, Sharma, S, Francis, D, Plotsky, PM and Meaney, MJ 1998 Maternal care during infancy regulates the development of neural systems mediating the expression of behavioral fearfulness in adulthood in the rat. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 95: 53355340CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corner, RA, Kenyon, PR, Stafford, JK, West, DM and Oliver, MH 2006 The effect of mid-pregnancy shearing or yarding stress on ewe post-natal behaviour and the birth weight and post-natal behaviour of their lambs. Livestock Science 102: 121129CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dwyer, CM and Lawrence, AB 2000 Effects of maternal genotype and behaviour on the behavioural development of their offspring in sheep. Behaviour 137: 16291654Google Scholar
Francis, DD, Diorio, J, Liu, D and Meaney, MJ 1999 Nongenomic transmission across generations in maternal behaviour and stress responses in the rat. Science 286: 11551158Google ScholarPubMed
Glazier, DS 2002 Resource-allocation rules and the heritability of traits. Evolution 56: 16961700CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hale, EB 1969 Domestication and the evolution of behavior. In: Hafez, ESE (ed) The Behaviour of Domestic Animals pp 2242, Tindall & Cassell: London, UKGoogle Scholar
Hargreaves, AL and Hutson, GD 1990a Changes in heart-rate, plasma-cortisol and haematocrit of sheep during a shearing procedure. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 26: 91101CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hargreaves, AL and Hutson, GD 1990b An evaluation of the contribution of isolation, up-ending and wool removal to the stress response to shearing. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 26: 103113Google Scholar
Hargreaves, AL and Hutson, GD 1990c Some effects of repeated handling on stress responses in sheep. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 26: 253265CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keverne, EB, Levy, F, Poindron, P and Lindsay, DR 1983 Vaginal stimulation: An important determinant of maternal bonding in sheep. Science 219: 8183CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kilgour, RJ, Melville, GJ and Greenwood, PL 2006 Individual differences in the reaction of beef cattle to situations involving social isolation, close proximity of humans, restraint and novelty. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 99: 2140CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mackenzie, SA, Oltenaca, EAB and Leighton, E 1985 Heritability estimate for temperament scores in German shepherd dogs and its genetic correlation with hip dysplasia. Behavior Genetics 15: 475482CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meaney, MJ 2001 The development of individual differences in behavioral and endocrine responses to stress. Annual Review of Neuroscience 24: 11611192CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moberg, GP, Anderson, CO and Underwood, TR 1980 Otogeny of the adrenal and behavioural responses of lambs to emotional stress. Journal of Animal Science 51: 138142CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mousseau, TA and Fox, CW 1998 The adaptive significance of maternal effects. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 13: 403407CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, PM 1999 Maternal behaviour and rearing ability of Merino ewes can be improved by strategic feed supplementation during late pregnancy and selection for calm temperament. PhD Thesis, The University of Western Australia, Perth, AustraliaGoogle Scholar
Murphy, PM, Lindsay, DR and Le Neindre, P 1998 Temperament of Merino ewes influences maternal behaviour and survival of lambs. Proceedings of the 32nd Congress of the International Society for Applied Ethology 32: 131Google Scholar
Murphy, PM, Purvis, IW, Lindsay, DR, Le Neindre, P, Orgeur, P and Poindron, P 1994 Measures of temperament are highly repeatable in Merino sheep and some are related to maternal behaviour. Proceedings of the Australian Society of Animal Production 20: 247250Google Scholar
Poindron, P, Levy, F and Krehbiel, D 1988 Genital, olfactory, and endocrine interactions in the development of maternal behaviour in the parturient ewe. Psychoneuroendocrinology 13: 99125Google ScholarPubMed
Poindron, P, Nowak, R, Lévy, F, Porter, RH and Schaal, B 1993 Development of exclusive mother-young bonding in sheep and goats. Oxford Reviews of Reproductive Biology 15: 311364Google ScholarPubMed
Price, EO 1984 Behavioral aspects of animal domestication. The Quarterly Review of Biology 59: 132Google Scholar
Putu, IG 1988 Maternal behaviour in Merino ewes during the first two days after parturition and survival of lambs. The University of Western Australia: Perth, AustraliaGoogle Scholar
Richard-Yris, MA, Michel, N and Bertin, A 2004 Nongenomic inheritance of emotional reactivity in Japanese quail. Developmental Psychobiology 46: 112CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roussel, S, Hemsworth, PH, Leruste, H, White, C, Duvaux-Ponter, C, Nowak, R and Boissy, A 2006 Repeated transport and isolation during pregnancy in ewes: Effects on the reactivity to humans and to their offspring after lambing. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 97: 172189CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmutz, SM, Stookey, JM, Winkelman-Sim, DC, Waltz, CS, Plante, Y and Buchanan, FC 2001 A QTL study of cattle behavioral traits in embryo transfer families. The Journal of Heredity 92: 290292Google ScholarPubMed
Webster, J 2005 Animal Welfare: Limping Towards Eden. Blackwell Publishing Ltd: Oxford, UKCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weinstock, M 1997 Does prenatal stress impair coping and regulation of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 21: 110CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinstock, M, Poltyrev, T, Schorer-Apelbaum, D, Men, D and McCarty, R 1998 Effect of prenatal stress on plasma corticosterone and catecholamines in response to footshock in rats. Physiology & Behavior 64: 439444CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zarrow, MX, Philpott, JE and Denenberg, VH 1970 Passage of 14C-4-corticosterone from the rat mother to the foetus and neonate. Nature 226: 10581059CrossRefGoogle Scholar