Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-5g6vh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-29T16:49:29.368Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Phonological and lexical development in normal and late-talking toddlers

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 March 2016

Donna J. Thal*
Affiliation:
San Diego State University
Melinda Oroz*
Affiliation:
San Diego State University
Valerie McCaw*
Affiliation:
San Diego State University
*
Donna J. Thai, Department of Communication Disorders, San Diego State University, San Diego, CA 92182. e-mail: , dthal@mail.sdsu.edu
Donna J. Thai, Department of Communication Disorders, San Diego State University, San Diego, CA 92182. e-mail: , dthal@mail.sdsu.edu
Donna J. Thai, Department of Communication Disorders, San Diego State University, San Diego, CA 92182. e-mail: , dthal@mail.sdsu.edu

Abstract

Toddlers in the lowest tenth percentile for lexical production, based on parental report (late talkers), were compared with age- and language-matched controls on measures of phonetic complexity, lexical development, and grammatical complexity. Late talkers were placed in the pre-meaningful speech group if they produced fewer than 10 different words or in the meaningful speech group if they produced more than 10 different words in a spontaneous language sample. Late talkers who had entered the meaningful speech stage scored higher than those who had not on most of the measures of phonology, lexical, and grammatical complexity in intelligible speech, but not on measures of phonological development in babble. These results are compatible with other studies that found an overlap between phonology, lexicon, and grammar, as well as with those that suggested the importance of true consonant production for lexical development.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1995

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1979). The emergence of symbob: Cognition and communication in early infancy. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
de Boysson-Bardies, B., & Vihman, M. (1991). Adaptation to language: Evidence from babbling and first words in four languages. Language, 67, 297319.Google Scholar
Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, S., Thai, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J., Pethick, P., & Reilly, J. (1993). The MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories. San Diego, CA: Singular Press.Google Scholar
Fischel, J., Whitehurst, G., & Conners, G. (1987, April). Language growth in children with expressive language delay. Paper based on a presentation at the Biennial Meetings of the Society for Research in Child Development, Baltimore, MD.Google Scholar
Frankenburg, W., & Dodds, J. (1969). The Denver Developmental Screening Test. Denver, CO: Denver Developmental Materials.Google Scholar
Lahey, M. (1988). Language disorders and language development. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B., & Snow, C. (1985). The child language data exchange system. Journal of Child Language, 12, 271296.Google Scholar
Marchman, V., Miller, R., & Bates, E. (1991). Babble and first words in children with focal brain injury. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olswang, L., Stoel-Gammon, C., Coggins, T., & Carpenter, R. (1987). Assessingprelinguistic and early linguistic behaviors in developmentally young children. Seattle: University of Washington Press.Google Scholar
Paul, R. (1989, April). Profiles of toddlers with delayed expressive language development. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Kansas City, MO.Google Scholar
Paul, R., & Jennings, P. (1992). Phonological behavior in toddlers with slow expressive language development. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35, 99107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paul, R., Spangle-Looney, S., & Dahm, P. (1991). Communication and socialization skills at ages 2 and 3 in “late-talking” young children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 858865.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams, and imitation in childhood. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Rescorla, L., & Schwartz, E. (1990). Outcome of toddlers with specific expressive language delay. Applied Psycholinguistics, 11, 393408.Google Scholar
Scarborough, H. (1990). Index of Productive Syntax. Applied Psycholinguistics, 11, 122.Google Scholar
Stark, R., & Tallal, P. (1981). Selection of children with specific language deficits. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 46, 114122.Google Scholar
Stoel-Gammon, C. (1985). Phonetic inventory, 15-24 months: A longitudinal study. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 28, 505512.Google Scholar
Stoel-Gammon, C. (1989). Prespeech and early speech development of two late talkers. First Language, 9, 207224.Google Scholar
Stoel-Gammon, C. (1991). Normal and disordered phonology in two year olds. Topics in Language Disorders, 11, 2132.Google Scholar
Thai, D., & Tobias, S. (1992). Communicative gestures in children with delayed onset of oral expressive vocabulary. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35, 12811289.Google Scholar
Thai, D., & Tobias, S. (1994). Relationships between language and gesture in normally developing and late-talking toddlers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 157170.Google Scholar
Thai, D., Tobias, S., & Morrison, D. (1991). Language and gesture in late talkers: A 1-year follow-up. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 604612.Google Scholar
Vihman, M. (1985). Language differentiation by the bilingual infant. Journal of Child Language, 12, 297324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vihman, M., Ferguson, C. & Elbert, M. (1986). Phonological development from babbling to speech: Common tendencies and individual differences. Applied Psycholinguistics, 7, 340.Google Scholar
Vihman, M., Macken, M., Miller, R., Simmons, H., & Miller, J. (1985). From babbling to speech: A reassessment of the continuity issue. Language, 61, 397445.Google Scholar
Vihman, M., & Miller, R. (1988). Words and babble at the threshold of language acquisition. In Smith, M. D. & Locke, J. L. (Eds.), The emergent lexicon: The child's development of a linguistic vocabulary (pp. 151183). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Weismer, S., Murray-Branch, J., & Miller, J. (1994). A prospective longitudinal study of language development in late talkers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 852867.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitehurst, G., Fischel, J., Arnold, D., & Lonigan, C. (1992). Evaluating outcomes with children with expressive language delay. In Warren, S. & Reichle, J. (Eds.), Causes and effects in communication and language intervention (Vol. 1, pp. 277313). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing.Google Scholar
Whitehurst, G., Smith, M., Fischel, J., Arnold, D., & Lonigan, C. (1991). The continuity of babble and speech in children with specific expressive language delay. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 11211129.Google Scholar