Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-hfldf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-14T06:58:10.700Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Characteristics of Traumatic Memories in Normal Subjects

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 June 2009

Ineke Wessel
Affiliation:
Limburg University, The Netherlands
Harald Merckelbach
Affiliation:
Limburg University, The Netherlands

Abstract

In recent years, there has been considerable debate about how accurately people remember emotionally significant information. Early studies concerned with “flashbulb memories” claimed that emotional memories are accurate and persistent. However, recent studies indicate that flashbulb memories are subject to deterioration over time. This finding fits with the idea that individuals who have experienced a traumatic event do not retain all the details of the trauma equally well. Instead, they have relatively good memory for central detail information and relatively poor memory for peripheral details of the traumatic situation. Using self-report data of a normal, healthy sample, Christianson and Loftus (1990) found evidence for this idea. The current study is an attempt to replicate Christianson and Loftus' finding in a normal student population (N = 106). Subjects were asked to describe their most traumatic situation. They rated the vividness of the memory, the intensity of emotion at the time of the trauma and at the time of recall, the amount of reliving, and the amount of central and peripheral detail information remembered. Results indicate that subjects who reported strong emotion at the time of the trauma remembered significantly more central detail information than subjects with relatively low past emotionality ratings. However, no differences in the amount of peripheral detail information remembered were found.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © British Association for Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapies 1994

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

American Psychiatric Association (1987). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Third Edition- Revised. Washington: APA.Google Scholar
Bradley, M. M., Greenwald, M. K., Petry, M. C. and Lang, P. J. (1992). Remembering pictures: pleasure and arousal in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 18, 379390.Google ScholarPubMed
Brown, R. and Kulik, J. (1977). Flashbulb memories. Cognition 5, 7379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bohannon, J. N. (1988). Flashbulb memories for the Space Shuttle disaster. Cognition 29, 179196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christianson, S.-Å. (1989). Flashbulb memories: special, but not so special. Memory and Cognition 17, 435443.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christianson, S.-Å. (1992). Emotional stress and eyewitness memory: a critical review. Psychological Bulletin 112, 284309.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christianson, S.-Å. and Hübinette, B. (1993). Hands up! A study of witnesses' emotional reactions and memories associated with bank robberies. Applied Cognitive Psychology 7, 365379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christianson, S.-Å. and Loftus, E. F. (1990). Some characteristics of people's traumatic memories. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 28, 195198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christianson, S.-Å. and Loftus, E. F. (1991). Remembering emotional events: the fate of detailed information. Cognition and Emotion 5, 81108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christianson, S.-Å. and Nilsson, L.-G. (1989). Hysterical amnesia: a case of aversively motivated isolation of memory. In Archer, T. and Nilsson, L.-G. (Eds). Aversion, Avoidance and Anxiety: Perspectives on Aversively Motivated Behavior. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Easterbrook, J. A. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. Psychological Review 66, 183201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kolk, B. A. Van Der and Kadish, W. (1987). Amnesia, dissociation, and the return of the repressed. In van der Kolk, B. A. (Ed). Psychological Trauma. Washington: American Psychiatric Press.Google Scholar
Kopelman, M. D. (1987). Amnesia: organic and psychogenic. British Journal of Psychiatry 150, 428442.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loftus, E. F. and Burns, T. E. (1982). Mental shock can produce retrograde amnesia. Memory and Cognition 10, 318323.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neisser, U. and Harsch, N. (1992). Phantom flashbulbs: false recollections of hearing the news about Challenger. In Winograd, E. and Neisser, U. (Eds). Affect and Accuracy in Recall: Studies of “Flashbulb” Memories. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Pillemer, D. B. (1984) Flashbulb memories of the assassination attempt on President Reagan. Cognition 16, 6380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schacter, D. L. (1986). Amnesia and crime: how much do we really know? American Psychologist 41, 286295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schacter, D. L., Wang, P. L., Tulving, E. and Freedman, M. (1982). Functional retrograde amnesia: a quantitative case study. Neuropsychologia 20, 523532.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winograd, E. and Killinger, W. A. (1983). Relating age at encoding in early childhood to adult recall: development of flashbulb memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology (General) 112, 413422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yuille, J. C. and Cutshall, J. L. (1986). A case study of eyewitness memory of a crime. Journal of Applied Psychology 2, 291301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.