Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-jbqgn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-20T16:32:07.226Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The selection of coloured stimuli by the horse (Equus caballus)

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 February 2018

C.A. Hall
Affiliation:
School of Animal, Rural and Environmental Sciences, Nottingham Trent University, Brackenhurst Campus, Southwell, Nottinghamshire NG25 0QF, U.K
H.J. Cassaday
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Nottingham, University Park, Nottingham NG7 2RD, U.K
C.J. Vincent
Affiliation:
Psychology, Brain and Behaviour, University of Newcastle upon Tyne, Newcastle upon Tyne NE1 7RU, U.K, Email: carol.hall@ntu.ac.uk
A.M. Derrington
Affiliation:
Psychology, Brain and Behaviour, University of Newcastle upon Tyne, Newcastle upon Tyne NE1 7RU, U.K, Email: carol.hall@ntu.ac.uk
Get access

Extract

Colour is an important feature that increases the visibility of objects and may aid recognition of con-specifics. The adaptive significance of the ability to utilize colour cues has been demonstrated in a number of species, in particular in relation to food selection. The evolution of trichromatic colour vision in primates has been associated with the advantages that it provided in the selection of yellow and orange fruits (Mollon, 1989) or tender young red foliage (Dominy and Lucas, 2001) from a background of green. The horse is a generalist herbivore that must both select nutritious plants and avoid toxic ones. Familiarity is an important factor in this selection process (Augner et al., 1998), although it is not clear exactly what features are used to recognise previously ingested and “safe” herbage.

The horse has been shown to possess dichromatic colour vision. There are two types of cone photopigment in the equine retina, with spectral peaks at 429 and 545 nanometres (Macuda, 2000).

Type
Posters
Copyright
Copyright © British Society of Animal Production 2006

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Augner, M., Provenza, F.D. and Villalba, J.J. (1998) A rule of thumb in mammalian herbivores? Animal Behaviour. 56: 337345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brainard, D.H. (1997) The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision 10: 433436.Google Scholar
Dominy, N.J. and Lucas, P.W. (2001) The ecological importance of trichromatic colour vision in primates. Nature. 410: 363366.Google Scholar
Govardovskii, V.I., Fyhrquist, N., Reuter, T., Kuzmin, D.G. and Donner, K. (2000) In search of the visual pigment template. Visual Neuroscience 17: 509528.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grzimek, B. (1952) Versuche uber das Farbsehen von Pflanzenessern. Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie 9: 2339.Google Scholar
Macuda, T. (2000) Equine colour vision. PhD thesis: University of Western Ontario. Google Scholar
Mollon, J.D. (1989) “Tho’ she kneel’d in that place where they grew…” The uses and origins of primate colour vision. Journal of Experimental Biology. 146: 2138.Google Scholar
Morgan, M.J., Adam, A. and Mollon, J.D. (1992) Dichromats detect colour-camouflaged objects that are not detected by trichromats. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. B. 248: 291295.Google Scholar
Wyldes, C.A. (2004) Colour vision in the horse and its importance in management and training. PhD thesis: University of Nottingham. Google Scholar