Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-skm99 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T16:11:26.988Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Personalised ecology and the future of biodiversity

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 July 2023

Kevin J. Gaston*
Affiliation:
Environment and Sustainability Institute, University of Exeter, Penryn, UK
Benjamin B. Phillips
Affiliation:
Environment and Sustainability Institute, University of Exeter, Penryn, UK
Masashi Soga
Affiliation:
Graduate School of Agricultural and Life Sciences, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
*
Corresponding author: Kevin J. Gaston; Email: k.j.gaston@exeter.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The future of biodiversity lies not just in the strategies and mechanisms by which ecosystems and species are practically best protected from anthropogenic pressures. It lies also, and perhaps foremost, in the many billions of decisions that people make that, intentionally or otherwise, shape their impact on nature and the conservation policies and interventions that are implemented. Personalised ecology – the set of direct sensory interactions that each of us has with nature – is one important consideration in understanding the decisions that people make. Indeed, it has long been argued that people’s personalised ecologies have powerful implications, as captured in such concepts as biophilia, extinction of experience and shifting baselines. In this paper, we briefly review the connections between personalised ecology and the future of biodiversity, and the ways in which personalised ecologies might usefully be enhanced to improve that future.

Type
Review
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Impact statement

Protecting and restoring nature depend on understanding the billions of decisions that people make. Such decisions range from simple acts like caring for wildlife in one’s garden to more complex decisions like what products to buy or which political candidate to support. These decisions are determined in part by direct experiences of, and relationships with, nature. These may affect nature directly (e.g., determining how much an individual disrupts wildlife habitats) or indirectly (e.g., affecting one’s thoughts and attitudes toward nature). Understanding how people’s relationships with nature differ, how they are changing and how they relate to people’s pro-nature attitudes and behaviours can help to reveal strategies that can benefit biodiversity. For example, people who feel more connected to nature are more likely to take action to help protect it. People’s relationships with nature might be improved, for example, by increasing the availability and accessibility of natural environments, and people’s inclination, ability and confidence to engage with nature. Such efforts have the potential to create a virtuous cycle of human–nature interactions, whereby increased engagement with nature leads to greater appreciation, enjoyment and desire to protect it. This is particularly important at a time when people’s relationships with nature are often declining.

Introduction

Scientific discussion of how to slow and reverse global biodiversity loss has concentrated far more on ecological solutions than on social change. This has been exemplified by papers published in the run-up to the Fifteenth Conference of the Parties (COP-15) to the UN Convention on Biological Diversity (CBD), where research focused strongly on how best to set targets and measure progress for conservation (e.g., Watson et al., Reference Watson, Keith, Strassburg, Venter, Williams and Nicholson2020; Obura et al., Reference Obura, Katerere, Mayet, Kaelo, Msweli, Mather, Harris, Louis, Kramer, Teferi, Samoilys, Lewis, Bennie, Kumah, Isaacs and Nantongo2021; Allan et al., Reference Allan, Possingham, Atkinson, Waldron, Di Marco, Butchart, Adams, Kissling, Worsdell, Sandbrook, Gibbon, Kumar, Mehta, Maron, Williams, Jones, Wintle, Reside and Watson2022; Leadley et al., Reference Leadley, Gonzalez, Obura, Krug, Londoño-Murcia, Millette, Radulovici, Rankovic, Shannon, Archer, Armah, Bax, Chaudhari, Costello, Dávalos, Roque, DeClerck, Dee, Essl, Ferrier, Genovesi, Guariguata, Hashimoto, Speranza, Isbell, Kok, Lavery, Leclère, Loyola, Lwasa, McGeoch, Mori, Nicholson, Ochoa, Öllerer, Polasky, Rondinini, Schroer, Selomane, Shen, Strassburg, Sumaila, Tittensor, Turak, Urbina, Vallejos, Vázquez-Domínguez, Verburg, Visconti, Woodley and Xu2022), the importance and maintenance of wilderness areas (e.g., Aycrigg et al., Reference Aycrigg, Mccarley, Belote and Martinuzzi2022; Pérez‐Hämmerle et al., Reference Pérez‐Hämmerle, Moon, Venegas‐Li, Maxwell, Simmonds, Venter, Garnett, Possingham and Watson2022), identifying priority areas for biodiversity conservation, and for expanding, and increasing the effectiveness of, protected area systems (e.g., Hanson et al., Reference Hanson, Rhodes, Butchart, Buchanan, Rondinini, Ficetola and Fuller2020; Ward et al., Reference Ward, Saura, Williams, Ramírez-Delgado, Arafeh-Dalmau, Allan, Venter, Dubois and Watson2020; Allan et al., Reference Allan, Possingham, Atkinson, Waldron, Di Marco, Butchart, Adams, Kissling, Worsdell, Sandbrook, Gibbon, Kumar, Mehta, Maron, Williams, Jones, Wintle, Reside and Watson2022; Brennan et al., Reference Brennan, Naidoo, Greenstreet, Mehrabi, Ramankutty and Kremen2022; Wauchope et al., Reference Wauchope, Jones, Geldmann, Simmons, Amano, Blanco, Fuller, Johnston, Langendoen, Mundkur, Nagy and Sutherland2022) and understanding the threats to, and recovery of, individual species (e.g., Grace et al., Reference Grace, Akçakaya, Bennett, Brooks, Heath, Hedges, Hilton‐Taylor, Hoffmann, Hochkirch, Jenkins, Keith, Long, Mallon, Meijaard, Milner‐Gulland, Rodriguez, Stephenson, Stuart, Young, Acebes, Alfaro‐Shigueto, Alvarez‐Clare, Andriantsimanarilafy, Arbetman, Azat, Bacchetta, Badola, Barcelos, Barreiros, Basak, Berger, Bhattacharyya, Bino, Borges, Boughton, Brockmann, Buckley, Burfield, Burton, Camacho‐Badani, Cano‐Alonso, Carmichael, Carrero, Carroll, Catsadorakis, Chapple, Chapron, Chowdhury, Claassens, Cogoni, Constantine, Craig, Cunningham, Dahal, Daltry, Das, Dasgupta, Davey, Davies, Develey, Elangovan, Fairclough, Febbraro, Fenu, Fernandes, Fernandez, Finucci, Földesi, Foley, Ford, Forstner, García, Garcia‐Sandoval, Gardner, Garibay‐Orijel, Gatan‐Balbas, Gauto, Ghazi, Godfrey, Gollock, González, Grant, Gray, Gregory, Grunsven, Gryzenhout, Guernsey, Gupta, Hagen, Hagen, Hall, Hallerman, Hare, Hart, Hartdegen, Harvey‐Brown, Hatfield, Hawke, Hermes, Hitchmough, Hoffmann, Howarth, Hudson, Hussain, Huveneers, Jacques, Jorgensen, Katdare, Katsis, Kaul, Kaunda‐Arara, Keith‐Diagne, Kraus, Lima, Lindeman, Linsky, Louis, Loy, Lughadha, Mangel, Marinari, Martin, Martinelli, McGowan, McInnes, Mendes, Millard, Mirande, Money, Monks, Morales, Mumu, Negrao, Nguyen, MdNH, Norbury, Nordmeyer, Norris, O’Brien, Oda, Orsenigo, Outerbridge, Pasachnik, Pérez‐Jiménez, Pike, Pilkington, Plumb, Portela, Prohaska, Quintana, Rakotondrasoa, Ranglack, Rankou, Rawat, Reardon, Rheingantz, Richter, Rivers, Rogers, Rosa, Rose, Royer, Ryan, Mitcheson, Salmon, Salvador, Samways, Sanjuan, Santos, Sasaki, Schutz, Scott, Scott, Serena, Sharma, Shuey, Silva, Simaika, Smith, JLY, Sultana, Talukdar, Tatayah, Thomas, Tringali, Trinh‐Dinh, Tuboi, Usmani, Vasco‐Palacios, Vié, Virens, Walker, Wallace, Waller, Wang, Wearn, Weerd, Weigmann, Willcox, Woinarski, Yong and Young2021; Mair et al., Reference Mair, Bennun, Brooks, Butchart, Bolam, Burgess, Ekstrom, Milner-Gulland, Hoffmann, Ma, Macfarlane, Raimondo, Rodrigues, Shen, Strassburg, Beatty, Mez-Creutzberg, Iribarrem, Irmadhiany, Lacerda, Mattos, Parakkasi, Tognelli, Bennett, Bryan, Carbone, Chaudhary, Eiselin, Fonseca, Galt, Geschke, Glew, Goedicke, Green, Gregory, Hill, Hole, Hughes, Hutton, Keijzer, Navarro, Lughadha, Plumptre, Puydarrieux, Possingham, Rankovic, Regan, Rondinini, Schneck, Siikamäki, Sendashonga, Seutin, Sinclair, Skowno, Soto-Navarro, Stuart, Temple, Vallier, Verones, Viana, Watson, Bezeng, Hm, Burfield, Clausnitzer, Clubbe, Cox, Freyhof, Gerber, Hilton-Taylor, Jenkins, Joolia, Joppa, Koh, Lacher, Langhammer, Long, Mallon, Pacifici, Polidoro, Pollock, Rivers, Roach, Rodríguez, Smart, Young, Hawkins and McGowan2021; Bolam et al., Reference Bolam, Ahumada, Akçakaya, Brooks, Elliott, Hoban, Mair, Mallon, McGowan, Raimondo, Rodríguez, Roe, Seddon, Shen, Stuart, Watson and Butchart2022). There is no doubt that these are all vitally important issues. However, the loss of biodiversity has been an outcome of many billions of decisions (with varying degrees of independence) by individual people. Such decisions, intentional and otherwise, include how people use and manage any natural resources that they have direct influence over (from domestic gardens and backyards to larger land and sea holdings), what resources and items they purchase as consumers, what organisations they encourage and assist (e.g., conservation NGOs), and which local, regional and national governmental policies and management interventions they support. These pathways, and how they can best be influenced, have long been studied within environmental sustainability. They have, however, attracted far less attention from the biodiversity conservation community.

This is not to say that behavioural decision making and social change have received no attention in the context of biodiversity conservation (Thomas-Walters et al., Reference Thomas-Walters, McCallum, Montgomery, Petros, Wan and Veríssimo2023). Interest has included such issues as managing demand for wildlife products (MacFarlane et al., Reference MacFarlane, Hurlstone, Ecker, Ferraro, van der Linden, Wan, Veríssimo, Burgess, Chen, Hall, Hollands and Sutherland2022), the promotion of farmers’ pro-environmental practices (Mastrangelo et al., Reference Mastrangelo, Gavin, Laterra, Linklater and Milfont2014), the application of ‘nudge theory’ (Nelson et al., Reference Nelson, Partelow and Schlüter2019), conservation messaging (Kidd et al., Reference Kidd, Garrard, Bekessy, Mills, Camilleri, Fidler, Fielding, Gordon, Gregg, Kusmanoff, Louis, Moon, Robinson, Selinske, Shanahan and Adams2019), the effectiveness of social marketing campaigns (Green et al., Reference Green, Crawford, Williamson and DeWan2019) and the influence of visual media on human–nature interactions (Silk et al., Reference Silk, Correia, Veríssimo, Verma and Crowley2021). Nonetheless, it does seem remarkable that, despite being raised at least a decade ago (e.g., St John et al., Reference St John, Edwards-Jones and Jones2010), it continues to be necessary for recent papers (including in high profile journals) to champion and highlight the role that the behavioural sciences, for example, could play in biodiversity conservation (e.g., Maynard et al., Reference Maynard, Monroe, Jacobson and Savage2020; Balmford et al., Reference Balmford, Bradbury, Bauer, Broad, Burgess, Burgman, Byerly, Clayton, Espelosin, Ferraro, Fisherj, Garnett, Jones, Marteau, Otieno, Polasky, Ricketts, Sandbrook, Sullivan-Wiley, Trevelyan, van der Linden, Veríssimo and Nielsen2021; Nielsen et al., Reference Nielsen, Marteau, Bauer, Bradbury, Broad, Burgess, Burgman, Byerly, Clayton, Espelosin, Ferraro, Fisher, Garnett, Jones, Otieno, Polasky, Ricketts, Trevelyan, van der, Veríssimo and Balmford2021).

A range of different viewpoints can help to understand how individual decisions are determined, the negative impacts on biodiversity and ways of reducing these (Clayton et al., Reference Clayton, Litchfield and Geller2013; Amel et al., Reference Amel, Manning, Scott and Koger2017; Reddy et al., Reference Reddy, Montambault, Masuda, Keenan, Butler, Fisher, Stanley and Gneezy2017; Ives et al., Reference Ives, Abson, von Wehrden, Dorninger, Klaniecki and Fischer2018). One is that of personalised ecology, which describes the set of direct interactions that each of us has with nature (Gaston et al., Reference Gaston, Soga, Duffy, Garrett, Gaston and Cox2018; Gaston, Reference Gaston2020; Soga and Gaston, Reference Soga and Gaston2022). Whilst the significance of such interactions, which are likely unique to each person in their composition, has long been recognised (e.g., Wilson, Reference Wilson1984; Kellert and Wilson, Reference Kellert and Wilson1993; Pyle, Reference Pyle1993; Stokes, Reference Stokes2006; Samways, Reference Samways2007), it has particularly come to the fore of recent (Clayton et al., Reference Clayton, Colléony, Conversy, Maclouf, Martin, Torres, Truong and Prévot2017; Soga and Gaston, Reference Soga and Gaston2022). This paper describes why personalised ecology provides a pertinent perspective by exploring the links with, and implications for, the future of biodiversity. Some of the issues discussed (e.g., biophilia, connection to nature, extinction of experience, shifting baselines) have been argued to be amongst the most vital for that future (e.g., Ehrlich and Kennedy, Reference Ehrlich and Kennedy2005; Kareiva, Reference Kareiva2008; Simaika and Samways, Reference Simaika and Samways2010). Given strong biases in the relevant research literature toward studies of culturally westernised societies, our considerations are similarly biased, although many may generalise more widely.

Personalised ecology

In the most fundamental sense, an individual’s personalised ecology describes all of their direct interactions with nature. This includes those with both micro- and macro-organisms. However, a narrower sense conception of personalised ecology, which is of more relevance in the present context, is the direct sensory interactions a person has with nature, predominantly through sight, sound, smell and touch. This is largely with macro-organisms. It is the focus on direct interactions which differentiates personalised ecology from broader considerations of ecosystem services (from which individual people frequently benefit without their necessarily interacting directly with the organisms providing such benefits; Gaston et al., Reference Gaston, Soga, Duffy, Garrett, Gaston and Cox2018).

Personalised ecology is concerned with interactions with nature. There has long been debate as to where the limits to what constitutes nature should lie and definitions can differ markedly, particularly across cultures and disciplines (e.g., Wohlwill, Reference Wohlwill and Wohlwill1983; Proctor, Reference Proctor1998; Wickson, Reference Wickson2008; Bratman et al., Reference Bratman, Hamilton and Daily2012; Hartig et al., Reference Hartig, Mitchell, de Vries and Frumkin2014; CBD, 2022). We use the same definition here as we have employed in other recent studies about human–nature interactions (e.g., Soga and Gaston, Reference Soga and Gaston2020, Reference Soga and Gaston2022), in which nature encompasses individual living organisms through to ecosystems, excluding those that are not self-sustaining. This enables a focus on essentially ‘wild’ organisms.

People’s direct interactions with some species and taxonomic groups have received much attention (e.g., sharks, snakes, bears; Chippaux, Reference Chippaux2017; Bombieri et al., Reference Bombieri, Delgado, Russo, Garrote, López-Bao, Fedriani and Penteriani2018; Gibbs, Reference Gibbs2021), often due to the perceived, potential or realised negative threat that they pose to people. On the whole, however, personalised ecologies remain poorly documented, and have been studied in relatively crude terms, for example, measuring the extent of greenspace in the vicinity of a person’s home or workplace, or the kind, frequency and duration of outdoor visits that they make (e.g., Shanahan et al., Reference Shanahan, Bush, Gaston, Lin, Dean, Barber and Fuller2016; Cox et al., Reference Cox, Plummer, Shanahan, Siriwardena, Fuller, Anderson, Hancock and Gaston2017b; White et al., Reference White, Alcock, Grellier, Wheeler, Hartig, Warber, Bone, Depledge and Fleming2019; Colley et al., Reference Colley, Irvine and Currie2022). Studies of human interactions with other species have almost invariably focused on the numbers and types of species that occur where an individual person lives or visits, rather than considering which ones, and in what numbers, an individual person actually encounters and experiences them (e.g., Fuller et al., Reference Fuller, Irvine, Devine-Wright, Warren and Gaston2007; Dallimer et al., Reference Dallimer, Irvine, Skinner, Davies, Rouquette, Armsworth, Maltby, Warren and Gaston2012; Methorst et al., Reference Methorst, Bonn, Marselle, Böhning-Gaese and Rehdanz2021).

Notwithstanding, it is apparent that personalised ecologies vary greatly amongst individual people, both within and between populations. On average, personalised ecologies will relate to the spatial variation of those components of biodiversity of which people tend to be more aware (e.g., larger organisms). In urbanised societies, and probably more widely, personalised ecologies can be very poor for many people. They are often also highly skewed such that the majority of nature interactions that do occur are experienced by only a small proportion of people (Cox et al., Reference Cox, Hudson, Shanahan, Fuller and Gaston2017a). In general, personalised ecologies are dependent on opportunity (e.g., the local presence and abundance of species), motivation (e.g., emotional affinity with nature) and capability (e.g., ability to see or hear particular species) (Dallimer et al., Reference Dallimer, Davies, Irvine, Maltby, Warren, Gaston and Armsworth2014; Soga and Gaston, Reference Soga and Gaston2022). These are in turn often related to socioeconomic circumstances. We return to these issues in more detail later.

Not only do personalised ecologies vary greatly between people, but an individual’s personalised ecology also varies across multiple time scales (Soga and Gaston, Reference Soga and Gaston2022). It changes through the day (often peaking when people are moving outdoors; Derks et al., Reference Derks, Giessen and Winkel2020), through the week (often peaking at weekends when people engage more in outdoor recreation; Veitch et al., Reference Veitch, Carver, Abbott, Giles-Corti, Timperio and Salmon2015), and through a person’s life course (often peaking both during childhood and during earlier periods of retirement; Hughes et al., Reference Hughes, Rogerson, Barton and Bragg2019). In much of the western world, personalised ecologies, especially those experienced by children, have also become progressively more limited across recent generations, a phenomenon referred to as extinction of experience (Pyle, Reference Pyle1993; Miller, Reference Miller2005; Soga and Gaston, Reference Soga and Gaston2016, Reference Soga and Gaston2023). On the other hand, international, and particularly intercontinental, travel has broadened (though not necessarily deepened) the personalised ecologies of a (typically small) minority of many human populations, allowing people to interact with species and ecosystems that are very different from those they would otherwise encounter. This is reflected most strongly through ecotourism.

Consequences for the future of biodiversity

People’s personalised ecologies have a wide array of consequences for the future of biodiversity. Most attention to positive direct interactions with nature has focused on the wellbeing benefits for people, with evidence of impacts on physical, psychological and social health (e.g., Keniger et al., Reference Keniger, Gaston, Irvine and Fuller2013; Hartig et al., Reference Hartig, Mitchell, de Vries and Frumkin2014; Bratman et al., Reference Bratman, Anderson, Berman, Cochran, Vries, Flanders, Folke, Frumkin, Gross, Hartig, Kahn, Kuo, Lawler, Levin, Lindahl, Meyer-Lindenberg, Mitchell, Ouyang, Roe, Scarlett, Smith, van den, Wheeler, White, Zheng and Daily2019; Marselle et al., Reference Marselle, Hartig, Cox, de, Knapp, Lindley, Triguero-Mas, Böhning-Gaese, Braubach, Cook, Vries, Heintz-Buschart, Hofmann, Irvine, Kabisch, Kolek, Kraemer, Markevych, Martens, Müller, Nieuwenhuijsen, Potts, Stadler, Walton, Warber and Bonn2021; Oh et al., Reference Oh, Zhang, Nghiem, Chang, Tan, Quazi, Shanahan, Lin, Gaston, Fuller and Carrasco2022). This has led to the development of a diversity of interventions to increase these benefits, focusing largely either on changing the environments in which people spend their time, or on changing their behaviour (Shanahan et al., Reference Shanahan, Astell-Burt, Barber, Brymer, Cox, Dean, Depledge, Fuller, Hartig, Irvine, Jones, Kikilus, Lovell, Mitchell, Niemelä, Nieuwenhuijsen, Pretty, Townsend, van Heezik, Warber and Gaston2019). The promotion of people–nature interactions for the purpose of improving human wellbeing does, of course, have the potential to benefit biodiversity directly (especially wild plants and animals living in urban areas), including through the creation and maintenance of accessible greenspaces that enable such interactions. However, there are a variety of other consequences of personalised ecologies, both positive and negative, that may have much greater importance for the future of biodiversity (Figure 1).

Figure 1. A conceptual diagram for understanding the drivers of personalised ecology and its consequences for biodiversity. There is likely a feedback loop in which the consequences of personalised ecology affect its drivers. In the consequences domain of Figure 1, each box contains an up or down arrow that denotes the direction of change in each factor or process caused by increased personalised ecology. For instance, the up arrow in the connection to nature box indicates that direct interactions with nature enhance one’s connection to nature.

(i) Connection to nature

People have a subjective, and perhaps innate, sense of connection with the natural world, sometimes known as biophilia (Wilson, Reference Wilson1984). Such nature connectedness varies dramatically amongst people and societies (Richardson et al., Reference Richardson, Hamlin, Elliott and White2022). It is increasingly seen as a core issue in human–nature relationships (Richardson et al., Reference Richardson, Dobson, Abson, Lumber, Hunt, Young and Moorhouse2020a), and meta-analyses have found that individuals with greater connection to nature have more pro-nature behaviours (Whitburn et al., Reference Whitburn, Linklater and Abrahamse2019; Barragan-Jason et al., Reference Barragan-Jason, de Mazancourt, Parmesan, Singer and Loreau2022). The strength of this connection to nature is thought to be enhanced in individuals with a richer and deeper personalised ecology and, conversely, to be weakened in those whose personalised ecology is poorer (Richardson et al., Reference Richardson, Passmore, Barbett, Lumber, Thomas and Hunt2020b; Mikołajczak et al., Reference Mikołajczak, Lees, Barlow, Sinclair, Trindade de Almeida, Souza and Parry2021; Li et al., Reference Li, Zhai, Chang, Merrill, Browning and Sullivan2022; Lim et al., Reference Lim, Sing, Chong, Jaturas, Dong, Lee, Tao, Le, Bonebrake, TPN, Chu, Brandon-Mong, Kong, Soga and Wilson2022). Indeed, whilst Wilson (Reference Wilson1984) defined biophilia as ‘the innate tendency to focus on life and lifelike processes’, in subsequent writings he emphasised that it is a complex set of learned behaviours, that is, a disposition that is reinforced, amplified and expressed through human culture (Wilson, Reference Kellert and Wilson1993). This is now supported by empirical evidence (Figure 2A; Collado et al., Reference Collado, Staats and Corraliza2013; Vanderstock et al., Reference Vanderstock, Grandi-Nagashiro, Kudo, Latty, Nakamura, White and Soga2022; Wu et al., Reference Wu, Ji and Jin2023). Connection to nature has also been found to mediate the link between personalised ecologies and pro-nature behaviours (Liu et al., Reference Liu, Cleary, Fielding, Murray and Roiko2022).

Figure 2. Empirical evidence suggesting several possible impacts of increased personalised ecology on biodiversity (A: connection to nature; B: pro-nature attitudes/behaviour; C: biophobia; D: disturbance of wildlife; E: ecological literacy; F: altruistic behaviour). Plots show: (A) changes in emotional connection to nature (measured by the Emotional Affinity toward Nature scale) before and after participating in a nature-based recreational program (summer camps) (Collado et al., Reference Collado, Staats and Corraliza2013); (B) differences in the likelihood of engaging in a pro-nature behaviour between birdwatchers and those who do not use nature for recreational purposes (non-recreationist) (Cooper et al., Reference Cooper, Larson, Dayer, Stedman and Decker2015); (C) changes in the proportion of people exhibiting fear of snakes before and after participating in a field trip (Ballouard et al., Reference Ballouard, Provost, Barré and Bonnet2012); (D) differences in the number of bird territories between sites with and without recreational activities (Bötsch et al., Reference Bötsch, Tablado and Jenni2017); (E) differences in species identification ability between people who live in urban versus rural areas (Bashan et al., Reference Bashan, Colléony and Shwartz2021); and (F) changes in the proportion of people exhibiting helping behaviour before and after experience of an urban green park (Guéguen and Stefan, 2014).

(ii) Pro-nature attitudes and behaviours

A key question relating to personalised ecologies is whether they influence a person’s pro-nature attitudes and behaviours, which has recently been termed the nature benefit hypothesis (Soga and Gaston, Reference Soga and Gaston2022). Several studies have documented positive relationships between levels of nature experience and pro-environmental attitudes and behaviours (Figure 2B; e.g., Wells and Lekies, Reference Wells and Lekies2006; Zelenski et al., Reference Zelenski, Dopko and Capaldi2015; Broom, Reference Broom2017; Rosa et al., Reference Rosa, Profice and Collado2018; Dean et al., Reference Dean, Barnett, Wilson and Turrell2019; Alcock et al., Reference Alcock, White, Pahl, Duarte-Davidson and Fleming2020; Martin et al., Reference Martin, White, Hunt, Richardson, Pahl and Burt2020; Liu and Chen, Reference Liu and Chen2021; Ngo et al., Reference Ngo, Hosaka and Numata2022). A smaller number have tested for and documented positive relationships for more explicitly pro-nature (a subset of pro-environmental) attitudes and behaviours (Cooper et al., Reference Cooper, Larson, Dayer, Stedman and Decker2015; Soga et al., Reference Soga, Gaston, Yamaura, Kurisu and Hanaki2016; Prévot et al., Reference Prévot, Cheval, Raymond and Cosquer2018).

(iii) Biophobia

Whilst reduced positive interactions with nature may weaken support for biodiversity conservation, there is an additional concern that these reductions may strengthen antagonism toward such interactions, that is, a negative feedback loop whereby the less that people interact with nature the less they want to do so. This could occur if the loss of positive nature interactions resulted in an increase in wariness or phobia toward nature, that is, biophobia (Ulrich, Reference Ulrich, Kellert and Wilson1993). Indeed, there is evidence that extinction of experience is associated with an increase in biophobia, including due to its associated loss of knowledge about nature (e.g., ability to identify species; Figure 2C; Ballouard et al., Reference Ballouard, Provost, Barré and Bonnet2012; Silva and Minor, Reference Silva and Minor2017; Ngo et al., Reference Ngo, Hosaka and Numata2019; Soga et al., Reference Soga, Evans, Yamanoi, Fukano, Tsuchiya, Koyanagi and Kanai2020; Fukano and Soga, Reference Fukano and Soga2021; Sugiyama et al., Reference Sugiyama, Hosaka, Takagi and Numata2021).

Elevated biophobia can have a wide range of detrimental impacts on the future of biodiversity. Fear emotions impose a significant psychological cost for humans, and thus, increased biophobia can reduce the willingness of local people to coexist with wild animals, particularly, those regarded as dangerous or harmful (e.g., wolves, bears, large cats). Biophobia can therefore decrease public acceptance of certain policies and actions to conserve and restore these organisms (e.g., reintroduction). Biophobia also often results in an increase in persecution of wild organisms, which can negatively impact biodiversity more directly (Pandey et al., Reference Pandey, Subedi Pandey, Devkota and Goode2016; Rocha et al., Reference Rocha, Aziz, Brook, Carvalho, Cooper-Bohannon, Frick, Huang, Kingston, López-Baucells, Maas, Mathews, Medellin, Olival, Peel, Plowright, Razgour, Rebelo, Rodrigues, Rossiter, Russo, Straka, Teeling, Treuer, Voigt and Webala2021). If increasing urbanisation of the human population, and general decline of biodiversity, result in increased biophobia, the impacts on the future of biodiversity could be severe.

(iv) Negative impacts of nature engagement on biodiversity

Increased human–nature interactions may lead to negative impacts on biodiversity in several ways. This includes, for example, disturbance of wildlife during recreational activities (Figure 2D; Larson et al., Reference Larson, Reed, Merenlender and Crooks2016; Bötsch et al., Reference Bötsch, Tablado and Jenni2017), loss of predator avoidance behaviour (Geffroy et al., Reference Geffroy, Samia, Bessa and Blumstein2015), the unintentional transport of organisms between sites (including both native and non-native species) (Hodkinson and Thompson, Reference Hodkinson and Thompson1997), changes to understorey vegetation (Erfanian et al., Reference Erfanian, Alatalo and Ejtehadi2021), increased chemical pollutants (e.g., negative impacts of sunscreen use on coral reefs; Danovaro et al., Reference Danovaro, Bongiorni, Corinaldesi, Giovannelli, Damiani, Astolfi, Grec and Pusceddu2008), increased litter and more frequent fires. Better connection to nature might therefore have negative consequences if it means that more people are going out and disturbing or damaging flora and fauna in sensitive areas.

(v) Two-way interactions

There is evidence of an asymmetry in people’s beliefs, whereby they commonly hold that human impacts on the natural environment are greater than the impacts of the natural environment on people (e.g., Coley et al., Reference Coley, Betz, Helmuth, Ellenbogen, Scyphers and Adams2021). This can weaken the role of self, family or human benefits in support for pro-nature behaviours, and is clearly at odds with the utter dependence of humanity on ecosystem services (IPBES, Reference Brondizio, Settele, Díaz and Ngo2019).

(vi) Shifting baselines

The personalised ecologies that people experience, particularly earlier in life, can have a profound impact on what one regards as ‘normal’ and ‘sound’. Faced with declines in the state of nature, this can result in a progressive ‘ratcheting down’ or shifting of baselines (Pauly, Reference Pauly1995; Soga and Gaston, Reference Soga and Gaston2018). People may, therefore, become more accepting of a much-depleted biodiversity, because the extent of the departure from a natural situation is poorly understood (Jones et al., Reference Jones, Turvey, Massimino and Papworth2020). Shifting baselines can have many negative impacts on biodiversity conservation as they may lead to an increased tolerance for the progressive degradation of ecosystems, changes in people’s expectations as to the state of nature that is worth protecting or restoring, and subsequently the establishment of less ambitious targets and goals for nature conservation (Soga and Gaston, Reference Soga and Gaston2018).

(vii) Ecological literacy

Reduced positive interactions with nature can weaken people’s knowledge about local ecosystems (Figure 2E; Bashan et al., Reference Bashan, Colléony and Shwartz2021). This is often called ecological literacy, or eco-literacy (Pilgrim et al., Reference Pilgrim, Smith and Pretty2007). It includes, for example, identification skills of local fauna and flora (Bashan et al., Reference Bashan, Colléony and Shwartz2021), ethnobotanical knowledge (e.g., traditional use of edible/medicinal plants; Okui et al., Reference Okui, Sawada and Yoshida2021), and traditional management practices of local ecosystems (Tsuchiya et al., Reference Tsuchiya, Aoyagi, Okuro and Takeuchi2014). Maintenance of such knowledge is fundamental for the continued support of local conservation efforts and the capacity of local communities to self-manage natural resources sustainably. Declines in local ecological knowledge can therefore have negative impacts on the conservation of biodiversity.

(viii) Altruistic behaviour

There is evidence that exposure to nature (e.g., viewing greenery) can contribute to enhancing altruism in humans, which has recently been termed the nature and sustainability hypothesis (Soga and Gaston, Reference Soga and Gaston2022). This includes various behaviours, including the reduction of impulsive and selfish decision making and the promotion of sustainable, cooperative and helping behaviour (Figure 2F; Van der Wal et al., Reference Van der Wal, Schade, Krabbendam and Van Vugt2013; Zelenski et al., Reference Zelenski, Dopko and Capaldi2015; Guéguen and Stefan, Reference Guéguen and Stefan2016). Increased altruistic decision making and behaviour can have a wide range of positive outcomes for biodiversity as those actions can contribute, either directly or indirectly, to the conservation and restoration of wild plants and animals.

(ix) Biodiversity data collection

Increased nature interactions may, in some cases, contribute to an increased amount of biodiversity data coming from citizens (Schuttler et al., Reference Schuttler, Sorensen, Jordan, Cooper and Shwartz2018). For example, it has been suggested that increased use of urban greenspaces during the COVID-19 pandemic resulted in increased numbers of wildlife observations submitted to citizen science projects (e.g., Hochachka et al., Reference Hochachka, Alonso, Gutiérrez-Expósito, Miller and Johnston2021). Citizen science data can offer a valuable source of species occurrence records and be used to generate species-level information for broad-scale biodiversity mapping and monitoring.

(x) Normative beliefs

Normative beliefs are the perceptions of what are socially typical or acceptable attitudes and behaviours. For a particular person, both their personalised ecology, and the actual and perceived personalised ecologies of those around them, will shape their normative beliefs. Normative beliefs are often a strong predictor of people’s attitudes and behaviours (Armitage and Conner, Reference Armitage and Conner2010), including those relating to biodiversity (van Riper et al., Reference Van Riper, Browning, Becker, Stewart, Suski, Browning and Golebie2019). Social norms and normative beliefs may influence attitudes and connection to nature (Oh et al., Reference Oh, Fielding, Nghiem, Chang, Carrasco and Fuller2021), or influence behaviour directly in the form of cultural taboos toward the exploitation of particular species, areas and natural resources (Jones et al., Reference Jones, Andriamarovololona and Hockley2008), or more subtly through a person’s propensity to engage in pro-nature behaviours, such as participation in urban greenspace management (Marshall et al., Reference Marshall, Grose and Williams2020).

Acting in combination

Of course, these 10 consequences of personalised ecologies do not act independently, but likely generate a synergistic effect on biodiversity conservation. For example, if people obtain eco-literacy through enhanced personalised ecologies, they may use natural environments in a way that does less harm to those environments (e.g., maintaining appropriate distances from wildlife). Likewise, those with greater species identification ability can provide more accurate and reliable data on biodiversity. Further, increased connection to nature is known to act as a protective factor against biophobia (Zsido et al., Reference Zsido, Coelho and Polák2022). However, there may equally be negative synergies between some of these consequences. For example, shifting baselines are likely to interact with normative beliefs because social norms (perceived or actual) provide a self-sustaining mechanism for maintaining poor personalised ecologies. This may result in a negative, self-reinforcing feedback loop, making it difficult to reverse historic declines in people’s personalised ecologies.

Strengthening personalised ecologies

If better developed personalised ecologies generate positive outcomes for biodiversity conservation, as described above, then strengthening those ecologies may be critically important for the future of biodiversity. People’s personalised ecologies can usefully be regarded as being shaped by three broad sets of factors: capability, opportunity and motivation (as distinguished by the COM-B model; Michie et al., Reference Michie, Van Stralen and West2011). Each of these provides a unique set of opportunities and approaches that could be targeted to strengthen personalised ecologies.

(i) Capability

Capability is an individual’s capacity to engage in interactions with nature. It has two components, physical capability and psychological capability. Physical capability includes the ease with which one can move around and the extent to which one has sufficient sensory abilities to detect particular species, for example, being able to see birds or hear birdsong. Psychological capability includes knowledge, skills, stamina and confidence. The component that has attracted the most attention is skills such as the ability to recognise particular species (Figure 3A; Lindemann-Matthies, Reference Lindemann-Matthies2002).

Figure 3. Empirical evidence demonstrating the role of (A) capability, (B) opportunity and (C) motivation in determining personalised ecology. Plots show: (A) effects of an educational program aimed at increasing children’s species identification ability on the number of animal and plant taxa children noticed on the way to school (Lindemann-Matthies, Reference Lindemann-Matthies2002); (B) association between neighbourhood tree cover and time spent in public greenspace (Shanahan et al., Reference Shanahan, Cox, Fuller, Hancock, Lin, Anderson, Bush and Gaston2017); and (C) differences between park users (based on time spent in parks) in terms of their emotional connection to nature (measured by the Nature Relatedness scale; Lin et al., Reference Lin, Fuller, Bush, Gaston and Shanahan2014).

Arguably, biodiversity conservation has been heavily fixated on improving a rather narrow conception of capability – assuming that education about the nature around you will improve your ability to access it, and willingness to protect it (Thomas-Walters et al., Reference Thomas-Walters, McCallum, Montgomery, Petros, Wan and Veríssimo2023). This is despite various studies finding that education and knowledge, by themselves, are relatively poor predictors of connection to nature (e.g., Barragan-Jason et al., Reference Barragan-Jason, de Mazancourt, Parmesan, Singer and Loreau2022) and pro-nature behaviours (e.g., Knapp et al., Reference Knapp, Phillips, Clements, Shaw and Osborne2021).

Capability might be improved by (i) improving ways for less physically able people to interact with nature (e.g., via views from windows, improved access to greenspaces); (ii) equipment that enables people to overcome or reduce sensory limitations in interacting with nature (e.g., vision and acoustic systems); (iii) accessible tools and learning that help improve psychological capability; and (iv) guides (particularly people rather than signage) who can facilitate and explain nature interactions to visitors to sites.

(ii) Opportunity

Opportunity is all of the factors in a person’s environment that make interactions with nature possible. It has two components, physical opportunity and social opportunity. Physical opportunity includes the availability of nature in a person’s environment with which they can interact (Figure 3B; Shanahan et al., Reference Shanahan, Cox, Fuller, Hancock, Lin, Anderson, Bush and Gaston2017). Social opportunity includes family values, social norms and public safety. Attention has particularly focused on the role of physical opportunity in personalised ecology, and the extent to which people have adequate or appropriate access to nature in their immediate neighbourhood or more widely. Indeed, some organisations have established targets for the availability of local greenspace, such as at least 0.5 ha within 200 m, 2 ha within 300 m and 10 ha within 1 km, all within a 15 min walk from home (Natural England, 2022). Social opportunity, on the other hand, has received much less attention in discussions on how to promote people’s use of nature, except for some particular cases such as children’s use of local nature (Button et al., Reference Button, Tillmann and Gilliland2020). However, recent studies suggest that the influence of social opportunity on personalised ecologies is comparable to – and sometimes stronger than – that of physical opportunity (e.g., Soga et al., Reference Soga, Yamanoi, Tsuchiya, Koyanagi and Kanai2018; Van Truong et al., Reference Van Truong, Nakabayashi and Hosaka2022).

Opportunity might be improved by: (i) improving the availability and accessibility of local nature, including in the vicinity of both home and work places; (ii) improving the ability of nature to move amongst greenspaces, influencing both species’ population sizes and the potential for human–nature interactions; (iii) physically ‘greening’ buildings and their immediate surroundings, through green roofs and walls, gardens, etc.; (iv) improving transport systems to enable better access to nature sites; (v) changing and challenging values, social norms and normative beliefs around nature interactions (e.g., through community engagement, community champions and role models); (vi) improving safety of the local environment (e.g., improving road safety, reducing crime, controlling the abundance of wild animals that can have severe negative health impacts on people); and (vii) providing more dedicated time for nature interactions (e.g., built into work/school schedules).

(iii) Motivation

This is the set of brain processes that energise and direct behaviour. Its two components are: automatic motivation and reflective motivation. Automatic motivations are unconscious responses, such as emotional reactions, whilst reflective motivations are more cognitive and purposeful, such as intentions (Figure 3C; Lin et al., Reference Lin, Fuller, Bush, Gaston and Shanahan2014). In the field of human–nature interactions, the importance of motivation (particularly automatic motivation) in promoting personalised ecologies has long been recognised, and indeed studies show that it is often the most impactful factor in predicting the quantity and quality of those interactions (e.g., Lin et al., Reference Lin, Fuller, Bush, Gaston and Shanahan2014; Soga and Akasaka, Reference Soga and Akasaka2019). Of course, motivation is likely to be improved by enhanced personalised ecologies (see earlier discussion on biophilia), implying that there exists a bidirectional relationship between motivation and personalised ecology.

Motivation might be improved by: (i) green social prescribing, which can provide an incentive for reflective motivation and intentions to interact with nature; (ii) nature-based educational programs in educational institutions (e.g., schools, museums) that can help to increase connection to nature, and therefore automatic motivation; and (iii) nature-oriented television and internet programs (e.g., nature documentaries), and social media that promote people’s desire to experience nature.

Strategies to improve opportunity, motivation and capability do not work independently but are interrelated in many ways. For example, providing nature-based education in schools can help to increase all of the three drivers (capability: ability to notice wildlife; opportunity: ensuring time to interact with nature; motivation: nature connectedness). Improving emotional connection to nature, through recreational and educational programs, is also known to be closely related to enhanced psychological wellbeing (Pirchio et al., Reference Pirchio, Passiatore, Panno, Cipparone and Carrus2021), suggesting that it may help to increase psychological capability.

Conclusions

Many factors shape people’s behavioural decisions, small or large, which collectively determine the future of biodiversity. People’s personalised ecologies are a central factor that may act directly (impacting nature during people’s interactions) or indirectly (influencing mediating factors such as attitudes, nature connectedness and normative beliefs). This raises the potential of a virtuous cycle whereby improving personalised ecologies encourages demand for improved biodiversity, at a time when a high proportion of the global population’s interactions with nature are extremely constrained, and becoming poorer.

Open peer review

To view the open peer review materials for this article, please visit http://doi.org/10.1017/ext.2023.15.

Data availability statement

Data availability is not applicable to this article as no new data were created or analysed in this study.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to three anonymous reviewers for their thoughtful comments.

Author contribution

Conception and design of work: K.J.G., B.B.P. and M.S.; Drafting and revising: K.J.G., B.B.P. and M.S.

Financial support

K.J.G. and B.B.P. were supported by the Natural Environment Research Council funded ‘Renewing biodiversity through a people-in-nature approach (RENEW)’ project (NE/W004941/1). M.S. was supported by the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (Grant Nos. 20H04375 and 23H03583).

Competing interest

The authors declare no competing interest exists.

References

Alcock, I, White, MP, Pahl, S, Duarte-Davidson, R and Fleming, LE (2020) Associations between pro-environmental behaviour and neighbourhood nature, nature visit frequency and nature appreciation: Evidence from a nationally representative survey in England. Environment International 136, 105441.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allan, JR, Possingham, HP, Atkinson, SC, Waldron, A, Di Marco, M, Butchart, SHM, Adams, VM, Kissling, WD, Worsdell, T, Sandbrook, C, Gibbon, G, Kumar, K, Mehta, P, Maron, M, Williams, BA, Jones, KR, Wintle, BA, Reside, AE and Watson, JEM (2022) The minimum land area requiring conservation attention to safeguard biodiversity. Science 376, 10941101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amel, E, Manning, C, Scott, B and Koger, S (2017) Beyond the roots of human inaction: Fostering collective effort toward ecosystem conservation. Science 356, 275279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Armitage, CJ and Conner, M (2010) Efficacy of the theory of planned behaviour: A meta-analytic review. British Journal of Social Psychology 40, 471499.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aycrigg, JL, Mccarley, TR, Belote, RT and Martinuzzi, S (2022) Wilderness areas in a changing landscape: Changes in land use, land cover and climate. Ecological Applications 32, e02471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ballouard, JM, Provost, G, Barré, D and Bonnet, X (2012) Influence of a field trip on the attitude of schoolchildren toward unpopular organisms: An experience with snakes. Journal of Herpetology 46, 423428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balmford, A, Bradbury, RB, Bauer, JM, Broad, S, Burgess, G, Burgman, M, Byerly, H, Clayton, S, Espelosin, D, Ferraro, PJ, Fisherj, B, Garnett, EE, Jones, JPG, Marteau, TM, Otieno, M, Polasky, S, Ricketts, TH, Sandbrook, C, Sullivan-Wiley, K, Trevelyan, R, van der Linden, S, Veríssimo, D and Nielsen, KS (2021) Making more effective use of human behavioural science in conservation interventions. Biological Conservation 261, 109256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barragan-Jason, G, de Mazancourt, C, Parmesan, C, Singer, MC and Loreau, M (2022) Human-nature connectedness as a pathway to sustainability: A global meta-analysis. Conservation Letters 15, e12852.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bashan, D, Colléony, A and Shwartz, A (2021) Urban versus rural? The effects of residential status on species identification skills and connection to nature. People and Nature 3, 347358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolam, FC, Ahumada, J, Akçakaya, HR, Brooks, TM, Elliott, W, Hoban, S, Mair, L, Mallon, D, McGowan, PJ, Raimondo, D, Rodríguez, JP, Roe, D, Seddon, MB, Shen, X, Stuart, SN, Watson, JE and Butchart, SH (2022) Over half of threatened species require targeted recovery actions to avert human‐induced extinction. Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment 21, 6470.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bombieri, G, Delgado, MDM, Russo, LF, Garrote, PJ, López-Bao, JV, Fedriani, JM and Penteriani, V (2018) Patterns of wild carnivore attacks on humans in urban areas. Scientific Reports 8, 19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bötsch, Y, Tablado, Z and Jenni, L (2017) Experimental evidence of human recreational disturbance effects on bird-territory establishment. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 284, 20170846.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bratman, GN, Anderson, CB, Berman, MG, Cochran, B, Vries, S de, Flanders, J, Folke, C, Frumkin, H, Gross, JJ, Hartig, T, Kahn, PH, Kuo, M, Lawler, JJ, Levin, PS, Lindahl, T, Meyer-Lindenberg, A, Mitchell, R, Ouyang, Z, Roe, J, Scarlett, L, Smith, JR, van den, BM, Wheeler, BW, White, MP, Zheng, H and Daily, GC (2019) Nature and mental health: An ecosystem service perspective. Science Advances 5, eaax0903.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bratman, GN, Hamilton, JP and Daily, GC (2012) The impacts of nature experience on human cognitive function and mental health. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1249, 118136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brennan, A, Naidoo, R, Greenstreet, L, Mehrabi, Z, Ramankutty, N and Kremen, C (2022) Functional connectivity of the world’s protected areas. Science 376, 11011104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Broom, C (2017) Exploring the relations between childhood experiences in nature and young adults’ environmental attitudes and behaviours. Australian Journal of Environmental Education 33, 3447.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Button, BL, Tillmann, S and Gilliland, J (2020) Exploring children’s perceptions of barriers and facilitators to physical activity in rural northwestern Ontario, Canada. Rural and Remote Health 20, 5791.Google ScholarPubMed
CBD (2022) Biodiversity and nature, close but not quite the same. Available at https://www.cbd.int/idb/activities/difference-biodiversity-nature.pdf (accessed 22 September 2022).Google Scholar
Chippaux, JP (2017) Incidence and mortality due to snakebite in the Americas. PLoS Neglected Tropical Diseases 11, e0005662.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clayton, S, Colléony, A, Conversy, P, Maclouf, E, Martin, L, Torres, AC, Truong, MX and Prévot, AC (2017) Transformation of experience: Toward a new relationship with nature. Conservation Letters 10, 645651.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clayton, S, Litchfield, C and Geller, ES (2013) Psychological sciences, conservation, and environmental sustainability. Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment 11, 377382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coley, JD, Betz, N, Helmuth, B, Ellenbogen, K, Scyphers, SB and Adams, D (2021) Beliefs about human-nature relationships and implications for investment and stewardship surrounding land-water system conservation. Land 10, 1293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collado, S, Staats, H and Corraliza, JA (2013) Experiencing nature in children’s summer camps: Affective, cognitive and behavioural consequences. Journal of Environmental Psychology 33, 3744.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colley, K, Irvine, KN and Currie, M (2022) Who benefits from nature? A quantitative intersectional perspective on inequalities in contact with nature and the gender gap outdoors. Landscape and Urban Planning 223, 104420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooper, C, Larson, L, Dayer, A, Stedman, R and Decker, D (2015) Are wildlife recreationists conservationists? Linking hunting, birdwatching, and pro‐environmental behavior. Journal of Wildlife Management 79, 446457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cox, DTC, Hudson, HL, Shanahan, DF, Fuller, RA and Gaston, KJ (2017a) The rarity of direct experiences of nature in an urban population. Landscape and Urban Planning 160, 7984.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cox, DTC, Plummer, KE, Shanahan, DF, Siriwardena, GM, Fuller, RA, Anderson, K, Hancock, S and Gaston, KJ (2017b) Doses of neighborhood nature: The benefits for mental health of living with nature. Bioscience 67, 147155.Google Scholar
Dallimer, M, Davies, ZG, Irvine, KN, Maltby, L, Warren, PH, Gaston, KJ and Armsworth, PR (2014) What personal and environmental factors determine frequency of urban greenspace use? International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 11, 79777992.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dallimer, M, Irvine, KN, Skinner, AMJ, Davies, ZG, Rouquette, JR, Armsworth, PR, Maltby, L, Warren, PH and Gaston, KJ (2012) Biodiversity and the feel-good factor: Understanding associations between self-reported human well-being and species richness. Bioscience 62, 4655.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Danovaro, R, Bongiorni, L, Corinaldesi, C, Giovannelli, D, Damiani, E, Astolfi, P, Grec, L and Pusceddu, A (2008) Sunscreens cause coral bleaching by promoting viral infections. Environmental Health Perspectives 116, 441447.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dean, AJ, Barnett, AG, Wilson, KA and Turrell, G (2019) Beyond the ‘extinction of experience’ – Novel pathways between nature experience and support for nature conservation. Global Environmental Change 55, 4857.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Derks, J, Giessen, L and Winkel, G (2020) COVID-19-induced visitor boom reveals the importance of forests as critical infrastructure. Forest Policy and Economics 118, 102253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehrlich, PR and Kennedy, D (2005) Millennium assessment of human behavior. Science 309, 562563.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Erfanian, MB, Alatalo, JM and Ejtehadi, H (2021) Severe vegetation degradation associated with different disturbance types in a poorly managed urban recreation destination in Iran. Scientific Reports 11, 19695.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fukano, Y and Soga, M (2021) Why do so many modern people hate insects? The urbanization-disgust hypothesis. Science of the Total Environment 777, 146229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuller, RA, Irvine, KN, Devine-Wright, P, Warren, PH and Gaston, KJ (2007) Psychological benefits of greenspace increase with biodiversity. Biology Letters 3, 390394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gaston, KJ (2020) Personalised ecology and detection functions. People and Nature 2, 575581.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaston, KJ, Soga, M, Duffy, JP, Garrett, JK, Gaston, S and Cox, DTC (2018) Personalised ecology. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 33, 916925.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geffroy, B, Samia, DSM, Bessa, E and Blumstein, DT (2015) How nature-based tourism might increase prey vulnerability to predators. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 30, 755765.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibbs, L (2021) Agency in human-shark encounter. Environment and Planning E: Nature and Space 4, 645666.Google Scholar
Grace, MK, Akçakaya, HR, Bennett, EL, Brooks, TM, Heath, A, Hedges, S, Hilton‐Taylor, C, Hoffmann, M, Hochkirch, A, Jenkins, R, Keith, DA, Long, B, Mallon, DP, Meijaard, E, Milner‐Gulland, EJ, Rodriguez, JP, Stephenson, PJ, Stuart, SN, Young, RP, Acebes, P, Alfaro‐Shigueto, J, Alvarez‐Clare, S, Andriantsimanarilafy, RR, Arbetman, M, Azat, C, Bacchetta, G, Badola, R, Barcelos, LMD, Barreiros, JP, Basak, S, Berger, DJ, Bhattacharyya, S, Bino, G, Borges, PAV, Boughton, RK, Brockmann, HJ, Buckley, HL, Burfield, IJ, Burton, J, Camacho‐Badani, T, Cano‐Alonso, LS, Carmichael, RH, Carrero, C, Carroll, JP, Catsadorakis, G, Chapple, DG, Chapron, G, Chowdhury, GW, Claassens, L, Cogoni, D, Constantine, R, Craig, CA, Cunningham, AA, Dahal, N, Daltry, JC, Das, GC, Dasgupta, N, Davey, A, Davies, K, Develey, P, Elangovan, V, Fairclough, D, Febbraro, MD, Fenu, G, Fernandes, FM, Fernandez, EP, Finucci, B, Földesi, R, Foley, CM, Ford, M, Forstner, MRJ, García, N, Garcia‐Sandoval, R, Gardner, PC, Garibay‐Orijel, R, Gatan‐Balbas, M, Gauto, I, Ghazi, MGU, Godfrey, SS, Gollock, M, González, BA, Grant, TD, Gray, T, Gregory, AJ, Grunsven, RHA, Gryzenhout, M, Guernsey, NC, Gupta, G, Hagen, C, Hagen, CA, Hall, MB, Hallerman, E, Hare, K, Hart, T, Hartdegen, R, Harvey‐Brown, Y, Hatfield, R, Hawke, T, Hermes, C, Hitchmough, R, Hoffmann, PM, Howarth, C, Hudson, MA, Hussain, SA, Huveneers, C, Jacques, H, Jorgensen, D, Katdare, S, Katsis, LKD, Kaul, R, Kaunda‐Arara, B, Keith‐Diagne, L, Kraus, DT, Lima, TM, Lindeman, K, Linsky, J, Louis, E, Loy, A, Lughadha, EN, Mangel, JC, Marinari, PE, Martin, GM, Martinelli, G, McGowan, PJK, McInnes, A, Mendes, ETB, Millard, MJ, Mirande, C, Money, D, Monks, JM, Morales, CL, Mumu, NN, Negrao, R, Nguyen, AH, MdNH, Niloy, Norbury, GL, Nordmeyer, C, Norris, D, O’Brien, M, Oda, GA, Orsenigo, S, Outerbridge, ME, Pasachnik, S, Pérez‐Jiménez, JC, Pike, C, Pilkington, F, Plumb, G, Portela, R de CQ, Prohaska, A, Quintana, MG, Rakotondrasoa, EF, Ranglack, DH, Rankou, H, Rawat, AP, Reardon, JT, Rheingantz, ML, Richter, SC, Rivers, MC, Rogers, LR, Rosa, P da, Rose, P, Royer, E, Ryan, C, Mitcheson, YJS, Salmon, L, Salvador, CH, Samways, MJ, Sanjuan, T, Santos, AS dos, Sasaki, H, Schutz, E, Scott, HA, Scott, RM, Serena, F, Sharma, SP, Shuey, JA, Silva, CJP, Simaika, JP, Smith, DR, JLY, Spaet, Sultana, S, Talukdar, BK, Tatayah, V, Thomas, P, Tringali, A, Trinh‐Dinh, H, Tuboi, C, Usmani, AA, Vasco‐Palacios, AM, Vié, J, Virens, J, Walker, A, Wallace, B, Waller, LJ, Wang, H, Wearn, OR, Weerd, M, Weigmann, S, Willcox, D, Woinarski, J, Yong, JWH and Young, S (2021) Testing a global standard for quantifying species recovery and assessing conservation impact. Conservation Biology 35, 18331849.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, KM, Crawford, BA, Williamson, KA and DeWan, AA (2019) A meta-analysis of social marketing campaigns to improve global conservation outcomes. Social Marketing Quarterly 25, 6987.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guéguen, N and Stefan, J (2016) “Green altruism” short immersion in natural green environments and helping behavior. Environment and Behavior 48, 324342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hanson, JO, Rhodes, JR, Butchart, SHM, Buchanan, GM, Rondinini, C, Ficetola, GF and Fuller, RA (2020) Global conservation of species’ niches. Nature 580, 232234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hartig, T, Mitchell, R, de Vries, S and Frumkin, H (2014) Nature and health. Annual Review of Public Health 35, 207228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hochachka, WM, Alonso, H, Gutiérrez-Expósito, C, Miller, E and Johnston, A (2021) Regional variation in the impacts of the COVID-19 pandemic on the quantity and quality of data collected by the project eBird. Biological Conservation 254, 108974.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hodkinson, DJ and Thompson, K (1997) Plant dispersal: The role of man. Journal of Applied Ecology 34, 14841496.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, J, Rogerson, M, Barton, J and Bragg, R (2019) Age and connection to nature: When is engagement critical? Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment 17, 265269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
IPBES (2019) Global Assessment Report on Biodiversity and Ecosystem Services of the Intergovernmental Science-Policy Platform on Biodiversity and Ecosystem Services. Brondizio, ES, Settele, J, Díaz, S and Ngo, HT (eds). Bonn: IPBES Secretariat.Google Scholar
Ives, CD, Abson, DJ, von Wehrden, H, Dorninger, C, Klaniecki, K and Fischer, J (2018) Reconnecting with nature for sustainability. Sustainability Science 13, 13891397.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, JPG, Andriamarovololona, MM and Hockley, MM (2008) The importance of taboos and social norms to conservation in Madagascar. Conservation Biology 22, 976986.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, LP, Turvey, ST, Massimino, D and Papworth, SK (2020) Investigating the implications of shifting baseline syndrome on conservation. People and Nature 2, 11311144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kareiva, P (2008) Ominous trends in nature recreation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences U.S.A. 105, 27572758.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kellert, SR and Wilson, EO (eds) (1993) The Biophilia Hypothesis. Washington, DC: Island Press.Google Scholar
Keniger, LE, Gaston, KJ, Irvine, KN and Fuller, RA (2013) What are the benefits of interacting with nature? International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 10, 913935.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kidd, LR, Garrard, GE, Bekessy, SA, Mills, M, Camilleri, AR, Fidler, F, Fielding, KS, Gordon, A, Gregg, EA, Kusmanoff, AM, Louis, W, Moon, K, Robinson, JA, Selinske, MJ, Shanahan, D and Adams, VM (2019) Messaging matters: A systematic review of the conservation messaging literature. Biological Conservation 236, 9299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knapp, JL, Phillips, BB, Clements, J, Shaw, RF and Osborne, JL (2021) Socio‐psychological factors, beyond knowledge, predict people’s engagement in pollinator conservation. People and Nature 3, 204220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Larson, CL, Reed, SE, Merenlender, AM and Crooks, KR (2016) Effects of recreation on animals revealed as widespread through a global systematic review. PLoS One 11, 121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leadley, P, Gonzalez, A, Obura, D, Krug, CB, Londoño-Murcia, MC, Millette, KL, Radulovici, A, Rankovic, A, Shannon, LJ, Archer, E, Armah, FA, Bax, N, Chaudhari, K, Costello, MJ, Dávalos, LM, Roque, F de O, DeClerck, F, Dee, LE, Essl, F, Ferrier, S, Genovesi, P, Guariguata, MR, Hashimoto, S, Speranza, CI, Isbell, F, Kok, M, Lavery, SD, Leclère, D, Loyola, R, Lwasa, S, McGeoch, M, Mori, AS, Nicholson, E, Ochoa, JM, Öllerer, K, Polasky, S, Rondinini, C, Schroer, S, Selomane, O, Shen, X, Strassburg, B, Sumaila, UR, Tittensor, DP, Turak, E, Urbina, L, Vallejos, M, Vázquez-Domínguez, E, Verburg, PH, Visconti, P, Woodley, S and Xu, J (2022) Achieving global biodiversity goals by 2050 requires urgent and integrated actions. One Earth 5, 597603.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, D, Zhai, Y, Chang, P-J, Merrill, J, Browning, MHEM and Sullivan, WC (2022) Nature deficit and senses: Relationships among childhood nature exposure and adulthood sensory profiles, creativity, and nature relatedness. Landscape and Urban Planning 226, 104489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lim, VC, Sing, KW, Chong, KY, Jaturas, N, Dong, H, Lee, PS, Tao, NT, Le, DT, Bonebrake, TC, TPN, Tsang, Chu, L, Brandon-Mong, GJ, Kong, WL, Soga, M and Wilson, JJ (2022) Familiarity with, perceptions of and attitudes toward butterflies of urban park users in megacities across east and Southeast Asia. Royal Society Open Science 9, 220161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, W and Chen, J (2021) Green spaces in Chinese schools enhance children’s environmental attitudes and pro-environmental behavior. Children, Youth and Environments 31, 5587.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lin, BB, Fuller, RA, Bush, R, Gaston, KJ and Shanahan, DF (2014) Opportunity or orientation? Who uses urban parks and why. PLoS One 9, e87422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindemann-Matthies, P (2002) The influence of an educational program on children’s perception of biodiversity. Journal of Environmental Education 33, 2231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, Y, Cleary, A, Fielding, KS, Murray, Z and Roiko, A (2022) Nature connection, pro-environmental behaviours and wellbeing: Understanding the mediating role of nature contact. Landscape and Urban Planning 228, 104550.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacFarlane, D, Hurlstone, MJ, Ecker, UKH, Ferraro, PJ, van der Linden, S, Wan, AKY, Veríssimo, D, Burgess, G, Chen, F, Hall, W, Hollands, GJ and Sutherland, WJ (2022) Reducing demand for overexploited wildlife products: Lessons from systematic reviews from outside conservation science. Conservation Science and Practice 4, e627.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mair, L, Bennun, LA, Brooks, TM, Butchart, SHM, Bolam, FC, Burgess, ND, Ekstrom, JMM, Milner-Gulland, EJ, Hoffmann, M, Ma, K, Macfarlane, NBW, Raimondo, DC, Rodrigues, ASL, Shen, X, Strassburg, BBN, Beatty, CR, Mez-Creutzberg, CG, Iribarrem, A, Irmadhiany, M, Lacerda, E, Mattos, BC, Parakkasi, K, Tognelli, MF, Bennett, EL, Bryan, C, Carbone, G, Chaudhary, A, Eiselin, M, Fonseca, GAB, Galt, R, Geschke, A, Glew, L, Goedicke, R, Green, JMH, Gregory, RD, Hill, SLL, Hole, DG, Hughes, J, Hutton, J, Keijzer, MPW, Navarro, LM, Lughadha, EN, Plumptre, AJ, Puydarrieux, P, Possingham, HP, Rankovic, A, Regan, EC, Rondinini, C, Schneck, JD, Siikamäki, J, Sendashonga, C, Seutin, G, Sinclair, S, Skowno, AL, Soto-Navarro, CA, Stuart, SN, Temple, HJ, Vallier, A, Verones, F, Viana, LR, Watson, J, Bezeng, S, Hm, MB, Burfield, IJ, Clausnitzer, V, Clubbe, C, Cox, NA, Freyhof, J, Gerber, LR, Hilton-Taylor, C, Jenkins, R, Joolia, A, Joppa, LN, Koh, LP, Lacher, TE, Langhammer, PF, Long, B, Mallon, D, Pacifici, M, Polidoro, BA, Pollock, CM, Rivers, MC, Roach, NS, Rodríguez, JP, Smart, J, Young, BE, Hawkins, F and McGowan, PJK (2021) A metric for spatially explicit contributions to science-based species targets. Nature Ecology and Evolution 5, 836844.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marselle, MR, Hartig, T, Cox, DTC, de, Bell S, Knapp, S, Lindley, S, Triguero-Mas, M, Böhning-Gaese, K, Braubach, M, Cook, PA, Vries, S de, Heintz-Buschart, A, Hofmann, M, Irvine, KN, Kabisch, N, Kolek, F, Kraemer, R, Markevych, I, Martens, D, Müller, R, Nieuwenhuijsen, M, Potts, JM, Stadler, J, Walton, S, Warber, SL and Bonn, A (2021) Pathways linking biodiversity to human health: A conceptual framework. Environment International 150, 106420.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshall, AJ, Grose, MJ and Williams, NSG (2020) Of mowers and growers: Perceived social norms strongly influence verge gardening, a distinctive civic greening practice. Landscape and Urban Planning 198, 103795.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, L, White, MP, Hunt, A, Richardson, M, Pahl, S and Burt, J (2020) Nature contact, nature connectedness and associations with health, wellbeing and pro-environmental behaviours. Journal of Environmental Psychology 68, 101389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mastrangelo, ME, Gavin, MC, Laterra, P, Linklater, WL and Milfont, TL (2014) Psycho‐social factors influencing forest conservation intentions on the agricultural frontier. Conservation Letters 7, 103110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maynard, L, Monroe, MC, Jacobson, SK and Savage, A (2020) Maximizing biodiversity conservation through behavior change strategies. Conservation Science and Practice 2, e193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Methorst, J, Bonn, A, Marselle, M, Böhning-Gaese, K and Rehdanz, K (2021) Species richness is positively related to mental health–A study for Germany. Landscape and Urban Planning 211, 104084.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Michie, S, Van Stralen, MM and West, R (2011) The behaviour change wheel: A new method for characterizing and designing behaviour change interventions. Implementation Science 6, 42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, JR (2005) Biodiversity conservation and the extinction of experience. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 20, 430434.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mikołajczak, K, Lees, AC, Barlow, J, Sinclair, F, Trindade de Almeida, O, Souza, AC and Parry, L (2021) Who knows, who cares? Untangling ecological knowledge and nature connection among Amazonian colonist farmers. People and Nature 3, 431445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Natural England (2022) How Natural England’s Green Infrastructure Framework can help create better places to live. Available at https://naturalengland.blog.gov.uk/2021/12/07/how-natural-englands-green-infrastructure-framework-can-help-create-better-places-to-live/ (accessed 4 November 2022).Google Scholar
Nelson, KM, Partelow, S and Schlüter, A (2019) Nudging tourists to donate for conservation: Experimental evidence on soliciting voluntary contributions for coastal management. Journal of Environmental Management 237, 3043.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ngo, KM, Hosaka, T and Numata, S (2019) The influence of childhood nature experience on attitudes and tolerance towards problem-causing animals in Singapore. Urban Forestry and Urban Greening 41, 150157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ngo, KM, Hosaka, T and Numata, S (2022) Attitudes and preferences of wildlife and their relationship with childhood nature experience amongst residents in a tropical urban city. Urban Ecosystem 25, 19391948.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nielsen, KS, Marteau, TM, Bauer, JM, Bradbury, RB, Broad, S, Burgess, G, Burgman, M, Byerly, H, Clayton, S, Espelosin, D, Ferraro, PJ, Fisher, B, Garnett, EE, Jones, JPG, Otieno, M, Polasky, S, Ricketts, TH, Trevelyan, R, van der, Linden S, Veríssimo, D and Balmford, A (2021) Biodiversity conservation as a promising frontier for behavioural science. Nature Human Behaviour 5, 550556.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Obura, DO, Katerere, Y, Mayet, M, Kaelo, D, Msweli, S, Mather, K, Harris, J, Louis, M, Kramer, R, Teferi, T, Samoilys, M, Lewis, L, Bennie, A, Kumah, F, Isaacs, M and Nantongo, P (2021) Integrate biodiversity targets from local to global levels. Science 373, 746748.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oh, RRY, Fielding, KS, Nghiem, LTP, Chang, CC, Carrasco, LR and Fuller, RA (2021) Connection to nature is predicted by family values, social norms and personal experiences of nature. Global Ecology and Conservation 28, e01632.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oh, RRY, Zhang, Y, Nghiem, LTP, Chang, C-C, Tan, CLY, Quazi, SA, Shanahan, DF, Lin, BB, Gaston, KJ, Fuller, RA and Carrasco, RL (2022) Connection to nature and time spent in gardens predicts social cohesion. Urban Forestry and Urban Greening 74, 127655.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Okui, K, Sawada, Y and Yoshida, T (2021) “Wisdom of the elders” or “loss of experience” as a mechanism to explain the decline in traditional ecological knowledge: A case study on Awaji Island, Japan. Human Ecology 49, 353362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pandey, DP, Subedi Pandey, G, Devkota, K and Goode, M (2016) Public perceptions of snakes and snakebite management: Implications for conservation and human health in southern Nepal. Journal of Ethnobiology and Ethnomedicine 12, 125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pauly, D (1995) Anecdotes and the shifting baseline syndrome of fisheries. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 10, 430.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pérez‐Hämmerle, K, Moon, K, Venegas‐Li, R, Maxwell, S, Simmonds, JS, Venter, O, Garnett, ST, Possingham, HP and Watson, JEM (2022) Wilderness forms and their implications for global environmental policy and conservation. Conservation Biology 36, e13875.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pilgrim, S, Smith, D and Pretty, J (2007) A cross-regional assessment of the factors affecting ecoliteracy: Implications for policy and practice. Ecological Applications 17, 17421751.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pirchio, S, Passiatore, Y, Panno, A, Cipparone, M and Carrus, G (2021) The effects of contact with nature during outdoor environmental education on students’ wellbeing connectedness to nature and pro-sociality. Frontiers in Psychology 12, 648458.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prévot, AC, Cheval, H, Raymond, R and Cosquer, A (2018) Routine experiences of nature in cities can increase personal commitment toward biodiversity conservation. Biological Conservation 226, 18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Proctor, JD (1998) The social construction of nature: Relativist accusations, pragmatist and critical realist responses. Annals of the Association of American Geographers 88, 352376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pyle, RM (1993) The Thunder Tree: Lessons from an Urban Wildland. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Reddy, SM, Montambault, J, Masuda, YJ, Keenan, E, Butler, W, Fisher, JR, Stanley, TA and Gneezy, A (2017) Advancing conservation by understanding and influencing human behavior. Conservation Letters 10, 248256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richardson, M, Dobson, J, Abson, DJ, Lumber, R, Hunt, A, Young, R and Moorhouse, B (2020a) Applying the pathways to nature connectedness at a societal scale: A leverage points perspective. Ecosystems and People 16, 387401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richardson, M, Hamlin, I, Elliott, LR and White, MP (2022) Country-level factors in a failing relationship with nature: Nature connectedness as a key metric for a sustainable future. Ambio 51, 22012213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richardson, M, Passmore, H-A, Barbett, L, Lumber, R, Thomas, R and Hunt, A (2020b) The green care code: How nature connectedness and simple activities help explain pro-nature conservation behaviours. People and Nature 2, 821839.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rocha, R, Aziz, SA, Brook, CE, Carvalho, WD, Cooper-Bohannon, R, Frick, WF, Huang, JC-C, Kingston, T, López-Baucells, A, Maas, B, Mathews, F, Medellin, RA, Olival, KJ, Peel, AJ, Plowright, RK, Razgour, O, Rebelo, H, Rodrigues, L, Rossiter, SJ, Russo, D, Straka, TM, Teeling, EC, Treuer, T, Voigt, CC and Webala, PW (2021) Bat conservation and zoonotic disease risk: A research agenda to prevent misguided persecution in the aftermath of COVID-19. Animal Conservation 24, 303307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosa, CD, Profice, CC and Collado, S (2018) Nature experiences and adults’ self-reported pro-environmental behaviors: The role of connectedness to nature and childhood nature experiences. Frontiers in Psychology 9, 1055.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Samways, MJ (2007) Rescuing the extinction of experience. Biodiversity and Conservation 16, 19951997.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schuttler, SG, Sorensen, AE, Jordan, RC, Cooper, C and Shwartz, A (2018) Bridging the nature gap: Can citizen science reverse the extinction of experience? Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment 16, 405411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shanahan, DF, Astell-Burt, T, Barber, EA, Brymer, E, Cox, DTC, Dean, J, Depledge, M, Fuller, RA, Hartig, T, Irvine, KN, Jones, A, Kikilus, H, Lovell, R, Mitchell, R, Niemelä, J, Nieuwenhuijsen, M, Pretty, J, Townsend, M, van Heezik, Y, Warber, S and Gaston, KJ (2019) Nature-based interventions for improving health and wellbeing: The purpose, the people and the outcomes. Sports 7, 141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shanahan, DF, Bush, R, Gaston, KJ, Lin, BB, Dean, J, Barber, E and Fuller, RA (2016) Health benefits from nature experiences depend on dose. Scientific Reports 6, 28551.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shanahan, DF, Cox, DTC, Fuller, RA, Hancock, S, Lin, BB, Anderson, K, Bush, R and Gaston, KJ (2017) Variation in experiences of nature across gradients of tree cover in compact and sprawling cities. Landscape and Urban Planning 157, 231238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silk, M, Correia, R, Veríssimo, D, Verma, A and Crowley, SL (2021) The implications of digital visual media for human-nature relationships. People and Nature 3, 11301137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silva, A and Minor, ES (2017) Adolescents’ experience and knowledge of, and attitudes toward, bees: Implications and recommendations for conservation. Anthrozoös 30, 1932.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simaika, JP and Samways, MJ (2010) Biophilia as a universal ethic for conserving biodiversity. Conservation Biology 24, 903906.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soga, M and Akasaka, M (2019) Multiple landscape-management and social-policy approaches are essential to mitigate the extinction of experience. Landscape and Urban Planning 191, 103634.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Soga, M, Evans, MJ, Yamanoi, T, Fukano, Y, Tsuchiya, K, Koyanagi, TF and Kanai, T (2020) How can we mitigate against increasing biophobia among children during the extinction of experience? Biological Conservation 242, 108420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Soga, M and Gaston, KJ (2016) Extinction of experience: The loss of human-nature interactions. Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment 14, 94101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Soga, M and Gaston, KJ (2018) Shifting baseline syndrome: Causes, consequences, and implications. Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment 16, 222230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Soga, M and Gaston, KJ (2020) The ecology of human–nature interactions. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 287, 20191882.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soga, M and Gaston, KJ (2022) Towards a unified understanding of human-nature interactions. Nature Sustainability 5, 374383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Soga, M and Gaston, KJ (2023) Global synthesis reveals heterogeneous changes in connection of humans to nature. One Earth 6, 131138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Soga, M, Gaston, KJ, Yamaura, Y, Kurisu, K and Hanaki, K (2016) Both direct and vicarious experiences of nature affect children’s willingness to conserve biodiversity. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 13, 529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soga, M, Yamanoi, T, Tsuchiya, K, Koyanagi, TF and Kanai, T (2018) What are the drivers of and barriers to children’s direct experiences of nature? Landscape and Urban Planning 180, 114120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
St John, FAV, Edwards-Jones, G and Jones, JPG (2010) Conservation and human behaviour: Lessons from social psychology. Wildlife Research 37, 658667.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stokes, DL (2006) Conservators of experience. Bioscience 56, 67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sugiyama, N, Hosaka, T, Takagi, E and Numata, S (2021) How do childhood nature experiences and negative emotions towards nature influence preferences for outdoor activity among young adults? Landscape and Urban Planning 205, 103971.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas-Walters, L, McCallum, J, Montgomery, R, Petros, C, Wan, AKY and Veríssimo, D (2023) Systematic review of conservation interventions to promote voluntary behavior change. Conservation Biology 37, e14000.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tsuchiya, K, Aoyagi, M, Okuro, T and Takeuchi, K (2014) The potential of, and threat to, the transfer of ecological knowledge in urban areas: The case of community-based woodland management in Tokyo, Japan. Ecology and Society 19, 25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ulrich, RS (1993) Biophilia, biophobia, and natural landscapes. In Kellert, SR Wilson, EO (eds), The Biophilia Hypothesis. Washington, DC: Island Press, pp. 73137.Google Scholar
Van der Wal, AJ, Schade, HM, Krabbendam, L and Van Vugt, M (2013) Do natural landscapes reduce future discounting in humans? Proceedings of the Royal Society B 280, 20132295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vanderstock, A, Grandi-Nagashiro, C, Kudo, G, Latty, T, Nakamura, S, White, TE and Soga, M (2022) For the love of insects: Gardening grows positive emotions (biophilia) towards invertebrates. Journal of Insect Conservation 26, 751762.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Riper, CJ, Browning, MHEM, Becker, D, Stewart, W, Suski, CD, Browning, L and Golebie, E (2019) Human-nature relationships and normative beliefs influence behaviors that reduce the spread of aquatic invasive species. Environmental Management 63, 6979.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Truong, M, Nakabayashi, M and Hosaka, T (2022) How to encourage parents to let children play in nature: Factors affecting parental perception of children’s nature play. Urban Forestry and Urban Greening 69, 127497.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veitch, J, Carver, A, Abbott, G, Giles-Corti, B, Timperio, A and Salmon, J (2015) How active are people in metropolitan parks? An observational study of park visitation in Australia. BMC Public Health 15, 18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ward, M, Saura, S, Williams, B, Ramírez-Delgado, JP, Arafeh-Dalmau, N, Allan, JR, Venter, O, Dubois, G and Watson, JEM (2020) Just ten percent of the global terrestrial protected area network is structurally connected via intact land. Nature Communications 11, 4563.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, JEM, Keith, DA, Strassburg, BBN, Venter, O, Williams, B and Nicholson, E (2020) Set a global target for ecosystems. Nature 578, 360362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wauchope, HS, Jones, JPG, Geldmann, J, Simmons, BI, Amano, T, Blanco, DE, Fuller, RA, Johnston, A, Langendoen, T, Mundkur, T, Nagy, S and Sutherland, WJ (2022) Protected areas have a mixed impact on waterbirds, but management helps. Nature 605, 103107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wells, NM and Lekies, KS (2006) Nature and the life course: Pathways from childhood nature experiences to adult environmentalism. Children Youth and Environments 16, 124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitburn, J, Linklater, W and Abrahamse, W (2019) Meta-analysis of human connection to nature and proenvironmental behavior. Conservation Biology 34, 180193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, MP, Alcock, I, Grellier, J, Wheeler, BW, Hartig, T, Warber, SL, Bone, A, Depledge, MH and Fleming, LE (2019) Spending at least 120 minutes a week in nature is associated with good health and wellbeing. Scientific Reports 9, 7730.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wickson, F (2008) What is nature, if it’s more than just a place without people? Nature 456, 29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, EO (1984) Biophilia. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wohlwill, JF (1983) The concept of nature. In Wohlwill, JF (ed.), Behavior and the Natural Environment. Boston, MA: Springer, pp. 537.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wu, H, Ji, R and Jin, H (2023) Parental factors affecting children’s nature connectedness. Journal of Environmental Psychology 87, 101977.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zelenski, JM, Dopko, RL and Capaldi, CA (2015) Cooperation is in our nature: Nature exposure may promote cooperative and environmentally sustainable behavior. Journal of Environmental Psychology 42, 2431.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zsido, AN, Coelho, CM and Polák, J (2022) Nature relatedness: A protective factor for snake and spider fears and phobias. People and Nature 4, 669682.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. A conceptual diagram for understanding the drivers of personalised ecology and its consequences for biodiversity. There is likely a feedback loop in which the consequences of personalised ecology affect its drivers. In the consequences domain of Figure 1, each box contains an up or down arrow that denotes the direction of change in each factor or process caused by increased personalised ecology. For instance, the up arrow in the connection to nature box indicates that direct interactions with nature enhance one’s connection to nature.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Empirical evidence suggesting several possible impacts of increased personalised ecology on biodiversity (A: connection to nature; B: pro-nature attitudes/behaviour; C: biophobia; D: disturbance of wildlife; E: ecological literacy; F: altruistic behaviour). Plots show: (A) changes in emotional connection to nature (measured by the Emotional Affinity toward Nature scale) before and after participating in a nature-based recreational program (summer camps) (Collado et al., 2013); (B) differences in the likelihood of engaging in a pro-nature behaviour between birdwatchers and those who do not use nature for recreational purposes (non-recreationist) (Cooper et al., 2015); (C) changes in the proportion of people exhibiting fear of snakes before and after participating in a field trip (Ballouard et al., 2012); (D) differences in the number of bird territories between sites with and without recreational activities (Bötsch et al., 2017); (E) differences in species identification ability between people who live in urban versus rural areas (Bashan et al., 2021); and (F) changes in the proportion of people exhibiting helping behaviour before and after experience of an urban green park (Guéguen and Stefan, 2014).

Figure 2

Figure 3. Empirical evidence demonstrating the role of (A) capability, (B) opportunity and (C) motivation in determining personalised ecology. Plots show: (A) effects of an educational program aimed at increasing children’s species identification ability on the number of animal and plant taxa children noticed on the way to school (Lindemann-Matthies, 2002); (B) association between neighbourhood tree cover and time spent in public greenspace (Shanahan et al., 2017); and (C) differences between park users (based on time spent in parks) in terms of their emotional connection to nature (measured by the Nature Relatedness scale; Lin et al., 2014).

Author comment: Personalised ecology and the future of biodiversity — R0/PR1

Comments

Please find herewith our invited contribution to ‘Extinction’, addressing as agreed the relations between personalised ecology and the future of biodiversity. We trust that you will find that this is appropriate. Please do not hesitate to contact us if you have any queries.

Review: Personalised ecology and the future of biodiversity — R0/PR2

Conflict of interest statement

Reviewer declares none.

Comments

This is excellent: the best manuscript I have read for a long time. Thank you!

Review: Personalised ecology and the future of biodiversity — R0/PR3

Conflict of interest statement

Reviewer declares none.

Comments

I’ve found the paper an interesting read and overall I only have very minor comments for improvement. However, I have to disclose that while I have experience in conservation research, I’m not familiar with the literature of personalised ecology and in general behavioural science applied to conservation, so I’m unable to judge the originality and correctness of the content. Nonetheless, below you will find my suggestions.

The document did not have line numbers, so I indicated the page and the whole sentence where needed.

page 4: “In general, personalised ecologies are dependent on opportunity (e.g., the local presence and abundance of species), motivation (e.g., emotional affinity with nature) and capability (e.g., ability to see or hear particular species) (Dallimer et al., 2014; Soga and Gaston, 2022”.

And previous experiences/Instruction? Both motivation and capabilities ultimely depend in large part on how a person has been raised. These interests can be shaped by family, friends, local culture, etc.

page 4: “a phenomenon known as extinction of experience”. I’d not use “known” since this was used in a few papers, perhaps “named”, “labeled” or “referred to”.

page 4: sentence starting with “On the other hand, international...” specify this relates to a minority of humanity (wealthy people that are already much attracted by nature, i.e. a minority of a minority).

“(ix) Biodiversity monitoring” I suggest changing this to “citizen science”, which clearly can be a form of biodiversity monitoring in some contexts (with many “if”s).

Page 8: “assuming that education about the nature around you will improve your ability to access it.” your ability or your interest? I guess this has to do with the definition of capabilities and my previous comment on education. But see next comment.

Page 8: “This is despite various studies finding that education and knowledge are poor predictors of connection to nature (e.g., Barragan-Jason et al., 2022) and pro-nature behaviours (e.g., Knapp et al., 2021).” This seems to contradict the statement in page 5 “...in subsequent writings he emphasized that it is a complex set of learned behaviours, i.e. a disposition that is reinforced, amplified and expressed through human culture (Wilson, 1993). This is now supported by empirical evidence (Figure 2A; Collado et al., 2013; Vanderstock et al., 2022).” It requires a comment from the authors.

Page 9: “Psychological capabilities” the term is not adequate in my opinion, it seem to refer to very different skills (e.g. self confidence or similar). I checked and the cited reference (Lindemann-Matthies) did not use it. I’d suggest something like “wildlife identification skills”, or more general, e.g. “nature-related skills”.

Page 10: “Motivation can be improved...” you may also consider social media and influencers in moderns times.

Fig. 1. Can you highlight the negative and positive consequences (e.g. different colours, position, etc..). I’d also change “Biodiversity monitoring” to “Citizen science”

Fig. 2. The figure requires going back and forth between each panel and the legend to be interpreted. It would be better if you could make it more intuitive at a first sight. e.g. in D i suggest replacing “absent/present” with “with recreational activities” “without recreational activites”. In F you may specify somehow that before/after refers to experience in urban green park. etc.

You could do this by adding a title. e.g. in A “Participation in a nature-based recreational program”

This is not much different than what you did in Fig. 3C for example.

Review: Personalised ecology and the future of biodiversity — R0/PR4

Conflict of interest statement

Reviewer declares none.

Comments

This manuscript deals with the relationship people have with nature and how this relationship impacts on the future of global biodiversity. Somewhat sadly, this topic is a great fit to the focus of this journal. The manuscript is well written and topical, building upon a number of recent publications on the same issue by the submitting authors (taking the conceptual framework from Soga & Gaston 2022) and looking more specifically at the consequences for biodiversity.

The primary issue I have with the manuscript in its current form is the overly western-centric framing on a topic that I would like to think has far broader relevance to non-western cultures. Granted, much of this centricity is implicit in the text, but I see value in considering a more explicitly diverse approach. At a number of points throughout the manuscript, this limitation is touched on or wider diversity is recognised. For example “nature connectedness varies dramatically amongst people and societies” (p 5) and “traditional management practices of local ecosystems” (p 7). Interestingly, in Soga & Gaston (2022), this framing was recognised as a major caveat to the underlying analysis: “…human-nature interactions can vary substantially across societies with different cultural backgrounds…”.

My point is that I think this manuscript would make a far more valuable contribution to the literature if it was to give greater consideration to how other non-western cultures fit within the outlined concept (i.e. that summarised in Fig. 1) and, in turn, how their different perspectives influence consequences for biodiversity. The reason I see this change as a priority is that much of the global biodiversity that is imperilled has a future that is strongly influenced by the personalised ecology of non-western communities. In particular, I see merit in giving greater consideration to the role of culture, particularly Indigenous culture, in refining the framing. For example, on one hand culture is likely to be a significant driver of personalised ecology for some communities in a way that doesn’t fit neatly into the existing boxes of the COM-B system. Equally, culture is inter-woven through many of the consequences of personalised ecology in a way possibly implied by the ‘feedback loops’ mentioned in the caption for Fig. 1.

A secondary issue is one of clarity – what is meant exactly by biodiversity, and how does this relate to non-native species? Again, implicitly, biodiversity appears to be used to refer to native biological diversity. However, some of the examples given, particularly in regard to ‘nature’ and ‘greenspaces’, is non-specific in regard to native status. Given that there is a well-established link between species introductions into urban areas (often for horticulture and as pets) becoming threatening non-native escapees, is this a missed opportunity to more explicitly recognise that there are possible downsides to these aspects of personalised ecology? This link might fit nicely with strategies to mitigate such risks that are already discussed, such as negative aspects of nature engagement, shifting baselines and ecological literacy.

Minor issues (noting no line numbering provided):

P 6, para 3: please change ‘invasive’ to ‘non-native’ or ‘alien’; the term invasive is frequently misunderstood/misused and it is more commonly accepted in the invasion literature to refer to a rapid increase in distribution, rather than non-native status.

P 6, para 3: isn’t the potential disturbance equally relevant to plants as it is to animals? If so, I suggest changing ‘wildlife’ to ‘flora and fauna’ (or something equivalent) in the last sentence.

P 8, para 3: the statement “biodiversity conservation has been heavily fixated… …improve your ability to access it” is a bold claim, and one that I think either needs stronger support from the literature via citation, or a more nuanced explanation as to why it is a sound assertion.

P 10, para 4: is ‘demand for greater biodiversity’ the most appropriate goal to list here? In the strict sense of the term, communities with more numerous species are not necessarily what we should be striving for if biodiversity conservation (i.e. determining the future of biodiversity) is the goal.

Figure 1: I suspect there is a more elegant way of presenting the concept than the structure currently presented. I would encourage the authors to think outside the box to try and include a version that incorporates the mentioned feedback loop and considers my earlier feedback on the matter of culture.

Figure 1: would it be clearer to say ‘drivers’ rather than ‘causes’ in the second sentence?

Soga, M., Gaston, K.J. Towards a unified understanding of human–nature interactions. Nat Sustain 5, 374–383 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41893-021-00818-z

Recommendation: Personalised ecology and the future of biodiversity — R0/PR5

Comments

We have now received three reviews for this submission. All found the topic interesting and important. However, two reviewers raised some issues with the current version of the manuscript. In particular, please address comments on the need to give greater consideration to how non-western cultures fit within your proposed concept. And how they can offer different perspectives to affect biodiversity conservation. As such, I would like to invite the authors to revise their manuscript.

Decision: Personalised ecology and the future of biodiversity — R0/PR6

Comments

No accompanying comment.

Author comment: Personalised ecology and the future of biodiversity — R1/PR7

Comments

No accompanying comment.

Review: Personalised ecology and the future of biodiversity — R1/PR8

Conflict of interest statement

Reviewer declares none.

Comments

This is excellent. Thank you!

Review: Personalised ecology and the future of biodiversity — R1/PR9

Conflict of interest statement

Reviewer declares none.

Comments

I’m satisfied with the response given and the revised version of the paper, I can now recommend publication.

Recommendation: Personalised ecology and the future of biodiversity — R1/PR10

Comments

The authors' responses to the reviewers have been satisfactory.

Decision: Personalised ecology and the future of biodiversity — R1/PR11

Comments

No accompanying comment.