Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-wzw2p Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-14T20:42:29.740Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

In Search of Animal Models for Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 November 2014

Abstract

Animal models of obsessive-compulsive disorder may be derived naturally or generated experimentally. Either may be considered a full or partial model, depending on the extent of resemblance to human OCD. Although many models appear to be valid at first glance, complete models should demonstrate “compelling similarities” in all areas examined. Partial models may have only one or two OCD-like features; however, they still provide some opportunities for research. Criteria to consider when evaluating potential models are etiology, symptomatology, and use in indicating methods of therapy and prevention. Demonstration of common underlying physiological mechanisms, patterns of development and expression, and familial associations are some specific elements to consider when assessing the relevance of more complete models.

Cognitive aspects of OCD are difficult, if not impossible, to explore in animals at present and may be quite different from what is found in humans. It is hard to imagine that animals experience ego-dystonicity; however, dogs with acral lick dermatitis (ALD) that have been scolded by their owners will resort to licking themselves when out of sight, implying that they know it is wrong. Because of the difficulty in evaluating an animal's internal state, some adopt the position that there is no complete animal model of OCD and that animal models are at best partial models. However, children who exhibit compulsive behaviors frequently do not experience accompanying obsessions. Even the expression of OCD in children—for example, licking and walking in geometric designs—resembles what is seen in animals.

In the descriptions that follow, only directly comparable aspects of compulsive behavior in animals will be discussed. Naturally occurring models that appear to correspond most closely to full models may be useful in understanding the fundamental mechanisms underlying OCD and the OC spectrum, including genetics and neurobiology. Experimental models, which may be either full or partial models, could contribute similarly or provide limited but specific information in a targeted area of study, depending on the extent of their similarity to OCD.

Type
Feature Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1996

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

1.Mineka, S. Animal models of obsessive compulsive disorder. International Workshop on Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder, March 29–31, 1993; Vail, CO.Google Scholar
2.Seligman, MEP. Depression and learned helplessness. In: Friedman, RJ, Katz, MM, eds. The Psychology of Depression: Contemporary Theory and Research. Washington, DC: Winston Wiley; 1974:83113.Google Scholar
3.Seligman, MEP. Learned helplessness in the rat. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1975;88:534541.Google Scholar
4.Rapoport, JL, Leonard, HL, Swedo, SE, Lenane, MC. Obsessive compulsive disorder in children and adolescents: issues in management. J Clin Psychiatry. 1993;54(6):2730.Google ScholarPubMed
5.Rettew, DC, Swedo, SE, Leonard, HL, et al. , Obsessions and compulsions across time in 79 children and adolescents with obsessive compulsive disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1992;31(6):10501056.Google Scholar
6.Luescher, UA, McKeown, DB, Halip, J. Stereotypic or obsessive-compulsive disorders in dogs and cats. Vet Clin North Am. 1991:21:401413.Google Scholar
7.Dodman, NH, Moon-Fanelli, A, Mertens, PA, Pflueger, S, Stein, D. Veterinary models of obsessive compulsive disorder. In: Hollander, E, Stein, D, eds. Obsessive Compulsive Disorder. New York, NY: Marcel Dekker. In press.Google Scholar
8.Franzblau, SH, Kanadian, M, Rettig, E. Critique of reductionistic models of obsessive compulsive disorder: toward a new explanatory paradigm. Soc Sci Med. 1995:41:99112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
9.Rapoport, JL. Obsessive-compulsive disorder: new models. Psychopharmacol Bull. 1988;24(3):365369.Google Scholar
10.Stein, DJ, Shoulberg, N, Helton, K, Hollander, E. The neuroethological approach to obsessive-compulsive disorder. Comp Psychiatry. 1992;33(4):274281.Google Scholar
11.Archer, J. The history and aims of aggression research. In: Barlow, G, Bateson, PPG, Hoppenheim, RW, eds. The Behavioural Biology of Aggression. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press; 1988:117.Google Scholar
12.Powers, WT. Feedback: beyond behaviorism. Science. 1973:179:351356.Google Scholar
13.Mertens, P, Dodman, NH. Use of Prozac to treat feather picking in birds. Tufts University, Grafton, MA. Data on file.Google Scholar
14.Inglauer, F, Rasim, R. Treatment of psychogenic feather picking in psittacine birds with a dopamine antagonist. J Small Anim Pract. 1993;34(11):564566.Google Scholar
15.Lennox, AM, Van der Heyden, N. Haloperidol for the use in treatment of psittacine self-mutilation and feather picking. Proceedings of the Annual Conference of the Association of Avian Veterinarians; August 31-September 4, 1993; Nashville, TN;p3.Google Scholar
16.McDougle, CJ, Goodman, Wk, Leckman, JF, Price, LH. The psychopharmacology of obsessive compulsive disorder. Psychialr CM North Am. 1993;16(4):749766.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17.Stein, DJ, Hollander, E. Low-dose pimozide augmentation of serotonin reuptake blockers in the treatment of trichotillomania. J Clin Psychiatry. 1992;53(4):123126.Google Scholar
18.Houpt, KA. Communication, In: Domestic Animal Behavior for Veterinarians and Animal Scientists. Ames, IA: Iowa State Press; 1991:31.Google Scholar
19.Song, MD. Diagnosing and treating feline eosinophilia granuloma complex. Vet Med (Praha). 1994:89:11411145.Google Scholar
20.Messinger, LM. Therapy for feline dermatoses. Vet Clin North Am. 1995;25(4):9811005.Google Scholar
21.Dodman, NH, Shuster, L, Court, MH, Dixen, R. Investigation into the use of narcotic antagonists in the treatment of a stereotypic behavior pattern (crib-biting) in the horse. Am J Vet Res. 1987;48(2):311319.Google Scholar
22.Dodman, NH, Shuster, L. Data on file. Tufts University, Grata, MA.Google Scholar
23.Smith, KC, Pittlekow, MR. Naltrexone for neurotic excoriations. J Am Acad Deraiatol. 1989:20:860861.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
24.Christenson, GA, Crow, SJ, Mackenzie, TB, Crosby, RD, Mitchell, JE. A placebo controlled double-blind study of naltrexone for trichotillomania. New Research Abstracts. 1994;212.Google Scholar
25.Sandyk, R. Naloxone obliterates obsessive compulsive disorder in Tourette's syndrome. Int J Neurosci. 1987:35:9394.Google Scholar
26.Sneider, JS. Basal ganglia role in behavior: importance of sensory gating and its relevance to psychiatry. Biol Psychiatry. 1984:19:16931709.Google Scholar
27.Vite, CH. Neurology for the behaviorist. In: Proceedings of Special Symposium on Animal Behavior; January 14–18, 1995:113.Google Scholar
28.Blum, K, Cull, JG, Braverman, ER, Comings, DE. Reward deficiency syndrome. Am Scientist. 1996:84:132145.Google Scholar
29.Vechiotti, GG, Galanti, R. Evidence of heredity of cribbing, weaving, and stall walking in thoroughbred horses. Livestock Prod Sci. 1987;14:9195.Google Scholar
30.Karschau-Lowenfish, S. Cribbers. Warmblood News. 1990:2226.Google Scholar
31.Dodman, NH. Shuster, L, Court, MH, Patel, J. The effects of nalmefene on self-mutilative behavior in a stallion. J Am VetMed Assoc. 1988:192:15851586.Google Scholar
32.Dodman, NH, Normile, J, Shuster L, Rand. Equine selfmutilation—a series of 57 cases. J Am Vet Med Assoc. 1994; 204(8):12191223.Google Scholar
33.Moon-Fanelli, A, Dodman, NH, Ginsberg, B. Compulsive stereotypy in bull terriers: a genetic system gone awry. In: Proceedings of the Animal Behavior Society; July 1995; Lincoln, NB.Google Scholar
34.Allsopp, M, Verduyn, C. Adolescents with obsessive—compulsive disorder: a case note review of consecutive patients referred to a provincial regional adolescent psychiatry unit. J Adolescence. 1990:13:157.Google Scholar
35.Moon-Fanelli, A, Dodman, NH. The phenomenology, development and pharmacotherapy of compulsive tail chasing in bull terriers: including an open trial of serotonin uptake inhibitors. J Am Vet Med Assoc.Google Scholar
36.Dodman, NH. The obsessive/repetitive dog. In: The Dog Who Loved Too Much. New York: Bantam; 1996:190225Google Scholar
37.Rapoport, JL. Drug treatment of canine acral lick: an animal model of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1992:49:517521.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
38.Overall, KL. Use of clomipramine to treat ritualistic stereotypic motor behavior in three dogs. J Am Vet Med ASSOC. 1994;205(12):17331741.Google Scholar
39.Shuster, L, Dodman, NH. Basic mechanisms underlying stereotypy and compulsive behaviors. In: Dodman, NH, Shuster, L, eds. Veterinary Behavioral Pharmacology and Therapeutics. Boston: Blackwell Scientific. In press.Google Scholar
40.Poulsen, EMB, Honeyman, V, Valentine, PA, et al. , Use of fluoxetine for the treatment of stereotypical pacing behavior in a captive polar bear. J Am Vet Med Assoc. 1996; 209:14701474.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
41.Dodman, NH. Pharmacological treatment of behavioural problems in cats. Vet International. 1994:6:1320.Google Scholar
42.Dodman, NH, Shuster, L, White, SD, Court, MH, Parker, D, Dixon, R. The use of narcotic antagonists as therapeutic agents to modify stereotypic self-licking, self-chewing and scratching behavior in dogs. J Am Vet Med Assoc. 1988:193:815819.Google Scholar
43.Turner, R. Opioid antagoinist feather picking in birds. Proceedings of Annual Conference of the Association of Avian Veterinarians; August 31–September 4, 1993; Nashville, TN; p116118.Google Scholar
44.Mason, GJ. Stereotypies: a critical review. Anim Behav. 1991:41:1015.Google Scholar
45.Randrup, A, Bunkvad, I. Sterotyped activities produced by amphetamine in several animal species and man. Psychopharmacology. 1967:11:300310.Google Scholar
46.Falk, JL. Production of polydipsia in normal rats by an intermittent food schedule. Science. 1961:133:195196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
47.Insel, TR, Mos, J, Olivier, B. Animal models of obsessivecompulsive disorder: a review. In: Current Insights in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. Hollander, E, Zohar, J, Marazitti, D, Olivier, B, eds. New York: John Wiley & Sons Ltd. 1994:117135.Google Scholar
48.Houben-Gelissen, M, Mos, J, Olivier, B. Animal models for obsessive-compulsive disorder: new perspectives. In: Olivier, B, Manceaux, A, eds. Destructive Drives and Impulse Control–Preclinical Considerations. Den Haag:CIP; 1991: 5769.Google Scholar
49.Olivier, B. Animal models in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Int Clin Psychopharmacology. 1992;7(Sl):2729.Google Scholar