Hostname: page-component-76dd75c94c-h9cmj Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-30T08:28:48.166Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Geographical proximity and the transmission of drug abuse among siblings: evaluating a contagion model in a Swedish National Sample

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 August 2019

Kenneth S. Kendler*
Affiliation:
Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA Department of Psychiatry, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA Department of Human and Molecular Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA
Henrik Ohlsson
Affiliation:
Center for Primary Health Care Research, Lund University, Malmö, Sweden
Alexis C. Edwards
Affiliation:
Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA Department of Psychiatry, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA
Jan Sundquist
Affiliation:
Center for Primary Health Care Research, Lund University, Malmö, Sweden Department of Family Medicine and Community Health, Department of Population Health Science and Policy, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, USA Department of Functional Pathology, Center for Community-based Healthcare Research and Education (CoHRE), School of Medicine, Shimane University, Shimane, Japan
Kristina Sundquist
Affiliation:
Center for Primary Health Care Research, Lund University, Malmö, Sweden Department of Family Medicine and Community Health, Department of Population Health Science and Policy, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, USA Department of Functional Pathology, Center for Community-based Healthcare Research and Education (CoHRE), School of Medicine, Shimane University, Shimane, Japan
*
Author for correspondence: Kenneth S. Kendler, E-mail: Kenneth.Kendler@vcuhealth.org
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Aims

Can drug abuse (DA) be transmitted psychologically between adult siblings consistent with a social contagion model?

Methods

We followed Swedish sibling pairs born in 1932–1990 until one of them, sibling1 (S1), had a first DA registration. We then examined, using Cox regression, the hazard rate for a first registration for DA in sibling2 (S2) within 3 years of a first DA registration in S1 as a function of their geographical proximity. We examined 153 294 informative pairs. To control for familial confounding, we repeated these analyses in sibships containing multiple pairs, comparing risk in different siblings with their proximity to S1. DA was recorded in medical, criminal or pharmacy registries.

Results

The best-fit model predicted risk for DA in S2 as a function of the log of kilometres between S1 and S2 with parameter estimates (±95% confidence intervals) of 0.94 (0.92; 0.95). Prediction of DA included effects of cohabitation and an interaction of proximity and time since S1 registration with stronger effects of proximity early in the follow-up period. Proximity effects were stronger for smaller S1–S2 age differences and for same- v. opposite-sex pairs. Sibship analyses confirmed sibling-pair results.

Conclusions

Consistent with a social contagion model, the probability of transmission of a first registration for DA in sibling pairs is related to their geographical proximity and similarity in age and sex. Such effects for DA are time-dependent and include cohabitation effects. These results illustrate the complexity of the familial aggregation of DA and support efforts to reduce their contagious spread within families in adulthood.

Type
Original Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2019

Drug use and drug abuse (DA) often clusters in social and spatial networks (Galea et al., Reference Galea, Nandi and Vlahov2004; Dishion and Dodge, Reference Dishion, Dodge, Dodge, Dishion and Lansford2006; Christakis and Fowler, Reference Christakis and Fowler2008; Rosenquist et al., Reference Rosenquist, Murabito, Fowler and Christakis2010). Two mechanisms are commonly proposed to explain spatial clustering: correlated exposures to psychological or social risk factors and person to person transmission (Cheng et al., Reference Cheng, Li, Silenzio and Caine2014) sometimes called ‘social contagion’ (Christakis and Fowler, Reference Christakis and Fowler2013). DA strongly aggregates within families, likely as a result of genetic and shared environmental factors (Bierut et al., Reference Bierut, Dinwiddie, Begleiter, Crowe, Hesselbrock, Nurnberger, Porjesz, Schuckit and Reich1998; Merikangas et al., Reference Merikangas, Stolar, Stevens, Goulet, Preisig, Fenton, Zhang, O'Malley and Rounsaville1998). We have recently examined DA among parent–offspring, sibling and cousin pairs in Sweden showing stronger transmission in those living together v. those residing in the same town which in turn was stronger than those living only in the same large metropolitan area (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Ohlsson, Sundquist and Sundquist2019). However, we are unaware of prior efforts to examine, using precise measures of geographical proximity, whether the transmission of DA within adult relatives contributes to this aggregation.

We here report a study building on our earlier results by utilising information in Sweden on the geographical location of all individuals' residence known to within 250 m. While our earlier report on DA was restricted to residences within the same metropolitan area, the current analyses follow-up sibling pairs throughout the country of Sweden. Specifically, we follow full-sibling pairs until one of them (S1) is first registered for DA. We then examine the hazard rate in the other sibling (S2) for DA over the next 3 years as a function of the geographical proximity of S2 to S1. We also examine whether the effect of proximity on risk for DA in S2 risk attenuates over time.

The validity of our approach towards elucidating the psychological transmission of DA is supported by evidence in adult siblings that proximity is related both to frequency of contact (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Mancini and Maxwell1990; White and Riedmann, Reference White and Riedmann1992; Stocker and Lanthier, Reference Stocker and Lanthier1997; White, Reference White2001; Spitze and Trent, Reference Spitze and Trent2006) and emotional closeness (Suggs, Reference Suggs1989; Lee et al., Reference Lee, Mancini and Maxwell1990; White and Riedmann, Reference White and Riedmann1992; White, Reference White2001; Spitze and Trent, Reference Spitze and Trent2006; Van Volkom, Reference Van Volkom2006; Mulder and van der Meer, Reference Mulder and van der Meer2009). Our second design examines sibships with multiple siblings. In them, we explore, after a first registration for DA in one sibling (S1), whether the risk among the remaining siblings (S2, S3, S4 etc.) is predicted by their geographical proximity to S1.

Methods

This study utilised several Swedish population-based registers with national coverage, the availability and content of which have been described previously (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Maes, Sundquist, Ohlsson and Sundquist2013a). Records are linked using unique personal identification numbers, replaced by anonymous serial numbers to maintain confidentiality. The study was approved by the Regional Ethical Review Board of Lund University. From the Swedish Multigenerational register, we included in the study database all possible full-sibling pairs, where both individuals within the pair were born between 1932 and 1990, and had a maximum age difference of 10 years. For pairs born prior to 1960, we required that both individuals were alive at 1985, and for individuals born 1960 and onwards we required that both were alive at age 15. The restriction that both siblings should be alive at age 15 was set because this is the age when individuals can be registered for DA in the criminal registers. The restriction that individuals born prior to 1960 also should be alive at age 25 is due to the fact that most of the registrations for DA occur before age 30 and the ascertainment for DA in the relevant registers are probably less complete during the 1970s and 1980s.

For all individuals, we included yearly information, from 1975 to 2012, about the place of residence within 250 m. This enabled us to examine the distance between places of sibling residence. DA was assessed in the Swedish Inpatient Register from 1973 to 2012, the Swedish Outpatient Register from 2001 to 2012, the Swedish Mortality Register (1969–2012), the Swedish Pharmacy Register (2005–2012), the Swedish Criminal (1973–2012) and Suspicion Registers (1998–2012). Specifically, DA was identified in the Swedish Medical and Mortality Registries by ICD codes (ICD8: Drug dependence (304); ICD9: Drug psychoses (292) and Drug dependence (304); ICD10: Mental and behavioural disorders due to psychoactive substance use (F10–F19), except for those due to alcohol (F10) or tobacco (F17)); in the Suspicion Register by codes 3070, 5010, 5011 and 5012, that reflect crimes related to DA and in the Crime Register by references to laws covering narcotics (law 1968:64, paragraph 1, point 6) and drug-related driving offences (law 1951:649, paragraph 4, subsection 2 and paragraph 4A, subsection 2). DA was identified in individuals (excluding those suffering from cancer) in the Prescribed Drug Register who had retrieved (in average) more than four defined daily doses a day for 12 months from either of hypnotics and sedatives (Anatomical Therapeutic Chemical (ATC) Classification System N05C and N05BA) or opioids (ATC: N02A).

We selected sibling pairs where at least one in the pair, sibling1 (S1), was registered for DA for the first time at which point follow-up began for sibling2 (S2) until registration for the specific phenotype, death, emigration or end of follow-up (3 years after S1's registration). We utilised a 3-year follow-up period because prior analyses identified elevated rates of DA registration for that period after a DA registration in a close relative (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Ohlsson, Sundquist and Sundquist2019).

We used Cox proportional hazards model where the main predictor was distance in kilometres between siblings at S1's registration. We tested several models that allowed the relationship between distance and the outcome variable to differ and included or excluded an extra effect if S1 and S2 cohabitated. In all models age at registration in S1, age difference between S1 and S2 and small areas for market statistics (SAMS) density were included. The models we tested included, for example, the crude distance, the natural log of distance and models with a spline at different distances. The spline models suggest that there is one effect during the first kilometres and after a predefined ‘knot’ the effect will change. We included models where we placed the ‘knot’ at 5, 10, 15, 20 and 25 km. The models controlled for age in S2 at S1's registration, S1–S2 age differences, gender composition and SAMS density which reflected, within the SAMS where S2 resided at S1's registration, the proportion of DA registrations in individuals with ±5 years age difference to S2 within a 3-year interval around that registration. We included SAMS density because of prior evidence from multi-level modelling analyses of substantial concentration of DA in certain SAMS. Specifically, in Sweden SAMS units accounted for 4.5% of the population variance for DA (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Ohlsson, Sundquist and Sundquist2015b).

Defined by Statistics Sweden to represent geographically distinct communities, there are ~9200 SAMS in Sweden, each with an average population of 1000. For the analysis of sibships, we included, in the Cox proportional hazards model, a separate stratum for each sibship that allows each sibship to have a separate baseline hazard function. The best fitting models were chosen by Akaike's information criterion (AIC) (Akaike, Reference Akaike1987).

We investigated the proportionality assumption by including an interaction term between log of time and proximity. This model was also used to calculate the hazard ratios (HR) at different time point during our follow-up period. Statistical analyses were performed in SAS 9.4 (SAS Institute, 2012).

Results

We identified 153 294 sibling pairs informative for DA proximity analyses (Table 1). Within 3 years of the first DA registration of S1, 3.4% of S2s also had first registrations for DA. The mean distance between concordant and discordant siblings was, respectively, 44.8 and 76.4 km. The DA density in the SAMS region in which S2 was residing was higher when S2 was v. was not registered for DA (3.2 v. 1.8%).

Table 1. Descriptive statistics on full sibling pairs from the Swedish population where at least one in the pair are registered for DA

DA, drug abuse; S1, Sibling 1; S2, Sibling 2; SAMS, Small Area for Market Statistics; YOB, year of birth.

Controlling for SAMS density of DA, age difference and age at S1's DA registration, the best fit model for all sibling pairs included a household and distance effect (Table 2). Residing outside S1's household had a substantial protective HR for S2: 0.76 (0.71; 0.82). The estimated HR per log of kilometres for DA in S2 was 0.94 (0.92; 0.95) (Table 2, Fig. 1). The risk of DA in S2 fell rapidly as a function of distance for those living within 75 km of S1, then declined at a slower pace nearly asymptoting at ~150 km.

Table 2. Model fit for different model. Values are for AICa

a Best fit model is given in bold and italics.

Fig. 1. The HR (±95% CI) for a first registration of DA in S2 in 3 years after a first registration for DA in S1 as a function of distance (in kilometres) they were residing apart at the time of S1's DA. The HR does not begin at unity because of the cohabitation effect which is significant in this model. For details and covariates, see Table 3.

Risk for DA in S2 was greater the younger the age at S1's registration, the smaller the S1–S2 age difference and the greater the DA density in the SAMS area where S2 was residing. Of note, as seen in Fig. 1, S2's risk for DA does not begin at 1.0 because of the household effect. Significant interactions were seen between both the household and proximity effects, and time since S1's registration, indicating a failure of the proportionality assumption. For example, the HR of log of kilometres in the prediction of risk for DA registration in S2 at 1 week, 1 month, 1, 2 and 3 years after DA registration in S1 was estimated to equal, respectively, 0.88 (0.84; 0.92), 0.94 (0.93; 0.96), 0.95 (0.93; 0.97) and 0.96 (0.94; 0.98) (Appendix Table A1 and Fig. 2).

Fig. 2. The HR (±95% CI) for a first registration of DA in S2 in 3 years after a first registration for DA in S1 as a function of distance (in kilometres) they were residing apart at the time of S1's DA and the time period from D1's first DA registration. For details and covariates, see Appendix Table A3.

Details about the informative male–male, female–female, female–male and male–female sibling pairs for proximity analysis of DA are also seen in Table 1. The best-fit model for male–male pairs is the same as that seen for the entire sample (household  + log of distance). For female–female and female–male pairs, the best fit model was log of distance only although the fit improvement over the covariate only model was slight. For male–female pairs, the best fit model included only a household effect. Proximity effects for male–male pairs (HR  =  0.93, 0.91; 0.95) were slightly stronger than seen with all pairs, while effects for the female–female and female–male pairs were weaker and not significant.

We identified 2470 informative sibships for DA where the best-fit model was the same as that obtained in the sibling pairs. The HR for distance was slightly stronger in the sibships than in the pair analyses (Table 3).

Table 3. Results from Cox proportional hazards models (±95% CI) on full sibling pairs and sibships from the Swedish population where at least one in the pair are registered for DAa

a Key results for log of distance are given in bold. For the ‘all’ analyses, sex of S1 and S2 were controlled for.

Examining registry effects for DA

Could registry effects bias upwards our estimates of proximity effects for DA if police or physicians were more likely to, respectively, arrest or treat siblings for DA who lived close together? We explored four registry based patterns of DA transmission from S1 → S2: medical → medical, criminal → criminal, criminal → medical and medical → criminal (Appendix Table A2). These did not significantly differ for either household or proximity effects (Appendix Table A3).

Discussion

We utilised information in Sweden on the geographical location of individuals to study the psychological transmission of DA, postulating, in accord with empirical evidence (Suggs, Reference Suggs1989; Lee et al., Reference Lee, Mancini and Maxwell1990; White and Riedmann, Reference White and Riedmann1992; Stocker and Lanthier, Reference Stocker and Lanthier1997; White, Reference White2001; Spitze and Trent, Reference Spitze and Trent2006; Van Volkom, Reference Van Volkom2006; Mulder and van der Meer, Reference Mulder and van der Meer2009), that sibling proximity in adulthood reflects both frequency of contact and emotional closeness. We controlled for a key potential confound – that the effect of sibling proximity might arise simply from correlated psychosocial risk for siblings living closer together. We did so by accounting for, in our Cox models, the density of DA in similarly aged individuals in the small community in which S2 resided at the time of S1's registration. The density of DA effectively controls for most community factors that impact on S2's risk for DA as well as any possible ascertainment biases in DA rates.

Furthermore, we sought to increase confidence in our conclusions by two further analyses. First, did time since S1 registration interact with proximity effect on risk in S2? Detecting such an effect supports the validity of our findings because such results could not plausibly arise from confounding community influences, as these would not change substantially over the short time periods examined. Second, sibling pairs share a wide range of familial risk factors – such as rearing environment, peer groups and social class – that could confound our analyses. We utilised a sibship design that compared risks for registration in multiple siblings of S1 as a function of their relative proximity to S1. In these analyses, all comparisons were within the same family, thereby controlling for many background, genetic, family and community environmental factors.

Transmission within social networks or ‘social contagion’ (Christakis and Fowler, Reference Christakis and Fowler2013) has been demonstrated for both alcohol intake (Rosenquist et al., Reference Rosenquist, Murabito, Fowler and Christakis2010) and smoking (Christakis and Fowler, Reference Christakis and Fowler2008). Most contagion models for DA have been developed within adolescent peer group or community settings and examine only illicit substance use (Dinger and Oetting, Reference Dinger and Oetting1993; Bauman and Ennett, Reference Bauman and Ennett1994; Galea et al., Reference Galea, Nandi and Vlahov2004; Anthony, Reference Anthony, Dodge, Dishion and Lansford2006; Dishion and Dodge, Reference Dishion, Dodge, Dodge, Dishion and Lansford2006; Ennett et al., Reference Ennett, Bauman, Hussing, Faris, Foshee, Cai and DuRant2006). Peer group nominations, which are critical to many such analyses, have potential biases (Liebow et al., Reference Liebow, McGrady, Branch, Vera, Klovdahl, Lovely, Mueller and Mann1995). We are unaware of prior efforts to examine transmission of DA in siblings, when the key predictor variable – geographical proximity – is objectively defined and the analysis focuses on DA rather than quantity of use.

Five additional analyses supported the validity of our DA findings. First, transmission of DA was stronger in sibling pairs closer together in age. Second, DA transmission was substantially stronger in same-sex v. opposite-sex sibling pairs. Third, controlling for familial effects in our sibship analyses, proximity effects were not attenuated. Fourth, the strength of the proximity effect was more robust immediately after S1's registration and this effect attenuated over time. Fifth, registry specific effects, which might result from police or physicians being more likely to arrest or treat siblings for DA who live close together, do not appear to contribute substantially to our findings. We also performed an additional validity check on our results. Could we confirm prior evidence that older siblings have a particularly strong influence on drug use in their younger siblings? (Brook et al., Reference Brook, Whiteman, Gordon and Brenden1983; Needle et al., Reference Needle, McCubbin, Wilson, Reineck, Lazar and Mederer1986; Sakai et al., Reference Sakai, Stallings, Mikulich-Gilbertson, Corley, Young, Hopfer and Crowley2004; Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Ohlsson, Sundquist and Sundquist2013b). We indeed found this to be the case. A model controlling for household effect, log of distance, age difference, age of S1 at registration and SAMS density, the HR for DA in S2, given that S2 was younger v. older than S1, was 1.70 (1.60; 1.80).

The transmission of DA among siblings might involve S1 changing attitudes about drug use in S2 as well as providing a positively perceived model whose behaviour could be imitated (Brook et al., Reference Brook, Whiteman, Gordon and Brenden1983). However, in addition, transmission of DA could include information about sources of supply and the direct transfer of the abused substance.

Prior relevant study on DA

Our findings on within-family transmission of DA based on precise proximity measures are consistent with findings from our prior analyses dividing parent–offspring, sibling and cousin pairs into three proximity categories: household, community and metropolitan area (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Ohlsson, Sundquist and Sundquist2019). In those analyses, the age of at-risk secondary cases was markedly restricted (to ages 19–23) to maximise the proportion of cohabiting parent–offspring and sibling pairs while no such restrictions were operative in our present analyses. Our results confirmed and extended our earlier findings for DA showing evidence for psychological transmission of DA among relatives (with transmission strongest for cohabiting relatives and weakest in those only residing in the same metropolitan area). In accord with our results, our prior analyses also found, in both parent–offspring and cousin pairs, stronger DA transmission in male–male pairs and in those closer in age (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Ohlsson, Sundquist and Sundquist2019).

The sibling pairs used in our prior analyses constituted 16.9% of our sample examined here. To determine whether the similarity of results arose from sample overlap, we repeated our main analyses without those pairs. As seen in Appendix Table A4, no appreciable changes were seen in our key results. For example, the HR for the log of distance in the sibling pairs was 0.94 (0.92; 0.95) in our entire sample and 0.94 (0.92; 0.96) in our analyses dropping the pairs utilised in the prior analyses thereby confirming that this report indeed represented an independent and methodologically quite different confirmation of our previous findings.

Limitations

These results should be interpreted in the context of six potential methodological limitations. First, our results apply only to the Swedish population assessed between 1975 and 2012. Electronic means of human communication have evolved dramatically across this period and may change the role of geographical proximity in transmission of DA. Second, our assessment of DA was limited to data available from Swedish registries. While such administrative data have important advantages (e.g. no refusals or reporting biases), it surely results in false negative reports. Our measures likely reflect the more severe end of DA that results in medical or legal consequences. Third, DA is an emerging phenomenon and the timing of registration is unlikely to capture precisely the onset of abuse. This is one reason why we used a 3-year window to assess transmission of DA registration within siblings, the second reason being we observed most within-family transmission of DA within this limit in a prior Swedish study (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Ohlsson, Sundquist and Sundquist2019). Fourth, our measure of distance between siblings was substantially skewed, hence our use of a logarithmic transformation. As an additional attempt to examine possible biases, we repeated our key analysis excluding the largest 1, 5 and 10% of inter-sibling distances with no appreciable change in parameter estimation. Fifth, we cannot rule out the possibility that transmission of risk to S2 might be influenced by onsets of DA in peers of S1. This is unlikely because such onsets would need to be tightly correlated in time with onsets in S1 to explain the stronger effects of distance on S2 risk shortly after S1's onset.

Furthermore, S2 is likely be in closer touch with and have a stronger emotional bond with his sibling than with his sibling's peers and peer effects would be unlikely to produce our strong evidence for stronger DA transmission among same-sex siblings. Finally, attribution of causal effects in non-experimental data is always fraught with hazard. While our results are consistent with a direct causal effect of DA in S1 and risk for DA in S2, and we have attempted to control for key relevant confounders, skepticism about such causal claims is warranted.

Conclusion

In Sweden, the probability of transmission of DA registration from a young adult to his or her sibling is stronger the shorter the distance between their places of residence. This relationship is best described by a logarithmic function. The probability of transmission falls rapidly over relatively short distances asymptoting at between 100 and 150 km. A range of analyses support the validity of these primary results. However, as with all observational epidemiological investigations, causal inference should be regarded as tentative. If correct, our findings, in line with our previous research (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Sundquist, Ohlsson, Palmer, Maes, Winkleby and Sundquist2012, Reference Kendler, Maes, Sundquist, Ohlsson and Sundquist2013a, Reference Kendler, Ohlsson, Sundquist and Sundquist2013b, Reference Kendler, Ohlsson, Sundquist and Sundquist2015a), suggest that the transmission of DA within families reflects a complex mixture of genetic and environmental effects. Some of the environmental effects on DA appear to act during childhood and adolescence, probably reflecting the influences of the home and of peer groups. We here show that environmental/psychological factors as sources of social contagion continue to play an important role in early adulthood. Our findings also have clinical relevance. After the onset of DA, the affected individual's siblings living nearby are at high risk and may constitute good targets for rapidly employed prevention efforts.

Data

Data used in these analyses are not publicly available due to confidentiality concerns of the Swedish

authorities.

Financial support

This project was supported by grant R01DA030005 from the National Institutes of Health, the Swedish Research Council (K2012-70X-15428-08-3), the Swedish Research Council for Health, Working Life and Welfare (in Swedish: Forte; Reg.nr: 2013-1836), the Swedish Research Council (2012-2378; 2014-10134) and FORTE (2014-0804) as well as ALF funding from Region Skåne awarded.

Conflict of interest

The authors have no conflict of interest to declare.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. The study was approved by the Regional Ethical Review Board of Lund University.

Appendix

Table A1. Test of the proportionality assumption for the Cox proportional hazards models on full sibling pairs from the Swedish population where at least one in the pair are registered for DA

Table A2. Descriptive statistics on full sibling pairs from the Swedish population where at least one in the pair are registered for DA

Table A3. Results from Cox proportional hazards models on full sibling pairs from the Swedish population where at least one in the pair are registered for DA

Table A4. Sib pair analyses (Table 3) repeated excluding individuals from prior study of ‘A contagion model for within-family transmission of drug abuse’

Footnotes

Numbers are HRs and 95% confidence intervals (CIs). All pairs are included in the analyses.

References

Akaike, H (1987) Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika 52, 317332.Google Scholar
Anthony, JC (2006) Deviant peer effects: Perspectives of an epidemiologist. In Dodge, KA, Dishion, TJ and Lansford, JE (eds), Deviant Peer Influences in Programs for Youth. New York, NY: The Guilford Press, pp. 4466.Google Scholar
Bauman, KE and Ennett, ST (1994) Peer influence on adolescent drug use. American Psychologist 49, 820822.Google Scholar
Bierut, LJ, Dinwiddie, SH, Begleiter, H, Crowe, RR, Hesselbrock, V, Nurnberger, JI Jr, Porjesz, B, Schuckit, MA and Reich, T (1998) Familial transmission of substance dependence: alcohol, marijuana, cocaine, and habitual smoking: a report from the Collaborative Study on the Genetics of Alcoholism. Archives of General Psychiatry 55, 982988.Google Scholar
Brook, JS, Whiteman, M, Gordon, AS and Brenden, C (1983) Older brother's influence on younger sibling's drug use. Journal of Psychology 114, 8390.Google Scholar
Cheng, Q, Li, H, Silenzio, V and Caine, ED (2014) Suicide contagion: a systematic review of definitions and research utility. PLoS One 9, e108724.Google Scholar
Christakis, NA and Fowler, JH (2008) The collective dynamics of smoking in a large social network. New England Journal of Medicine 358, 22492258.Google Scholar
Christakis, NA and Fowler, JH (2013) Social contagion theory: examining dynamic social networks and human behavior. Statistics in Medicine 32, 556577.Google Scholar
Dinger, MM and Oetting, ER (1993) Similarity in drug use patterns between adolescents and their friends. Adolescence 28, 253266.Google Scholar
Dishion, TJ and Dodge, KA (2006) Deviant peer contagion in interventions and programs: An ecological framework for understanding influence mechanisms. In Dodge, KA, Dishion, TJ and Lansford, JE (eds), Deviant Peer Influences in Programs for Youth. New York, NY: The Guilford Press, pp. 1443.Google Scholar
Ennett, ST, Bauman, KE, Hussing, A, Faris, R, Foshee, VA, Cai, L and DuRant, RH (2006) The peer context of adolescent substance use: findings from social network analysis. Journal of Research on Adolescence 16, 159186.Google Scholar
Galea, S, Nandi, A and Vlahov, D (2004) The social epidemiology of substance use. Epidemiologic Reviews 26, 3652.Google Scholar
Kendler, KS, Sundquist, K, Ohlsson, H, Palmer, K, Maes, H, Winkleby, MA and Sundquist, J (2012) Genetic and familial environmental influences on the risk for drug abuse: a national Swedish adoption study. Archives of General Psychiatry 69, 690697.Google Scholar
Kendler, KS, Maes, HH, Sundquist, K, Ohlsson, H and Sundquist, J (2013 a) Genetic and family and community environmental effects on drug abuse in adolescence: a Swedish national twin and sibling study. American Journal of Psychiatry 171, 209217.Google Scholar
Kendler, KS, Ohlsson, H, Sundquist, K and Sundquist, J (2013 b) Within-family environmental transmission of drug abuse: a Swedish national study. JAMA Psychiatry 70, 235242.Google Scholar
Kendler, KS, Ohlsson, H, Sundquist, J and Sundquist, K (2015 a) Triparental families: a new genetic-epidemiological design applied to drug abuse, alcohol use disorders, and criminal behavior in a Swedish National Sample. American Journal of Psychiatry 172, 553560.Google Scholar
Kendler, KS, Ohlsson, H, Sundquist, K and Sundquist, J (2015 b) Environmental clustering of drug abuse in households and communities: multi-level modeling of a national Swedish sample. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 50, 12771284.Google Scholar
Kendler, KS, Ohlsson, H, Sundquist, J and Sundquist, K (2019) A contagion model for within-family transmission of drug-abuse. American Journal of Psychiatry 176, 239248.Google Scholar
Lee, TR, Mancini, JA and Maxwell, JW (1990) Sibling relationships in adulthood: contact patterns and motivations. Journal of Marriage and Family 52, 431440.Google Scholar
Liebow, E, McGrady, G, Branch, K, Vera, M, Klovdahl, A, Lovely, R, Mueller, C and Mann, E (1995) Eliciting social network data and ecological model-building: focus on choice of name generators and administration of random-walk study procedures. Social Networks 17, 257272.Google Scholar
Merikangas, KR, Stolar, M, Stevens, DE, Goulet, J, Preisig, MA, Fenton, B, Zhang, H, O'Malley, SS and Rounsaville, BJ (1998) Familial transmission of substance use disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry 55, 973979.Google Scholar
Mulder, CH and van der Meer, MJ (2009) Geographical distances and support from family members. Population, Space and Place 15, 381399.Google Scholar
Needle, R, McCubbin, H, Wilson, M, Reineck, R, Lazar, A and Mederer, H (1986) Interpersonal influences in adolescent drug use – the role of older siblings, parents, and peers. International Journal of Addictions 21, 739766.Google Scholar
Rosenquist, JN, Murabito, J, Fowler, JH and Christakis, NA (2010) The spread of alcohol consumption behavior in a large social network. Annals of Internal Medicine 152, 426433, W141.Google Scholar
Sakai, JT, Stallings, MC, Mikulich-Gilbertson, SK, Corley, RP, Young, SE, Hopfer, CJ and Crowley, TJ (2004) Mate similarity for substance dependence and antisocial personality disorder symptoms among parents of patients and controls. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 75, 165175.Google Scholar
SAS Institute I (2012) SAS/STAT® Online Documentation, Version 9.4. Cary, N.C.: SAS Institute, Inc.Google Scholar
Spitze, G and Trent, K (2006) Gender differences in adult sibling relations in two-child families. Journal of Marriage and Family 68, 977992.Google Scholar
Stocker, CM and Lanthier, RP (1997) Sibling relationships in early adulthood. Journal of Family Psychology 11, 210221.Google Scholar
Suggs, PK (1989) Predictors of association among older siblings: a black/white comparison. American Behavioral Scientist 33, 7080.Google Scholar
Van Volkom, M (2006) Sibling relationships in middle and older adulthood: a review of the literature. Marriage & Family Review 20, 151170.Google Scholar
White, L (2001) Sibling relationships over the life course: a panel analysis. Journal of Marriage and Family 63, 555568.Google Scholar
White, LK and Riedmann, A (1992) Ties among adult siblings. Social Forces 71, 85102.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive statistics on full sibling pairs from the Swedish population where at least one in the pair are registered for DA

Figure 1

Table 2. Model fit for different model. Values are for AICa

Figure 2

Fig. 1. The HR (±95% CI) for a first registration of DA in S2 in 3 years after a first registration for DA in S1 as a function of distance (in kilometres) they were residing apart at the time of S1's DA. The HR does not begin at unity because of the cohabitation effect which is significant in this model. For details and covariates, see Table 3.

Figure 3

Fig. 2. The HR (±95% CI) for a first registration of DA in S2 in 3 years after a first registration for DA in S1 as a function of distance (in kilometres) they were residing apart at the time of S1's DA and the time period from D1's first DA registration. For details and covariates, see Appendix Table A3.

Figure 4

Table 3. Results from Cox proportional hazards models (±95% CI) on full sibling pairs and sibships from the Swedish population where at least one in the pair are registered for DAa

Figure 5

Table A1. Test of the proportionality assumption for the Cox proportional hazards models on full sibling pairs from the Swedish population where at least one in the pair are registered for DA

Figure 6

Table A2. Descriptive statistics on full sibling pairs from the Swedish population where at least one in the pair are registered for DA

Figure 7

Table A3. Results from Cox proportional hazards models on full sibling pairs from the Swedish population where at least one in the pair are registered for DA

Figure 8

Table A4. Sib pair analyses (Table 3) repeated excluding individuals from prior study of ‘A contagion model for within-family transmission of drug abuse’