Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-ttngx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-09T06:51:09.642Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Negativity in the news and electoral behavior between first- and second-order elections

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 April 2021

Moreno Mancosu*
Affiliation:
Department of Cultures, Politics, and Society, University of Turin, Turin, Italy C. A. Campus Luigi Einaudi, Lungo Dora Siena 100/A, 10153, Turin, Italy
Antonella Seddone
Affiliation:
Department of Cultures, Politics, and Society, University of Turin, Turin, Italy
Giuliano Bobba
Affiliation:
Department of Cultures, Politics, and Society, University of Turin, Turin, Italy Collegio Carlo Alberto, Piazza Vincenzo Arbarello, 8 - 10122, Torino, Italy
*
*Corresponding author. Email: moreno.mancosu@unito.it

Abstract

The effects of news coverage on political attitudes in election campaigns have been widely studied in academic research. In particular, a fertile branch of the literature investigated the impact of news media negativity on turnout. To date, however, findings are mixed, precluding to state a clear relationship. This paper aims to shed a light on this topic by testing whether negative coverage may affect voters' turnout and to what extent. It approaches this research question by accounting for two different dimensions, controlling whether the interplay of media negativity (press and TV coverage) with the type of election (first- vs. second-order elections) has an impact on citizens' propensity to turnout. We test our hypotheses by taking Italy as a case study because it offers a combination of systemic and media characteristics that allows addressing properly the topic. We rely on four datasets covering the 2018 Italian general elections and the 2019 European Parliament Elections in Italy, respectively, with opinion data (2018 ITANES survey and 2019 ITANES-University of Milan survey) and two datasets measuring media negativity by means of a human content analysis carried out on media coverage during the 7 weeks before the election days (2018 and 2019 ITEM data). Our findings show that individual exposure to negative coverage leads to an increase in turnout mainly to the detriment of indecision. This pattern holds both in first- and second-order elections.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the Società Italiana di Scienza Politica

Introduction

The impact that mass media messages have on political attitudes and behavior in election campaigns is a widely debated topic in academic research. Starting from the late 1980s, the literature has investigated the impact of news media negativity on citizens’ beliefs and behaviors – such as, for instance, turnout (e.g. Ansolabehere and Iyengar, Reference Ansolabehere and Iyengar1995), voting behavior (Kahn and Kenney, Reference Kahn and Kenney1999), knowledge (De Vreese and Boomgaarden, Reference De Vreese and Boomgaarden2006), perceived salience of certain issues (Roberts et al., Reference Roberts, Wanta and Dzwo2002) – producing mixed evidence. In this paper, we aim at assessing whether and to what extent negative coverage may harm or boost citizens' propensity to cast their ballot. Also, we are interested in understanding whether the impact of negativity of media coverage on voters' turnout differs in first- or second-order elections.

The literature in this field is extensive, but far from being settled. Several scholars argue that negativity has a de-mobilizing effect on citizens' propensity to vote: negative campaigns – namely, a political debate characterized by mutual attacks, conflict, and criticism among candidates – are most likely to alienate citizens (Ansolabehere et al., Reference Ansolabehere, Iyengar, Simon and Valentino1994; Ansolabehere and Iyengar, Reference Ansolabehere and Iyengar1995). By increasing dissatisfaction with politics and cynicism, negativity keeps people far from the polling stations. However, other studies contend the opposite, suggesting that negative messages contribute to stimulating citizens' attention and political interest, turning to be a boost for political participation (Finkel and Geer, Reference Finkel and Geer1998; Wattenberg and Brians, Reference Wattenberg and Brians1999; Goldstein and Freedman, Reference Goldstein and Freedman2002).

When the attention moves specifically on media and negative coverage, the picture is even more blurred. Indeed, scholars agree that negativity is becoming a common standard and routinized trait of journalism (West, Reference West2009), to the extent that media coverage is even more negative than party communication (Geer, Reference Geer2006, Reference Geer2012; Pedersen, Reference Pedersen2014). Contemporary media outlets are keen to offer higher visibility to negative campaigning: not just by providing visibility to negative contents and messages derived from political actors, but also by adopting a negative tone within the coverage (Jamieson, Reference Jamieson1992). This pattern mirrors precisely the media logic, which puts special emphasis on sensationalism, conflict, and a negative frame. Politicians are aware of this dynamic: given that candidates seek higher visibility within media coverage, they tend to engage in negative campaigning because they know that then media will be more likely to cover them (Hansen and Pedersen, Reference Hansen and Pedersen2008; Walter and Vliegenthart, Reference Walter and Vliegenthart2010; Haselmayer et al., Reference Haselmayer, Meyer and Wagner2019).

However, Min (Reference Min2004) points out that, for a better understanding of media coverage impact, additional individual and contextual features have to be taken into account, suggesting that the type of connection between negativity and political attitudes is less direct and obvious than expected. Media coverage could be received differently by the audience, depending on their traits and on the type of political context in which they are embedded. This latter argument leads us to consider the impact of media negativity on turnout from a wider perspective, considering the political supply and, in particular, the electoral context in which the individual decision process is carried out.

In this respect, the literature raises a very clear point: the elections are not all equally relevant from the voters' perspective. In particular, the paradigm of the second-order elections (Reif and Schmitt, Reference Reif and Schmitt1980) argues that European Parliament Elections are less prominent if compared to the national elections, where voters understand more clearly how their vote will affect the future government of the country. Instead, European Parliamentary Elections are intended as elections where there is ‘less at stake’. This is mainly due to the institutional framework and the functioning mechanisms of the decisional power of the European Parliament. The entire electoral process appears ambiguous and citizens feel to not have a say on the EU Commission formation (Decker, Reference Decker2002), perceived as a distant institution. However, it should be noticed that, although the recent recession has brought the EU at the center of the domestic political agenda, EU issues still appear as domesticated, namely depicted (and addressed) according to a national perspective (Kriesi, Reference Kriesi2016). Similarly, although studies on the Europeanization of national public debates (Koopmans and Erbe, Reference Koopmans and Erbe2004; Koopmans and Statham, Reference Koopmans and Statham2010) showed that media tend to offer an increasing and larger visibility to EU and related issues (Pfetsch and Eschner, Reference Pfetsch, Adam and Eschner2008), the EP elections still maintain specific traits in terms of voting behavior and lower turnout rates (Franklin and Hobolt, Reference Franklin, Hobolt and Richardson2015). In short, European Parliamentary elections fail to mobilize citizens' interest and do not incentivize their participation in the vote.

In light of these arguments, in this article, we test the effects of negative media coverage on voters’ propensity to turnout in different settings. On the one hand, we consider whether negative media coverage per se affects individuals, and in which way. On the other hand, we test whether negative media coverage may impact differently in different electoral contexts, namely distinguishing between national and EP elections. The aim of the paper is therefore twofold: (i) assessing the effect that negative political coverage might have on the propensity to turnout in a context, the Italian one, characterized by a strong presence of negative campaigning; (ii) testing whether the electoral setting (first-order elections vis-à-vis second-order elections) might alter the magnitude and substantive significance of these effects.

To test our research questions, this study relies on four different datasets. Two of them are based on individual data measuring our dependent variable (turnout) as well as individual control variables and media exposure. Individual data were collected via CAWI mode in the election campaigns that preceded the 2018 Italian General Elections (2018 ITANES survey) and the 2019 European Parliament Elections in Italy (2019 ITANES-University of Milan Survey). The other two datasets provide information about the media coverage of the 2018 and 2019 election campaigns carried out through a human content analysis of the political contents published or broadcasted by the main Italian daily newspapers and evening newscasts during the 7 weeks before the vote (2018 and 2019 ITEM project – ITalian Election on the Media). We derived here our main independent variable, namely the negativity of coverage tone. Since each individual-level dataset reported information concerning the media exposure, we have thus been able to build a merged dataset – providing, for each election year, both individual and media coverage information.

Our findings suggest a positive impact of negative coverage on turnout on the whole audience, especially at the detriment of pre-electoral uncertainty. Negativity exposure, on the other side, does not affect people who are decided not to vote in the upcoming election. For what concerns the effect driven by the electoral context (first- vis-à-vis second-order election), we do not find any impact – namely, the effects that we see in the first-order election are substantively equal to those seen in the second-order election. Indeed, the effects of media negativity on undecided voters are similar in both first- and second-order elections.

The paper is organized as follows: the next section provides literature background and illustrates the hypotheses tested within the study, the following section presents methodological details concerning datasets, variables, and models employed, while the fourth section describes results. The final section discusses the findings, outline the limits, and refine the theoretical contribution that the work brings to existing literature.

Background

Over the past decades, political communication research has been committed to understanding and testing the presence of the so-called ‘media effects’, namely, the way in which mass-media-conveyed information is able to change people behavior and attitudes (Kahn and Kenney, Reference Kahn and Kenney1999; Slater, Reference Slater2004; De Vreese and Boomgaarden, Reference De Vreese and Boomgaarden2006). After a long phase driven by the Klapper's (Reference Klapper1960) claim that the effects of mass communication were not relevant, in the last two decades scholars argued the opposite, by providing some evidence that media effects were ‘far from minimal’ (Iyengar and Simon, Reference Iyengar and Simon2000). From a psychological point of view, exposure to mass media might influence people's political ideas through processes of learning (the acquisition of new information), which, in turn, are potentially able to (partly or completely) change one's system of beliefs (Lee, Reference Lee2015). An extensive political communication literature has demonstrated that media effects may affect a variety of attitudes and behaviors. For instance, De Vreese and Boomgaarden (Reference De Vreese and Boomgaarden2006) show that, consistently with learning mechanisms, exposure to news outlets presenting a larger amount of political content contributes to increasing political knowledge. Also, the ways in which politicians and parties are (positively or negatively) covered in the mass media might shape the audience's attitudes and behaviors (namely, voting) toward various political objects (see Ansolabehere and Iyengar, Reference Ansolabehere and Iyengar1995; Hillygus and Jackman, Reference Hillygus and Jackman2003; Hopmann et al., Reference Hopmann, Vliegenthart, De Vreese and Albæk2010).

Media effects and the relationship between negativity and turnout

The academic debate aiming at connecting media effects and participation is particularly extensive (Basil et al., Reference Basil, Schooler, Reeves and Biocca1991; Ansolabehere et al., Reference Ansolabehere, Iyengar, Simon and Valentino1994; Ansolabehere and Iyengar, Reference Ansolabehere and Iyengar1995; Germond and Witcover, Reference Germond and Witcover1996; Finkel and Geer, Reference Finkel and Geer1998; Kahn and Kenney, Reference Kahn and Kenney1999; Min, Reference Min2004), and it has been mainly committed to assessing whether people participation might be increased/decreased by negative political advertising, a topic that is particularly relevant in the US literature. Likewise, the relationship between negativity aired by mass media (through advertising or news coverage) and turnout has represented a leitmotif of the academic debate since the beginning of the 1990s (Ansolabehere and Iyengar, Reference Ansolabehere and Iyengar1995; Finkel and Geer, Reference Finkel and Geer1998). With this respect, several scholars argue that candidates and parties engaging in mudslinging may alienate citizens from the electoral process, with the consequence of depressing turnout (see Ansolabehere and Iyengar, Reference Ansolabehere and Iyengar1995; Germond and Witcover, Reference Germond and Witcover1996). In particular, these studies contend that being exposed to advertising that negatively addresses or attacks political competitors suppresses turnout. Additional literature, moreover, clarifies that negative political debate, incivility, and conflict contribute to depress citizen's engagement in politics and contribute to increasing distrust (Mutz and Reeves, Reference Mutz and Reeves2005). People dislike negative campaigning, they declare to prefer positive discussion of the issues, rather than attacks and mudslinging strategies. Therefore, it seems reasonable to assume that negativity will hurt citizens' turnout, contributing to consolidating their negative feelings concerning politicians and politics.

However, research proves that ‘people think is often at odds with how they act’ (Brooks, Reference Brooks2006: 684). As concerns negativity in election campaigning, for example, we know that negative information tends to be fixed in memory longer than positive information and tends to offer more substantive information than positive ones (Reeves et al., Reference Reeves, Newhagen, Maibach, Basil and Kurz1991; Bradley et al., Reference Bradley, Angelini and Lee2007). This may contribute to developing a more sound knowledge about the candidates and the political agenda, favoring in the end awareness and mobilization (Lau, Reference Lau1982; Garramone et al., Reference Garramone, Atkin, Pinkleton and Cole1990; Geer, Reference Geer2006). Furthermore, subsequent studies contend that negative campaigning may even increase turnout (Finkel and Geer, Reference Finkel and Geer1998; Wattenberg and Brians, Reference Wattenberg and Brians1999). This strand of literature argues that negative campaigning in the mass media exposes citizens to alternative opinions and, thus, is more effective in increasing citizens' knowledge. Citizens, in other words, may count on a larger variety of tools and information for evaluating candidates and parties and, eventually, to cast a more thoughtful vote. Similar arguments come from further research showing that negativity observed in political campaigns contributes to raising voters' attention and incentivizing their participation (Lau and Pomper, Reference Lau and Pomper2001, Reference Lau and Pomper2004; Clinton and Lapinski, Reference Clinton and Lapinski2004; Ladini et al., Reference Ladini, Mancosu and Vezzoni2020). Martin (Reference Martin2008), starting from Zaller (Reference Zaller1992) seminal research, claims that negative coverage stimulates citizens' awareness about the country's problems and, in the end, may boost turnout. Other studies (Mutz and Reeves, Reference Mutz and Reeves2005), instead, while recognizing the media's negativity effect on political interest and knowledge, also point out that negative coverage could harm citizens' political trust – which is usually correlated with propensity to political participation.

All things considered, the literature connecting negative campaigning and turnout has come to a stalemate presenting contradictory results. According to Min (Reference Min2004), this heterogeneity of findings hugely depends on the research setting (experimental vis-à-vis observational), the type of data (individual vis-à-vis aggregate), and the type of campaign (national or local). In this respect, this article aims at assessing whether negative coverage may affect people's turnout, and, in particular, at clarifying in which way. We test our hypotheses in Italy, which represents a very appropriate case study for our purposes. Italian politics, indeed, is characterized by a fierce contrast between parties (Sani and Sartori, Reference Sani, Sartori, Daalder and Mair1982; Bellucci and Segatti, Reference Bellucci and Segatti2011), it presents a high level of polarization at the mass-level (Mancini, Reference Mancini2013), and it also shows high levels of negative coverage of politics and election campaigning (Bobba et al., Reference Bobba, Legnante, Roncarolo and Seddone2013). Given that previous literature on this topic provides mixed results, we state two alternative hypotheses, which read as follows:

H1a: People exposed to high levels of negative political coverage in the outlet they read/watch have higher propensities to turnout.

H1b: People exposed to high levels of negative political coverage in the outlet they read/watch have lower propensities to turnout.

First- and second-order elections: does the electoral context affect media effects?

Countless moderating or mediating factors, not directly related to the individual characteristics of the audience, can be identified in the literature that attempts to link media negativity and turnout. Although previous studies have investigated separately the role of media effects in different electoral contexts, by analyzing those effects in the election campaigns before national (see Hopmann et al., Reference Hopmann, Vliegenthart, De Vreese and Albæk2010) or European Elections (see van Spanje and de Vreese, Reference Van Spanje and de Vreese2014), little research has committed to systematically comparing different types of elections. More precisely, investigations on whether media effects are equally effective, irrespective of the electoral context in which those effects take place, are virtually absent.

Drawing upon the seminal work of Reif and Schmitt (Reference Reif and Schmitt1980), the literature addressed the study of parties' strategies in the various European Election campaigns, as well as outcomes from both the supply-side (namely, party and leaders campaigning, communication, etc.; for instance, see De Vreese, Reference De Vreese2009; Schuck et al., Reference Schuck, Azrout, Boomgaarden, Elenbaas, van Spanje, Vliegenthart, De Vreese, Maier, Strömbäck and Kaid2011) and the demand-side viewpoint (namely, interest in the campaign by voter, turnout rates, and voting behavior; see, for instance, Franklin, Reference Franklin2001; Mattila, Reference Mattila2003). This scholarship agrees in underlining that in European Elections there is much less at stake with respect to a national election. While in National Elections the stake is the government of the country, in European Elections the stake is sending a – more or less limited – number of MEPs in the new (and distant) parliament. The most relevant consequence of this asymmetry is that, compared to national elections, in European elections parties fail to mobilize voters, and thus the electorate shows little interest in the campaign (Lodge, Reference Lodge1996; Norris and Reif, Reference Norris and Reif1997).Footnote 1 Literature underlines also that, being more focused on European issues, second-order elections are usually less interesting for the electorate which is usually more attentive to domestic issues (see Hobolt and Wittrock, Reference Hobolt and Wittrock2011; van Spanje and de Vreese, Reference Van Spanje and de Vreese2014). As a result, because of the less-at-stake argument discussed above, parties are also less prone to invest at the monetary level in election campaigns (Petithomme, Reference Petithomme2012), strengthening the citizens' estrangement from second-order elections.

All those elements lead to arguing that the European Election campaign context might entail a low-profile campaign. This implies that also media effects influencing individuals' propensity to turnout are less relevant with respect to the National elections. The second hypothesis thus reads as follows:

H2: The effect of negative political coverage on turnout is weaker in the context of the European Elections campaign.

Data, measures, and methods

Data

The data employed to test our hypotheses are derived from four datasets. Two of them refer to individual data (measuring our dependent variable, turnout, as well as individual moderator and control variables) gathered from the 2018 ITANES survey and 2019 ITANES-University of Milan survey. Both the surveys were collected by means of CAWI mode before the 2018 National Elections (held on 4 March 2018) and the 2019 European Parliament elections (held on 26 May 2019). However, the two surveys present several differences: first, the companies collecting the data in the two elections are different (IPSOS for 2018 and SWG for 2019 elections); second, the 2018 ITANES survey is collected following a rolling cross-section design (Johnston and Brady, Reference Johnston and Brady2002), which started about 2 months before the election days (every day about 200 cases were randomly selected from an opt-in basket). The 2019 ITANES survey is collected in five waves before the election day. Therefore, in order to maintain the comparability of the two datasets, we had to reach the ‘lowest common denominator’ of the two surveys. Accordingly, we had to harmonize the two datasets so that the cases selected were contacted in the same n days before the respective Election Day. We thus subdivided both the datasets into three waves: the first one covering from 46 to 32 days before the Election Day; the second one covering from 31 to 18 days; the third one covering from 17 to 11 days.

Both the datasets provide information concerning media exposure since respondents were asked to declare: (a) their main source of information and (b) the TV/newspaper outlet they were exposed to in the previous few days. This information has been matched with data from the ITEM project, which studied the media coverage of the 2018 and 2019 electoral campaigns throughout human content analysis of the political contents published or broadcast by the main Italian daily newspapers and TV-news programs. For each election, the analysis has been performed covering the 7 weeks before the election day. The set of outlets analyzed was defined according to two main criteria: the readership/audience and the outlet's political leaning. As the press is concerned, we considered the following outlets: Corriere Della Sera, Il Giornale, Il Fatto Quotidiano, la Repubblica, and La Stampa. Within this selection of newspapers, all political news items published on the first-page and recalled in internal pages were coded. As regards the TV, both private and public companies have been covered: more specifically, we included the following newscasts in their evening editions: TG1, TG2, Tg3, Tg5, and La7 News. In this case, due to the differences between printed and TV media, all political news items broadcasted were coded.

Once harmonized with the days available for the individual datasets, the final media datasets include 2776 news items in 2018 and 3227 items in 2019. Both the media datasets provide a measure of negative tone (Lengauer et al., Reference Lengauer, Esser and Berganza2012)Footnote 2 within the Italian media coverage of the 2018 Italian General Elections and the 2019 EP elections within every outlet covered and in every wave.

The data employed for the analysis are thus a pooled dataset containing respondents interviewed before both the 2018 and 2019 elections in Italy, with, as an additional independent variable, the level of negativity of the media outlet to which they are exposed.

Measures

The literature investigating the relationship between turnout and negativity generally operationalizes turnout as a dichotomous choice (namely, one might be more or less prone to vote). However, more recent literature addressing the effect of media/external factors on voting behavior stresses the importance of voting uncertainty in the processes of opinion formation during the election campaigns (see Redlawsk, Reference Redlawsk2002; Ohme et al., Reference Ohme, de Vreese and Albaek2018). From both a theoretical and an empirical point of view, uncertainty (namely, the impossibility for the respondent of declaring whether one will go to the polls or not) represents a relevant factor that might impinge the way in which results are interpreted. We argue that there is a strong difference in interpreting a result assessing that, for instance, negativity increases turnout at the expense of non-voters, or, rather, at the expense of the group of the uncertain voters, or both. In order to deepen the relationship between electoral behaviors and media negativity, this study employs an operationalization of the turnout variable that distinguishes between three diverse/possible outputs. For both 2018 and 2019 election campaign datasets, we construct a variable that scores 0 when the respondent declares to be undecided, 1 when the respondent declares that he/she will not vote, and 2 when the respondent declares a valid vote choice in the upcoming election.

The main independent variable is the negativity of the media outlet the respondent is exposed to. Empirically, it is derived from the ‘general tone of the news’ variable within the ITEM dataset. We coded a dummy variable that scores 1 when a news item presents a generally negative tone and 0 otherwise; after that, we calculated the average of the variable by wave (namely, over the days of each harmonized wave exposed above) and outlet, and multiplied it by 100. We, therefore, merged the variable to the individual data, in a way that every respondent exposed to a non-missing outlet is coupled with a variable indicating the average negativity that he/she has been exposed to in every wave (the procedure is performed in a way that, to every respondent, only the single main outlet is coupled).

In addition to the other moderator of the effect, we included a further variable identifying the electoral context. It is the case of a dummy variable indicating whether the respondent has been interviewed in the first- or second-order election. We have added a set of additional controls that represent possible alternative explanations for predicting our dependent variable: gender, age, educational level (subdivided in primary, secondary, and tertiary), left-right self-placement (subdivided in right, center-right, center, center-left, left, not located, do not know), political involvement,Footnote 3 and a dummy variable indicating whether the respondent is primarily exposed to a newscast or newspaper.

Models

The two hypotheses are tested through a set of multinomial logistic regression models, having, as the dependent variable, the trichotomous turnout variable. Multinomial logistic regressions allow predicting the probabilities of the three different outcomes at different levels of the same independent variables. Model 1 tests the alternative hypotheses H1a and H1b by simply assessing the effects of the negativity variable, while Model 2 (testing H2) is fitted by performing a two-way interaction between outlet negativity and electoral context. However, interpreting multinomial logistic regression might be particularly complicated, in particular for what concerns the significance and magnitude of the effects of the different outcomes. Also, the interpretation becomes even more difficult when evaluating the levels and significance of interaction terms (Long, Reference Long1997). We thus rely on graphical summaries of probabilities and discrete changes in probabilities (average marginal effects) in order to measure the substantive effects of our independent scores.

Results

Table 1 reports the coefficients for the two models. As regards control variables, it shows that, for what concerns the gender variable, women present lower likelihoods with respect to men to vote compared to uncertain respondents. The same applies to more centrist/non-located people. This trend is consistent in both the models and does not change according to their different parametrizations (namely, the presence/absence of the interaction term).

Table 1. Two multinomial logistic models studying turnout in two Italian elections

Standard errors in parentheses.

***P < 0.01, **P < 0.05, *P < 0.1.

As pertains to our first hypothesis, Model 1 shows that being exposed to more negative news outlets significantly increases turnout in our sample, at the expense of indecision. Figure 1 illustrates the predicted probabilities for the differential effect of negativity on each of the three outcomes.

Figure 1. Predicted probabilities of voting, being undecided, and non-voting at different levels of media negativity.

The increase of decided voters at increasing levels of negativity corresponds to lower levels of indecision. In other words, negativity leads people to be more sure to vote (corroborating hypothesis H1b) at the expense of those who are undecided, while it does not affect people who are sure not to go to the polls.

H2, the hypothesis arguing that the electoral context moderates the effect of negativity, is tested in Model 2. In this case, the effect of the interaction between the main independent variable, negativity, and the moderator is not significant. Figure 2 indicates the average marginal effects of the negativity variable on the three outcomes of the dependent variable, according to whether the electoral context is a first-order (FOE) or a second-order (SOE) one. Our analyses reveal that the sole – slight – difference between the two figures is the effect of negativity on undecided voters, which is non-significant to the 5% threshold in the FOE (the figure is, however, significant to the 10%) and stronger and significant in the SOE. However, a Wald test on the difference between the two average marginal effects resulted as non-significant (P = 0.40). We can thus say that the effects of negativity are not substantively different in the two electoral contexts.

Figure 2. Average marginal effects of negativity on voting, being undecided, and non-voting at different electoral contexts.

Discussion and conclusion

In this article, we were interested in clarifying the linkage between media negativity and voters' turnout. Despite the extensive literature on this field, we have shown that research results are mixed and struggle to provide a clear answer about the impact of negative coverage on participation. Literature suggests that negativity depresses citizens' turnout (Ansolabehere et al., Reference Ansolabehere, Iyengar, Simon and Valentino1994; Ansolabehere and Iyengar, Reference Ansolabehere and Iyengar1995). Negative campaigning provides the image of conflictual politics, and deriving cynicism turns to alienate from politics, disincentivizing citizens to go to the polls. However, other scholarship states that – on the contrary – negativity could bring in virtuous dynamics. In this respect, for instance, Finkel and Geer (Reference Finkel and Geer1998) argue that negative messages when reporting important and relevant information are able to catch the citizens' attention contributing to stimulate their mobilization. Goldstein and Freedman (Reference Goldstein and Freedman2002) come to similar conclusions. Also, Wattenberg and Brians (Reference Wattenberg and Brians1999) posit that the intent of negative campaigning is not to demobilize the people, but rather to change their minds. Negativity, in other words, does not keep away from the polling station the supporters of a candidate, but rather it results to change and shifts their support.

Although findings are controversial, they all agree that negativity does not play alone, as several factors may moderate the effect of media negativity on citizens' political behavior. In this respect, Min (Reference Min2004) points out that both individual and contextual factors may shape how individuals receive media inputs, and, in turn, the media impact could be less direct than how scholars are used to positing.

Accordingly, in this article, we focused on the contextual dimension, by considering the type of electoral context (whether first- or second-order election).

First, we test the effects of negative coverage on turnout; our analyses reveal that increasing levels of negativity exposure appear to boost turnout, in particular at the expenses of undecided voters, and do not affect non-voters. Put in other terms, negativity increases the certainty to vote, while it does not impinge those who are sure to desert the vote. As regards the type of election and, more specifically, whether the salience of the election makes citizens more sensitive to negativity, we did not find clear differences between first- and second-order elections.

This study has some limitations. They pertain mainly to the research design, and the different methodological setting applied in 2018 and 2019 for the individual data collections. Due to the organization per waves of the CAWI survey in 2019, and for harmonization necessities, we were forced to re-design also the 2018 dataset. This entailed a loss in data detail since we had to avoid employing the daily data that the RCS design of 2018 provided. An additional element that represents a limitation of the paper is the fact that it focuses on the influence that TV newscast and newspaper have on voters, by neglecting other external elements, such as, for instance, interpersonal communication (such as agreement/disagreement in one's political network) or strictly online news outlets. Unfortunately, comparing first- and second-order elections led us to choose a relatively small subset of variables that were present in both datasets. For this reason, it was for us impossible to test possible alternative explanations related to these issues.

Summarizing, our study found that negativity in Italy, a largely polarized environment, is able to boost turnout, and in particular leads people to be more certain to go to the polls. On the other side, negativity does not affect the likelihood of abstaining. This difference is particularly relevant because it suggests that exposure to negative information affects voters who have some chances to be mobilized (namely, undecided voters). On the other side, our results show that media negativity does not spur people to go to the polls if they are already sure to abstain. These results are in line with the literature arguing that political media coverage has a positive – rather than negative – effect on turnout (i.e. De Vreese, Reference De Vreese2005; De Vreese and Boomgaarden, Reference De Vreese and Boomgaarden2006; Schuck et al., Reference Schuck, Boomgaarden and de Vreese2013; Schuck et al., Reference Schuck, Vliegenthart and De Vreese2016), disconfirming the idea of political cynicism induced by media negativity. While we can rely on the extensive literature on the effects of media negative frames on leader evaluation (i.e. Druckman and Perkin, Reference Druckman and Parkin2005; Hopmann et al., Reference Hopmann, Vliegenthart, De Vreese and Albæk2010; Ha, Reference Ha2011), few studies so far have addressed the turnout dimension (i.e. Min, Reference Min2004). Previous research focused in particular on voting behavior, rather than participation in elections. For example, Krupnikov (Reference Krupnikov2011) argues that media bias has a demobilizing effect when negativity focuses on the voter's preferred candidate. According to our results, indeed, a negative frame in media coverage is effective in mobilizing those who are not sure to go to the polls, while among those who are certain about their voting strategies (even to desert the election) no effects are found. Our research, thus, contributes to shed a light on undecided voters, and the mechanisms of media effects on their shift toward participation or abstain.

Furthermore, our analyses show that this mechanism is not dependent on the electoral context (namely, whether the election is a first- or second-order election). This latter result can be the combination of several factors: it could be due to the high level of polarization that Italy has experienced in the last years (which, in turn, tends to make an event of every election), by a contingent, strong, and violent style of communication that emerged in the last years, mainly engaged by populist entrepreneurs (see, for instance, Cremonesi et al., Reference Cremonesi, Bobba, Legnante, Mancosu, Roncarolo and Seddone2019), or by the very fact that, also for what concerns the communication styles and their effects, the differences between first- and second-order elections are becoming increasingly thinner. Further results (both employing a comparative and a longitudinal analysis) will be able to assess whether the invariance between electoral contexts shown here is an exceptional result or signals that some aspects of the second-order framework are becoming less crucial.

Funding

The research received no grants from public, commercial, or non-profit funding agency.

Data

The replication dataset is available at http://thedata.harvard.edu/dvn/dv/ipsr-risp

Footnotes

1 This is mainly why Elections present lower levels of participation compared to national elections (Reif and Schmitt, Reference Reif and Schmitt1980).

2 Negativity was operationalized through a categorical variable accounting for the overall tone of the news item. ‘Indications of negative tonality are the framing of the story as a political failure, fiasco, disaster, crisis, frustration, collapse, flop, denial, rejection, neglect, default, deterioration, resignation, skepticism, threats, cynicism, defeatism, or disappointment. Indications of positive tonality are depictions of political success, problem solutions, achievement, improvement, advance, prosperity, accomplishment, enthusiasm, hope, benefit, gain, sustainability, gratification, or accomplishment. If a report does not reflect indications of negative tonality or positive tonality, then it has to be coded as ‘neutral’. In case of the presence of both negative and positive frames, the tone was coded as ‘balanced/ambivalent’ (Lengauer et al., Reference Lengauer, Esser and Berganza2012: 195).

3 In this case, since the dataset does not provide variables measuring precisely the concept of political sophistication, we constructed the variable relying on a method already employed by Solvak and Vassil (Reference Solvak and Vassil2015) and, similarly, by Vegetti et al. (Reference Vegetti, Poletti and Segatti2014). They used the highest score on the ‘Propensity to vote’ (PTV) for five parties (a variable measured on a 0–10 scale, where 0 is ‘not at all likely’ and 10 is very likely (Franklin et al. Reference Franklin, Van der Eijk, Oppenhuis, Franklin and Van der Eijk1996). In order to solve the issues of skewness, the variable has been successively recoded into a dummy which is 0 when the highest PTV stops at 5 and is 1 when the highest PTV is 6 or more. A 0 for this variable indicates that the respondent does not consider any of the relevant parties as a good voting option. According to Solvak and Vassil (Reference Solvak and Vassil2015) ‘this low score […] would mean dissatisfaction with the party choice on the ballot’.

References

Ansolabehere, S and Iyengar, S (1995) Going Negative: How Political Advertisements Shrink and Polarize the Electorate. New York: The Free Press.Google Scholar
Ansolabehere, S, Iyengar, S, Simon, A and Valentino, N (1994) Does attack advertising demobilize the electorate? American Political Science Review 88, 829838.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Basil, M, Schooler, C and Reeves, B (1991) Positive and negative political advertising: Effectiveness of ads and perceptions of candidates. In Biocca, F (ed). Television and Political Advertising. Hillsdale: Erlbaum, pp. 245262.Google Scholar
Bellucci, P and Segatti, P (2011) Votare in Italia. Dall'appartenenza alla scelta. Bologna: il Mulino.Google Scholar
Bobba, G, Legnante, G, Roncarolo, F and Seddone, A (2013) Candidates in a negative light. The 2013 Italian election campaign in the media. Rivista Italiana di Scienza Politica 43(3), 353380.Google Scholar
Bradley, SD, Angelini, JR and Lee, S (2007) Psychophysiological and memory effects of negative political ads: aversive, arousing, and well remembered. Journal of Advertising 36, 115127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brooks, DJ (2006) The resilient voter: moving toward closure in the debate over negative campaigning and turnout. The Journal of Politics 68, 684696.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clinton, J and Lapinski, J (2004) ‘Targeted’ advertising and voter turnout: an experimental study of the 2000 presidential election. The Journal of Politics 66, 6996.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cremonesi, C, Bobba, G, Legnante, G, Mancosu, M, Roncarolo, F and Seddone, A (2019) Political information exposure and populist attitudes in the ‘laboratory of populism’. An exploratory analysis of the 2018 Italian general election campaign. Comunicazione Politica 20(1), 3962.Google Scholar
Decker, F (2002) Governance beyond the nation-state. Reflections on the democratic deficit of the European Union. Journal of European Public Policy 9, 256272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Vreese, CH (2005) The spiral of cynicism reconsidered. European Journal of Communication 20, 283301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Vreese, CH (2009) Second-rate election campaigning? An analysis of campaign styles in European parliamentary elections. Journal of Political Marketing 8, 719.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Vreese, CH and Boomgaarden, H (2006) News, political knowledge and participation: the differential effects of news media exposure on political knowledge and participation. Acta Politica 41, 317341.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Druckman, JN and Parkin, M (2005) The impact of media bias: how editorial slant affects voters. The Journal of Politics 67, 10301049.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Finkel, SE and Geer, JG (1998) A spot check: casting doubt on the demobilizing effect of attack advertising. American Journal of Political Science 42, 573595.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franklin, MN (2001) How structural factors cause turnout variations at European Parliament elections. European Union Politics 2, 309328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franklin, M and Hobolt, SB (2015) European elections and the European voter. In Richardson, J (ed.), European Union, Power and Policy Making. London: Routledge, pp. 399418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franklin, MN, Van der Eijk, C and Oppenhuis, E (1996) The institutional context: turnout. In Franklin, MN & Van der Eijk, C (eds), Choosing Europe? The European Electorate and National Politics in the Face of Union. Ann Arbor: The University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
Garramone, GM, Atkin, CT, Pinkleton, BE and Cole, RT (1990) Effects of negative political advertising on the political process. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media 34, 299311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geer, JG (2006) In Defense of Negativity: Attack Ads in Presidential Campaigns. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geer, JG (2012) The news media and the rise of negativity in presidential campaigns. PS: Political Science & Politics 45, 422427.Google Scholar
Germond, JW and Witcover, J (1996) Why the Americans don't go to the polls. National Journal 28, 25622562.Google Scholar
Goldstein, K and Freedman, P (2002) Campaign advertising and voter turnout: new evidence for a stimulation effect. Journal of Politics 64, 721740.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ha, S (2011) Attribute priming effects and presidential candidate evaluation: the conditionality of political sophistication. Mass Communication and Society 14, 315342.Google Scholar
Hansen, KM and Pedersen, RT (2008) Negative campaigning in a multiparty system. Scandinavian Political Studies 31, 408427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haselmayer, M, Meyer, TM and Wagner, M (2019) Fighting for attention: media coverage of negative campaign messages. Party Politics 25, 412423.Google Scholar
Hillygus, DS and Jackman, S (2003) Voter decision making in election 2000: campaign effects, partisan activation, and the Clinton legacy. American Journal of Political Science 47, 583596.Google Scholar
Hobolt, SB and Wittrock, J (2011) The second-order election model revisited: an experimental test of vote choices in European Parliament elections. Electoral Studies 30, 2940.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopmann, DN, Vliegenthart, R, De Vreese, C and Albæk, E (2010) Effects of election news coverage: how visibility and tone influence party choice. Political Communication 27, 389405.Google Scholar
Iyengar, S and Simon, A (2000) New perspectives and evidence on political communication and campaign effects. Annual Review of Psychology 51, 149169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jamieson, KH (1992) Dirty Politics: Deception, Distraction, and Democracy. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Johnston, R and Brady, HE (2002) The rolling cross-section design. Electoral Studies 21, 283295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahn, KF and Kenney, PJ (1999) Do negative campaigns mobilize or suppress turnout? Clarifying the relationship between negativity and participation. American Political Science Review 93, 877889.Google Scholar
Klapper, JT (1960) The Effects of Mass Communication. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Koopmans, R and Erbe, J (2004) Towards a European public sphere? Vertical and horizontal dimensions of Europeanized political communication. Innovation: The European Journal of Social Science Research 17, 97118.Google Scholar
Koopmans, R and Statham, P (eds) (2010) The Making of a European Public Sphere: Media Discourse and Political Contention. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kriesi, H (2016) The politicization of European integration. Journal of Common Market Studies 54, 3247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krupnikov, Y (2011) When does negativity demobilize? Tracing the conditional effect of negative campaigning on voter turnout. American Journal of Political Science 55, 797813.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ladini, R, Mancosu, M and Vezzoni, C (2020) Electoral participation, disagreement, and diversity in social networks: a matter of intimacy? Communication Research 47(7), 10561078.Google Scholar
Lau, R (1982) Negativity in person perception. Political Behavior 4, 343–77.Google Scholar
Lau, RR and Pomper, GM (2001) Effects of negative campaigning on turnout in U.S. Senate Elections, 1988–1998. Journal of Politics 63(3), 804–19.Google Scholar
Lau, RR and Pomper, GM (2004) Negative Campaigning: An Analysis of U.S. Senate Elections. Oxford: Rowman & Littlefield Publishers, Inc.Google Scholar
Lee, NY (2015) Two different motivations on agenda setting: need for orientation and motivated reasoning. International Journal of Public Opinion Research 28, 484510.Google Scholar
Lengauer, G, Esser, E and Berganza, R (2012) Negativity in political news: a review of concepts, operationalizations and key findings. Journalism 13, 179202.Google Scholar
Lodge, J (1996) The 1994 Elections to the European Parliament. London: Pinter Pub Ltd.Google Scholar
Long, S (1997) Regression Models for Categorical and Limited Dependent Variables. London: Sage.Google Scholar
Mancini, P (2013) The Italian public sphere: a case of dramatized polarization. Journal of Modern Italian Studies 18, 335347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, PS (2008) The mass media as sentinel: why bad news about issues is good news for participation. Political Communication 25, 180193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mattila, M (2003) Why bother? Determinants of turnout in the European elections. Electoral Studies 22, 449468.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Min, Y (2004) News coverage of negative political campaigns: an experiment of negative campaign effects on turnout and candidate preference. Harvard International Journal of Press/Politics 9, 95111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mutz, DC and Reeves, B (2005) The new videomalaise: effects of televised incivility on political trust. American Political Science Review 99, 115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norris, P and Reif, K (1997) Second-order elections. European Journal of Political Research 31, 109124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ohme, J, de Vreese, CH and Albaek, E (2018) The uncertain first-time voter: effects of political media exposure on young citizens’ formation of vote choice in a digital media environment. New Media & Society 20, 32433265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pedersen, RT (2014) News media framing of negative campaigning. Mass Communication and Society 17, 898919.Google Scholar
Petithomme, M (2012) Second-order elections, but also ‘low-cost’ campaigns? National parties and campaign spending in European elections: a comparative analysis. Perspectives on European Politics and Society 13, 149168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pfetsch, B, Adam, S and Eschner, B (2008) The contribution of the press to Europeanization of public debates: a comparative study of issue salience and conflict lines of European integration. Journalism 9, 465492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Redlawsk, DP (2002) Hot cognition or cool consideration? Testing the effects of motivated reasoning on political decision making. The Journal of Politics 64, 10211044.Google Scholar
Reeves, BR, Newhagen, J, Maibach, E, Basil, M and Kurz, K (1991) Negative and positive television messages: effects of message type and context on attention and memory. American Behavioral Scientist 34, 679694.Google Scholar
Reif, K and Schmitt, H (1980) Nine second-order national elections – a conceptual framework for the analysis of European Election results. European Journal of Political Research 8, 344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, M, Wanta, W and Dzwo, TH (2002) Agenda setting and issue salience online. Communication Research 29, 452465.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sani, G and Sartori, G (1982) Polarization, Fragmentation and Competition in Western Democracies. In Daalder, H and Mair, P (eds), Western European Party Systems. Beverly Hills: Sage, pp. 307340.Google Scholar
Schuck, AR, Azrout, R, Boomgaarden, H, Elenbaas, M, van Spanje, J, Vliegenthart, R and De Vreese, C (2011) Media visibility and framing of the European Parliamentary Elections 2009: a media content analysis in 27 countries. In Maier, M, Strömbäck, J and Kaid, LL (eds), Political Communication in European Parliamentary Elections. Farnham: Ashgate Publishing, pp. 175196.Google Scholar
Schuck, AR, Boomgaarden, HG and de Vreese, CH (2013) Cynics all around? The impact of election news on political cynicism in comparative perspective. Journal of Communication 63, 287311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schuck, AR, Vliegenthart, R and De Vreese, CH (2016) Who's afraid of conflict? The mobilizing effect of conflict framing in campaign news. British Journal of Political Science 46, 177194.Google Scholar
Slater, MD (2004) Operationalizing and analyzing exposure: the foundation of media effects research. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly 81, 168183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Solvak, M and Vassil, K (2015) Indifference or indignation? Explaining purposive vote spoiling in elections. Journal of Elections, Public Opinion and Parties 25, 463481.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Spanje, J and de Vreese, C (2014) Europhile media and Eurosceptic voting: effects of news media coverage on Eurosceptic voting in the 2009 European parliamentary elections. Political Communication 31, 325354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vegetti, F, Poletti, M and Segatti, P (2014) Availability or disengagement? How Italian citizens reacted to the two-faced parliamentary grand coalition supporting the Monti government. Polis 28, 6184.Google Scholar
Walter, AS and Vliegenthart, R (2010) Negative campaigning across different communication channels: different ballgames? International Journal of Press/Politics 15, 441461.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wattenberg, MP and Brians, CL (1999) Negative campaign advertising: demobilizer or mobilizer? American Political Science Review 93, 891899.Google Scholar
West, D (2009) AirWars: Television Advertising in Election Campaigns, 1952–2008. Washington: CQ Press.Google Scholar
Zaller, JR (1992) The Nature and Origins of Mass Opinion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Two multinomial logistic models studying turnout in two Italian elections

Figure 1

Figure 1. Predicted probabilities of voting, being undecided, and non-voting at different levels of media negativity.

Figure 2

Figure 2. Average marginal effects of negativity on voting, being undecided, and non-voting at different electoral contexts.

Supplementary material: Link

Mancosu et al. Dataset

Link