Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-ttngx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-14T07:02:47.955Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Children's knowledge of the presuppositions of know and other cognitive verbs*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Leonard Abbeduto
Affiliation:
University of Illinois at Chicago
Sheldon Rosenberg
Affiliation:
University of Illinois at Chicago

Abstract

This study investigated the development of knowledge about the presuppositions of cognitive verbs that take sentential complements. The verbs included factives, which presuppose the truth of their complements, and nonfactives, which carry no such presupposition. Three tasks assessed children's ability to (a) assign truth values to complements according to the presuppositions of the main verbs; (b) select verbs to describe people's mental states; and (c) state the presuppositions of the verbs in definitions. The results indicated that the presuppositions of the factives know, forget, and remember and the nonfactive think are not learned until age 4. Believe, which has factive and nonfactive properties, is mastered after age 7. The children's performance differed across tasks due to variations in processing requirements.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1985

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Blank, M. (1975). Eliciting verbalizations from young children in experimental tasks: a methodological note. ChDev 46. 254–7.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1979). The aquisition of complex sentences. In Fletcher, P. & Garman., M. (eds), Language acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Glucksberg, S., Krauss, R. & Higgins, E. T. (1975). The development of referential communication skills. In Horowitz, F. D. (ed.), Review of child development research, Vol. 4. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Greenhouse, S. W. & Geisser, S. (1959). On methods in the analysis of profile data. Psychometrika 24. 95112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, R. (1975). Children's comprehension of complex sentences. JExpChPsychol 19. 42O–33.Google Scholar
Hidi, S. & Hildyard, A. (1979). Four-year-olds' understanding of pretend and forget: no evidence for propositional reasoning. JChLang 6. 493510.Google ScholarPubMed
Hopmann, M. R. & Maratsos, M. P. (1978). A developmental study of factivity and negation in complex syntax. JChLang 5. 295309.Google Scholar
Johnson, C. N. (1982). Acquisition of mental verbs and the concept of mind. In Kuczaj., S. (ed.), Language development. Vol. 1. Syntax and semantics. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Johnson, C. N. & Maratsos, M. P. (1977). Early comprehension of mental verbs: think and know. ChDev 48. 1743–7.Google Scholar
Johnson, C. N. & Wellman, H. M. (1980). Children's developing understanding of mental verbs: ‘remember’, ‘know’, and ‘guess’. ChDev 51. 10951102.Google Scholar
Karttunen, L. (1973). Presuppositions of compound sentences. LI 4. 169–73.Google Scholar
Kempson, R. (1975). Presupposition and the delimitation of semantics. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Kiparsky, P. & Kiparsky, C. (1970). Fact. In Bierwisch, M. & Heidolph., K. (eds), Progress in linguistics. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Lehrer, A. (1974). Semantic fields and lexical structure. Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Levy, K. J. & Narula, S. C. (1976). An empirical comparison of several methods for testing the quality of dependent proportions. Communications in Statistics: Simulation and Computation 5. 189–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Limber, J. (1973). The genesis of complex sentences. In Moore, T. E. (ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Litowitz, B. (1977). Learning to make definitions. JChLang 4. 289304.Google Scholar
Macnamara, J., Baker, E. & Olson, C. (1976). Four-year-olds' understanding of pretend, forget and know: evidence for propositional operations. ChDev 47. 6270.Google Scholar
Miscione, J. L., Marvin, R. S.O'Brien, R. G. & Greenberg, M. T. (1978). A developmental study of children's understanding of the words ‘know’ and ‘guess’. ChDev 49. 1107–13.Google Scholar
Myers, J. L., DiCecco, J. V., White, J. B. & Borden, V. M. (1982). Repeated measurements on dichotomous variables: Q and F tests. PsycholBull 92. 517–25.Google Scholar
Rosenberg, S. (1974). Linguistic maturity and language development in elementary school-age children. A report prepared for Computer Psychometric Affiliates, Inc., Chicago, Illinois.Google Scholar
Scoville, R. P. & Gordon, A. M. (1980). Children's understanding of factive presuppositions: an experiment and a review. JChLang 7. 381–99.Google ScholarPubMed
Seeger, P. & Gabrielson, A. (1968). Applicability of the Cochran Q test and the F test for statistical analysis of dichotomous data for dependent samples. PsycholBull 69. 269–71.Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M. & Johnson, C. N. (1979). Understanding mental processes: a developmental study of ‘remember’ and ‘forget’. ChDev 50. 7988.Google Scholar