Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-ndmmz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-15T19:39:38.862Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Uses of past forms in the social pretend play of Italian children*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Tullia Musatti*
Affiliation:
Consiglio Nazionale Ricerche, Rome
Margherita Orsolini
Affiliation:
Università D'Annunzio, Chieti
*
Address for correspondence: Istituto di Psicologia, Consiglio Nazionale Ricerche, Rome, Italy.

Abstract

In different languages children have been found to use past verb forms to express the meaning of ‘unreality’ during social pretend play. In this study, the verb forms used by 14 four-year-old Italian children in their pretend play were investigated. Results show that different Italian past verb forms tend to co-occur with different play activities. The imperfect occurs mainly when children plan and negotiate their pretend activities and marks the transition from a real to a pretend frame of reference. The present perfect occurs mainly when children implement their plans and communicate within a pretend frame that has already been established. Results of this study are discussed in comparison with findings on the pretend language of children speaking other languages.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1993

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

We are grateful to Catherine Garvey who suggested that we should study children's pretend language and supported us with her advice and comments throughout the study. We also wish to thank Hermine Sinclair for many insightful suggestions and comments.

References

REFERENCES

Antinucci, F. & Miller, R. (1976). How children talk about what happened. Journal of Child Language 3, 167–89.Google Scholar
Auwarter, M. (1986). Development of communicative skills: the construction of fictional reality in children's play. In Cook-Gumperz, J., Corsaro, W. & Streeck, J. (eds), Childrens' worlds and children's language. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Bazzanella, C. (1990). ‘Modal’ uses of the Italian indicativo imperfetto in a pragmatic perspective. Journal of Pragmatics 14, 439–57.Google Scholar
Bertinetto, P. M. (1986). Tempo, aspetto e azione nel verbo Italiano. Il sistema dell'indicativo. Firenze: Accademia delia Crusca.Google Scholar
Field, T., De Stefano, L. & Koewler, J. (1982). Fantasy play of toddlers and preschoolers. Developmental Psychology 18, 503–8.Google Scholar
Forbes, D., Kats, M. & Paul, B. (1986). ‘Frame talk’: a dramatistic analysis of children's fantasy play. In Mueller, E. & Cooper, C. (eds), Process and outcome in peer relations. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Garvey, C. (1974). Some properties of social play. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 20, 163–80.Google Scholar
Garvey, C. & Berndt, R. (1977). The organization of pretend play. JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology 2 (MS 1589).Google Scholar
Garvey, C. & Kramer, T. L. (1989). The language of social pretend play. Developmental Review 9, 364–82.Google Scholar
Gee, J. & Savasir, I. (1985). On the use of will and gonna: toward a description of activitytypes for child language. Discourse Processes 8, 143–75.Google Scholar
Giffin, H. (1984). The coordination of meaning in the creation of a shared make-believe reality. In Bretherton, I. (ed.), Symbolic play: the development of social understanding. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Goncu, A. & Kessel, F. (1984). Children's play: a contextual-functional perspective. In Kessel, F. & Goncu, A. (eds), Analyzing children's play dialogues. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Grégoire, A. (1947). L'apprentissage du language, vol. II. Liège: Bibliothèque de la Faculté de Philosophie et Lettres.Google Scholar
James, D. (1982). Past tense and the hypothetical. A cross-linguistic study. Studies in language 6, 375403.Google Scholar
Kaper, W. (1980). The use of the past tense in games of pretend. Journal of Child Language 7, 213–15.Google Scholar
Lepschy, G. (1978). Saggi di linguistica Italiana. Bologna: Zanichelli.Google Scholar
Lodge, K. R. (1979). The use of the past tense in games of pretend. Journal of Child Language 6, 365–9.Google Scholar
McTear, M. (1985). Children's conversation. New York: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Musatti, T. (1983). Echanges dans une situation de jeux de faire semblant. In Stambak, M., Barrière, M., Bonica, L., Maisonnet, R., Musatti, T., Rayna, S. & Verba, M. Les bébés entre eux: inventer, decouvrir et jouer ensemble. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.Google Scholar
Orsolini, M. & Musatti, T. (1985). Lo sviluppo dell'argomento nel gioco simbolico tra bambini in età prescolare. Giornale Italiano di Psicologia 2, 235–57.Google Scholar
Pohl, J. (1967). Imparfait et indiens. Témoignage sur la fiction dans le langage de l'enfant. Le langage et l'homme 5, 127–36.Google Scholar
Sachs, J., Goldman, J. & Chaillé, C. (1983). Planning in pretend play: using language to coordinate narrative development. In Pellegrini, A. & Yawkey, T. (eds), The development of oral and written language in social contexts. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Van Gessel-Hotcker, R. (1989). Comunication épiludique verbale dans le jeu de fiction chez des enfants entre 3 et 5 ans en interaction spontanée dans la crèche. Thèse de doctorat, Université de Genève.Google Scholar
Warnant, L. (1966). ‘Moi, j'étais le papa’. L'imparfait préludique et quelques remarques relatives à la recherche grammaticale. In Mélanges de grammaire française offerts à M. Grévisse. Gembloux: Duculot.Google Scholar
Weist, R. M. (1986). Tense and aspect. In Fletcher, P. & Garman, M. (eds), Language acquisition (2nd ed). Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Wolf, D., Goldfield, B., Beeghly, M. & Cardona, L. (1985). ‘And then the baby said, There's not enough room’: the emergence of multiple discourse strands within play texts. Paper presented at the Boston University Child Language Conference.Google Scholar