Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-mwx4w Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-14T23:55:20.724Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

What is Psychophysics?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 February 2022

Lawrence A. Shapiro*
Affiliation:
University of Wisconsin, Madison

Extract

Gustav Fechner published his monumental Elemente der Psychophysik in 1860. In the years to follow the book became the object of such intense criticism and such caustic invective that Fechner, prophetically, observed: “The tower of Babel was never finished because the workers could not reach an understanding on how they should build it; my psychophysical edifice will stand because the workers will never agree on how to tear it down” (Stevens 1957, 153). Fechner was right. Here we are, 134 years later, and psychophysics is still with us. Moreover, if it's true that psychophysics thrives on disagreement then likely it will be around for another millennium, for there is currently even less accord about what psychophysics is and whether it is possible than there was when Fechner was alive. Some psychophysicists retain Fechner's view of psychophysics and so claim to be searching for laws relating our sensory experience of the world to physical magnitudes in the world.

Type
Part II. Philosophy of Psychology as Philosophy of Science
Copyright
Copyright © 1995 by the Philosophy of Science Association

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

1

For helpful comments on earlier drafts of this paper I am grateful to Malcolm Forster, Gary Hatfield, Eric Saidel, and Elliott Sober.

References

Barlow, H. (1982), “General Principles: The Senses Considered as Physical Instruments”, in Barlow, H. and Mollon, J., (eds.), The Senses. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 132.Google Scholar
Boring, E. (1921), “The Stimulus Error”, American Journal of Psychology 32: 449471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boring, E. (1928), “Did Fechner Measure Sensation?”, Psychological Review 35: 443445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boring, E. (1950), A History of Experimental Psychology, 2nd ed. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Fechner, G. (1860), Elemente Der Psychophysik, Vols. I, II. Leipzig: Breitkopf und Hartel.Google Scholar
Graham, C. (1952), “Behavior and the Psychophysical Methods: An Analysis of Some Recent Experiments”, Psychological Review 59: 6270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gundlach, H. (1993), “Psychophysics, Its History and Ontology”, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16: 144145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hornstein, G. (1993), “The Chimera of Psychological Measurement”, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16: 148149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luce, R. and Edwards, W. (1958), “The Derivation of Subjective Scales from Just Noticeable Differences”, Psychological Review 65: 222237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McKenna, F. (1985), “Another Look at the ‘New Psychophysics’”, British Journal of Psychology 76: 97109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Savage, C. (1970), The Measurement of Sensation. Berkeley: The University of California Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevens, S. (1957), “On the Psychophysical Power Law”, Psychological Review 64: 153181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevens, S. (1966), “Operations or Words?: A Reply to SavagePsychological Monographs 80: 3338.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Titchener, E. (1905), Experimental Psychology, Vol. II, pt. 2. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Treisman, M. (1964), “Sensory Scaling and the Psychophysical Law”, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 16: 1121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar