Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-ndmmz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-17T03:17:28.165Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Evolution of Human Uniqueness

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 January 2017

Robert Boyd*
Affiliation:
Arizona State University (USA)
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Robert Boyd. School of Human Evolution and Social Change. Arizona State University. Arizona (USA). E-mail: Robert.Boyd.1@asu.edu

Abstract

The human species is an outlier in the natural world. Two million years ago our ancestors were a slightly odd apes. Now we occupy the largest ecological and geographical range of any species, have larger biomass, and process more energy. Usually, this transformation is explained in terms of cognitive ability—people are just smarter than all the rest. In this paper I argue that culture, our ability to learn from each other, and cooperation, our ability to make common cause with large groups of unrelated individuals are the real roots of human uniqueness, and sketch an evolutionary account of how these crucial abilities co-evolved with each other and with other features of our life histories.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos de Madrid 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alba, R., & Nee, V. (2003). Remaking the American mainstream: Assimilation and the New Immigration. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Appiah, K. A. (2010). The honor code: How moral revolutions happen. New York, NY: WW Norton.Google Scholar
Barrett, C., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2007). The hominid entry into the cognitive niche. In Gangestad, S. & Simpson, J. (Eds.), Evolution of mind, fundamental questions and controversies. (pp. 241248). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Barton, N. H., & Rouhani, S. (1993). Adaptation and the shifting balance. Genetical Research, 61, 5774. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0016672300031098 Google Scholar
Beard, M. (2015). SPQR. A Story of Ancient Rome. London, UK: Profile books.Google Scholar
Boomsma, J. J., Beekman, M., Cornwallis, C. K., Griffin, A. S., Holman, L., Hughes, W. O. H., … Ratnieks, F. L. W. (2011). Only full sibling families evolved eusociality. Nature, 471, 45. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature09832 Google Scholar
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1987). The evolution of social learning: The effects of spatial and temporal variation. In Zentall, T. R. & Galef, B. G. (Eds.), Social learning: Psychological and biological perspectives. New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1995). Why does culture increase human adaptability? Ethology and Sociobiology, 16, 125143. https://doi.org/10.1016/0162-3095(94)00073-G Google Scholar
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1996). Why culture is common but cultural evolution is rare. Proceedings of the British Academy, 88, 7393.Google Scholar
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J., (2002). Group beneficial norms spread rapidly in a structured population. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 215, 287296. https://doi.org/10.1006/jtbi.2001.2515 Google Scholar
Boyd, R., Schonmann, R. H., & Vicente, R. (2014). Hunter-Gatherer population structure and the evolution of contingent cooperation. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35, 219227. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2014.02.002 Google Scholar
Cathcart, M. (2014). Starvation in a land of plenty: Will’s diary of the fateful burke and wills expedition. Sidney, Australia: Newsouth books.Google Scholar
Collias, E., & Collias, N. (1964). The development of nest-building behavior in a weaverbird. The Auk, 81, 4252. https://doi.org/10.2307/4082609 Google Scholar
Cronk, L. (2002). From true Dorobo to Mukogodo Maasai: Contested ethnicity in Kenya. Ethnology, 41, 2749. https://doi.org/10.2307/4153019 Google Scholar
Darwent, J., & Darwent, C. M., (2014). Scales of violence across the North American arctic. In Allen, M. W. & Jones, T. L. (Eds.), Violence and warfare among hunter-gatherers. (pp. 149167). Walnut Creek, CA: Left Coast Press.Google Scholar
Fry, D. (2006). The human potential for peace: An anthropological challenge to assumptions about war and violence. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gat, A. (2015). Proving communal warfare among hunter-gatherers: The quasi-rousseauan error. Evolutionary Anthropology, 24, 111126. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.21446 Google Scholar
Gould, J. L., & Gould, C. G. (2007). Animal architects: Building and the evolution of intelligence. New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Guala, F. (2012). Reciprocity: Weak or strong? What punishment experiments do (and do not) demonstrate. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 159. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X11000069 Google Scholar
Hämäläinen, P. (2008). The Comanche empire. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Hansell, M. (2005). Animal architecture. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hart, C. W. M., & Pilling, A. R. (1960). The Tiwi of North Australia. New York, NY: Holt Rinehart Winston.Google Scholar
Henrich, J., & Gil-White, F. J. (2001). The evolution of prestige–Freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22, 165196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henrich, J., & Henrich, N. (2010). The evolution of cultural adaptations: Fijian food taboos protect against dangerous marine toxins. Proceedings of the Royal Society (B), 277, 37153724. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.1191 Google Scholar
Hill, K. R., Walker, R. S., Božičević, M., Eder, J., Headland, T., Hewlett, B., … Wood, B. (2011). Co-residence patterns in hunter-gatherer societies show unique human social structure. Science, 331, 286289. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1199071 Google Scholar
Hill, K. R., Wood, B. M., Baggio, J., Hurtado, A. M., & Boyd, R. (2014). Hunter-gatherer inter-band interaction rates: Implications for cumulative culture. PLoS One, 9, e102806. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0102806 Google Scholar
Kaplan, H., Hill, K., Lancaster, J., & Hurtado, A. M. (2000). A theory of human life history evolution: Diet, intelligennce, and longevity. Evolutionary Anthropology, 9, 156185. https://doi.org/10.1002/1520-6505(2000)9:4%3C156::AID-EVAN5%3E3.3.CO;2-Z Google Scholar
Kelly, R. (1985). The nuer conquest. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
Klein, R. (2009). The human career (3 rd Ed.). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Marlowe, F. (2009). Hadza cooperation, second party punishment, yes; third party punishment, no. Human Nature, 20, 417430.Google Scholar
Martin, P. (2005). Migrants in the global labor market (pp. 159). Davis, CA: Global Commission on International Migration. Retrieved from http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.460.6744&rep=rep1&type=pdf Google Scholar
Mathew, S., & Boyd, R. (2011). Punishment sustains large-scale cooperation in prestate warfare. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 1137511380. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1105604108 Google Scholar
Mathew, S., & Boyd, R. (2014). The cost of cowardice. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35, 5864.Google Scholar
McElreath, R., Bell, A. V., Efferson, C., Lubell, M., Richerson, P. J., & Waring, T. (2008). Beyond existence and aiming outside the laboratory: Estimating frequency-dependent and payoff-biased social learning strategies. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 363, 35153528. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0131 Google Scholar
Perreault, C., Moya, C., & Boyd, R. (2012). A Bayesian approach to the evolution of social learning. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33, 449459. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.12.007 Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (2010). The cognitive niche: Coevolution of intelligence, sociality, and language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 88938999.Google Scholar
Richerson, P. J., Bell, A., Demps, K., Frost, K., Hillis, V., Matthew, S., … Zefferman, M. (in press). Behavioural and Brain Sciences.Google Scholar
Schonmann, R. H., & Boyd, R. (2016). A simple rule for the evolution of contingent cooperation in large groups. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society (B), 371, 1687. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0099 Google Scholar
Smith, B. D. (2011). General patterns of niche construction and the management of ‘wild’ plant and animal resources by small-scale pre-industrial societies. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society (B), 366, 836848.Google Scholar
Smith, M. W. (1938). The war complex of the Plains Indians. Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society, 78, 425464.Google Scholar
Stark, R. (1997). The rise of Christianity: How the obscure, marginal Jesus movement became the dominant religious force in the Western world in a few centuries. San Francisco, CA: Harper/Collins.Google Scholar
Sutton, M. Q. (2014). Warfare and expansion: An ethnohistoric perspective on the Numic spread. In Allen, M. W. & Jones, T. L. (Eds.), Violence and warfare among hunter-gatherers. (pp. 149167). Walnut Creek, CA: Left Coast Press.Google Scholar
Swezey, S. L., & Heizer, R. H. (1977). Ritual management of salmonid fish resources in California. The Journal of California Anthropology, 4, 429.Google Scholar
Tooby, J., & DeVore, I. (1987). The cognitive niche. In Kinzey, W. (Ed.), Primate models of hominid behavior. (pp. 183237). New York, NY: SUNY Press.Google Scholar
Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 46, 3557. https://doi.org/10.1086/406755 Google Scholar
Wilke, P. J. (2013). The Whisky Flat pronghorn trap complex, Mineral County, Nevada, Western United States: Preliminary report. Quaternary International, 297, 7992. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.quaint.2013.01.018 Google Scholar
Young, P. (2001). Individual strategy and social structure: An evolutionary theory of institutions. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar