Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-vfjqv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-29T02:21:12.822Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Psychopathology of Cognitive Deficit

The Blake Marsh Lecture for 1975, delivered before the Royal College of Psychiatrists, 3 February 1975

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 January 2018

Neil O'Connor*
Affiliation:
Drayton House, Gordon Street, WC1H 0AN

Extract

Binet created the psychology of mental deficiency and incidentally the concept of intelligence which for many years has been a central issue in both psychology and education. He was primarily concerned with learning difficulties in children and it is a wry paradox that his development of the notion of intelligence has proved such a barrier to the study of the learning process. Intelligence for Binet was a summary term which stood in place of all the component elements of learning weakness which he noticed and assessed. His successors such as Spearman (1923), Burt (1937), Vernon (1950) and Jensen (1975) tend to use the term in an explanatory fashion although it is doubtful whether ‘intelligence’ explains ‘mental age’ or ‘mental deficiency’ except as a descriptive cross-reference.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 1976 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Attneave, F. & Benson, L. (1969) Spatial coding of tactual stimulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 216–22.Google Scholar
Burt, C. (1937) The Backward Child. London: University of London Press.Google Scholar
Clarke, A. D. B. & Blakemore, C. B. (1961) Age and perceptual motor transfer in imbeciles. British Journal of Psychology, 52, 125–31.Google Scholar
Clausen, J. (1966) Ability Structure and Subgroups in Mental Retardation. London: Macmillan & Co.Google Scholar
Creak, M. (1961) Schizophrenic syndrome in childhood: progress report of a working party. April 1961. Cerebral Palsy Bulletin, 3, 501–4.Google Scholar
Ellis, N. R. (1963) The stimulus trace and behavioral inadequacy. In Handbook of Mental Deficiency (ed. Ellis, N. R.). New York: McGraw Hill.Google Scholar
Furth, H. G. (1966) Thinking Without Language: Psychological Implications of Deafness. Canada: Collier-Macmillan.Google Scholar
Hebb, D. O. (1949) The Organization of Behavior. London: Chapman & Hall.Google Scholar
Held, R. (1961) Exposure history as a factor in maintaining stability of perception and coordination. Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases, 132, 2632.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hermelin, B., & O'Connor, N. (1971) Spatial coding in normal, autistic and blind children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 33, 127–32.Google Scholar
Hermelin, B., & O'Connor, N. (1975a) Location and distance estimates by blind and sighted children. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27, 224–30.Google Scholar
Hermelin, B., & O'Connor, N. (1975b) The recall of digits by normal, deaf and autistic children. British Journal of Psychology (to appear).Google Scholar
House, B. J. & Zeaman, D. (1958) Visual discrimination learning in imbeciles. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 63, 447–52.Google ScholarPubMed
Jensen, A. R. (1973) Educability and Group Differences. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Luria, A. R. (1961) The Role of Speech in the Regulation of Normal and Abnormal Behavior. Oxford: Pergamon.Google Scholar
Lyle, J. G. (1959) The effect of institution environment upon verbal development of imbecile children: 1. Verbal intelligence. Journal of Mental Deficiency Research, 3, 122–8.Google Scholar
Mackinney, J. P. (1964) Hand schema in children. Psychonomic Science, 1, 99100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Connor, N. & Hermelin, B. (1963) Speech and Thought in Severe Subnormality. Oxford: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
O'Connor, N. & Hermelin, B. (1971) Inter- and intra-modal transfer in children with modality-specific and general handicaps. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 10 346–54.Google Scholar
O'Connor, N. & Hermelin, B. (1973) The spatial or temporal organization of short-term memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 25, 335–43.Google Scholar
O'Connor, N. & Hermelin, B. (1975) Modality-Specific spatial Coordinates in Perception. Psychophysics. 17, 213–16.Google Scholar
O'Connor, N. & Hermelin, B. (1976) Backward and forward recall by deaf and hearing children (to appear).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riesen, A. H. (1947) The development of visual perception in man and chimpanzees. Science, 106, 107–8.Google Scholar
Senden, M. (1932) Raum und Gestaltauffassung bei operierten Blindgeborenen vor und nach der Operation. Leipzig: Barth.Google Scholar
Spearman, C. (1923) The Nature of Intelligence and the Principles of Cognition. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Spitz, H. H. (1963) Field theory in mental deficiency. In Handbook of Mental Deficiency (ed. Ellis, N. R.). New York: McGraw Hill.Google Scholar
Vernon, P. E. (1950) The Structure of Human Abilities. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1962) Thought and Language. Massachusetts: Institute of Technology & Wiley.Google Scholar
Woodward, M. (1963) The application of Piaget's theory to research in mental deficiency. In Handbook of Mental Deficiency (ed. Ellis, N. R.). New York: McGraw Hill.Google Scholar
Wundt, W. (1874) Greindzeige der physiologischen Psychologie. Leipzig: Wilhelm Engelmann.Google Scholar
Zeaman, D. & House, B. J. (1963) The role of attention in retardate discrimination learning. In Handbook of Mental Deficiency (ed. Ellis, N. R.). New York: McGraw Hill.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.