Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-m9kch Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-21T06:41:32.325Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Ability of the D-15 panel tests and HRR pseudoisochromatic plates to predict performance in naming VDT colors

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 April 2005

SHANKARAN RAMASWAMY
Affiliation:
School of Optometry, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario N2L 3G1, Canada
JEFFERY K. HOVIS
Affiliation:
School of Optometry, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario N2L 3G1, Canada

Abstract

Color codes in VDT displays often contain sets of colors that are confusing to individuals with color-vision deficiencies. The purpose of this study is to determine whether individuals with color-vision deficiencies (color defectives) can perform as well as individuals without color-vision deficiencies (color normals) on a colored VDT display used in the railway industry and to determine whether clinical color-vision tests can predict their performance. Of the 52 color defectives, 58% failed the VDT test. The kappa coefficients of agreement for the Farnsworth D-15, Adams desaturated D-15, and Richmond 3rd Edition HRR PIC diagnostic plates were significantly greater than chance. In particular, the D-15 tests have a high probability of predicting who fails the practical test. However, all three tests had an unacceptably high false-negative rate (9.5–35%); so that a practical test is still needed.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2004 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Cole, B. & Orenstein, J. (2003). Does the Farnsworth D-15 test predict the ability to name colors? Clinical and Experimental Optometry 86, 221229.Google Scholar
Farnsworth, D. (1947). The Farnsworth Dichotomous Test for Color Blindness Panel D-15. New York: The Psychological Corp.
Kuyk, T.K., Veres, J.G., Lahey, M.A., & Clark, D.J. (1987). Ability of deutan color defectives to perform simulated air traffic control tasks. American Journal of Optometry and Physiological Optics 64, 210.Google Scholar
Mahon, L.E. & Jacobs, R.J. (1991). Electronic flight information system displays and colour defective observers. Clinical and Experimental Optometry 74, 196203.Google Scholar
Margrain, T.H., Birch, J., & Owen, C.G. (1996). Color vision requirements of firefighters. Occupational Medicine 46, 114124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siu, A.W. & Yap, M. (2003). The performance of color deficient individuals on airfield color tasks. Aviation Space and Environmental Medicine 74, 546550.Google Scholar
Voke, J. (1980). Color vision testing in specific industries and professions. In Color Vision Testing and Their Occupational Suitability, ed. Voke & J., Chapter 5, pp. 3335. London: Keeler Academic House.
Walraven, P.L. & Leebeek, H.L. (1960). Recognition of color code by normals and color defectives at several illumination levels. An evaluation study of the H.R.R. plates. American Journal of Optometry and Archives of American Academy of Optometry 37, 8292.Google Scholar