Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-sxzjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T16:43:33.764Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A New Look at Cardiac Defense: Attention or Emotion?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 April 2014

Jaime Vila*
Affiliation:
University of Granada
María Carmen Fernández
Affiliation:
University of Granada
Joaquín Pegalajar
Affiliation:
University of Granada
María Nieves Vera
Affiliation:
University of Granada
Humbelina Robles
Affiliation:
University of Granada
Nieves Pérez
Affiliation:
University of Granada
María B. Sánchez
Affiliation:
University of Granada
Isabel Ramírez
Affiliation:
University of Granada
Elisabeth Ruiz-Padial
Affiliation:
University of Granada
*
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jaime Vila, Departamento de Personalidad de Evaluación y Tratamiento Psicológico, Facultad de Psicología, Campus La Cartuja s/n, 18011 Granada (Spain). E-mail: jvila@ugr.es

Abstract

The study of cardiac defense has a long tradition in psychological research both within the cognitive approach—linked to Pavlov, Sokolov, and Graham's work on sensory reflexes—and within the motivational one—linked to the work of Cannon and subsequent researchers on the concepts of activation and stress. These two approaches have been difficult to reconcile in the past. We summarize a series of studies on cardiac defense from a different perspective, which allows integration of the traditional approaches. This new perspective emphasizes a sequential process interpretation of the cardiac defense response. Results of descriptive and parametric studies, as well as those of studies examining the physiological and psychological mechanisms underlying the response, show a complex response pattern with both accelerative and decelerative components, with both sympathetic and parasympathetic influences, and with both attentional and emotional significance. The implications of this new look at cardiac defense are discussed in relation to defensive reactions in natural settings, the brain mechanisms controlling such reactions, and their effects on health and illness.

El estudio de la respuesta cardiaca de defensa tiene una larga tradición en la investigación psicológica, tanto desde la perspectiva cognitiva –vinculada a los trabajos de Pavlov, Sokolov y Graham sobre los reflejos sensoriales–, como desde la perspectiva motivacional –vinculada a los trabajos de Cannon e investigadores posteriores, sobre los conceptos de activación y estrés–. Estas dos perspectivas han sido difíciles de integrar en el pasado. El presente artículo resume una serie de estudios sobre la respuesta cardiaca de defensa, desde una perspectiva diferente, que permite integrar las dos aproximaciones tradicionales. La nueva perspectiva hace hincapié en una interpretación procesual y secuencial de la respuesta cardiaca de defensa. Los resultados de estudios descriptivos y paramétricos, junto con los de estudios que examinan los mecanismos fisiológicos y psicológicos subyacentes a la respuesta, ponen de manifiesto un patrón complejo de cambios cardiacos en los que aparecen componentes tanto acelerativos como desacelerativos, con influencias simpáticas y parasimpáticas, y con significación tanto atencional como emocional. Las implicaciones de esta nueva visión de la respuesta defensiva del corazón se discuten en relación con la defensa en contextos naturales, los mecanismos cerebrales que controlan tales reacciones y sus efectos sobre la salud y la enfermedad.

Type
Research trends
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2003

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Belkic, K. (1986). Light stress and the cardiovascular system: The glare pressor test. Ergonomics, 29, 563568.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blanchard, R. J., & Blanchard, D. C. (1989). Attack and defense in rodents as ethoexperimental models for the study of emotion. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 13, 314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bond, D. D. (1943). Sympathetic and vagal interaction in emotional responses of the heart rate. American Journal of Physiology, 138, 468478.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradley, M. M. (2000). Emotion and motivation. In Cacioppo, J. T., Tassinary, L. G., & Berntson, G. G. (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology (pp. 602642). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cannon, W. B. (1929). Bodily changes in pain, hunger, fear and rage. An account of researches into the function of emotional excitement. New York: Appleton Century Crofts.Google Scholar
Coles, M. G. H., Jennings, J. R., & Stern, J. A. (Eds.) (1984). Psychophysiological perspectives: Festschrift for Beatrice and John Lacey. New York: Reinhold.Google Scholar
Davis, M. (1996). Differential roles of the amygdala and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis in conditioned fear and startle enhanced by corticotropin-releasing hormone. In Ono, T., McNaughton, B. L., Molotchnikoff, S., Rolls, E. T., & Nishijo, H. (Eds.), Perception, memory and emotion: Frontiers in neuroscience (pp. 525548). Oxford: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Eves, F. F., & Gruzelier, J. M. (1984). Individual differences in the cardiac response to high intensity auditory stimulation. Psychophysiology, 21, 342352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eves, F. F., & Gruzelier, J. M. (1985). Individual differences in the cardiac response to novel stimuli. In Orlebeke, J. F., Mulder, G., & Van Doornen, L. J. P. (Eds.), Psychophysiology of cardiovascular control (pp. 637648). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Fanselow, M. S. (1994). Neural organization of the defense behavior system responsible for fear. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 1, 429438.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fendt, M., & Fanselow, M. S. (1999). The neuroanatomical and neurochemical basis of conditioned fear. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 23, 743760.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernández, M. C., & Vila, J. (1989a). Sympathetic-parasympathetic mediation of the cardiac defense response in humans. Biological Psychology, 28, 123133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernández, M. C., & Vila, J. (1989b). Cognitive versus motivational significance of the cardiac response to intense auditory stimulation. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 8, 4959.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
García León, A. (1997). Efectos de la hostilidad/ira sobre la reactividad cardiovascular en paradigmas tónicos y fásicos (La respuesta cardiaca de defensa). Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Granada, Spain.Google Scholar
García-León, A., Vila, J., Reyes del Paso, G., & Pérez, M. N. (2002). Efectos de la frustración y el hostigamiento en la modulación emocional de la respuesta cardiaca de defensa. Psicothema, 14, 450455.Google Scholar
Graham, F. K. (1979). Distinguishing among orienting, defense and startle reflexes. In Kimmel, H. D., van Olst, E. H., & Orlebeke, F. J. (Eds.), The orienting reflex in humans (pp. 137167). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hare, R. D. (1973). Orienting and defensive responses to visualstimuli. Psychophysiology, 10, 453464.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klorman, R., Weissbert, R. P., & Wiessenfeld, A. R. (1977). Individual differences in fear and autonomic reactions to affective stimulation. Psychophysiology, 14, 4551.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kryter, K. D. (1970). The effect of noise on man. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Lacey, B. C., & Lacey, J. I. (1974). Studies of heart rate and other bodily processes in sensoriomotor behavior. In Obrist, P. A., Black, A. H., Brener, J., & DiCara, L. V. (Eds.), Cardiovascular psychophysiology: Current issues in response mechanisms, biofeedback and methodology (pp. 358564). Chicago, IL: Aldine-Atherton.Google Scholar
Lacey, J. I., & Lacey, B. C. (1970). Some autonomic-central nervous system interrelationships. In Black, P. (Ed.), Physiological correlates of emotion (pp. 205227). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Lane, R. D., & Nadel, L. (Eds.). (2000). Cognitive neuroscience of emotion. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lang, P. J. (1995). The emotion probe: Studies of motivation and attention. American Psychologist, 50, 372385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1997). Motivated attention: Affect, activation and action. In Lang, P. J., Simons, R. F., & Balaban, M. (Eds.), Attention and orienting: Sensory and motivational processes (pp. 97135). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Lang, P. J., Davis, M., & Öhman, A. (2002). Fear and anxiety: Animal models and human cognitive psychophysiology. In Bäckman, L. & von Hofsten, C. (Eds.), Psychology at the turn of the millennium (Vol. 1, pp. 377411). Hove, UK: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1884). Stress, appraisal and coping. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
LeDoux, J. E. (1996). The emotional brain. New York: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
LeDoux, J. E. (2000). Cognitive-emotional interactions: Listen to the brain. In Lane, R. D. & Nadel, L. (Eds.), Cognitive neuroscience of emotion (pp. 129155). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Moltó, J., Montañés, S., Poy, R., Segarra, P., Pastor, M. C., Tormo, M. P., Ramírez, I., Hernández, M. A., Sánchez, M., Fernández, M. C., & Vila, J. (1999). Un nuevo método para el estudio experimental de las emociones: el International Affective Picture System (IAPS). Spanish adaptation. Revista de Psicología General y Aplicada, 52, 5587.Google Scholar
Obrist, P. A. (1981). Cardiovascular psychophysiology: A perspective. New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Öhman, A., Hamm, A., & Hugdahl, K. (2000). Cognition and the autonomic nervous system: Orienting, anticipation, and conditioning. In Cacioppo, J. T., Tassinary, L. G., & Berntson, G. G. (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology (pp. 533575). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Öhman, A., & Soares, J. J. F. (1994). Unconscious anxiety. Phobic responses to masked stimuli. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 231240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Öhman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness. Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychophysiological Review, 108, 483522.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pavlov, I. (1927). Conditioned reflexes. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pérez, N., Fernández, M. C., Vila, J., & Turpin, G. (2000). Attentional and emotional modulation of cardiac defense. Psychophysiology, 37, 275282.Google Scholar
Ramírez, I., Pérez, N., Sánchez, M., & Vila, J. (1999). Attentional modulation of cardiac defense: External versus internal mechanisms. Psychophysiology, 36, S92.Google Scholar
Reyes, G., Godoy, J., & Vila, J. (1993). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia as an index of parasympathetic cardiac control during the cardiac defense response. Biological Psychology, 35, 1735.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reyes, G., & Vila, J. (1993). Respiratory influences on the cardiac defense response. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 15, 1526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reyes, G., Vila, J., & García, A. (1994). Physiological significance of the defense response to intense auditory stimulation: A pharmacological blockade study. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 15, 1526.Google Scholar
Ruiz-Padial, E. (2003). Modulación emocional de los reflejos defensivos bajo condiciones preatencionales. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Granada, Spain.Google Scholar
Ruiz-Padial, E., Sánchez, M. B., Thayer, J. F., & Vila, J. (2002). Modulación no consciente de la respuesta cardiaca de defensa por imágenes fóbicas. Psicothema, 14, 739745.Google Scholar
Sánchez, M. (2000). Modulación emocional de los reflejos de sobresalto y defensa. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Granada, Spain.Google Scholar
Sánchez, M., Ruiz-Padial, E., Pérez, N., Fernández, M. C., Cobos, P., & Vila, J. (2002). Modulación emocional de los reflejos defensivos mediante visualización de imágenes afectivas. Psicothema, 14, 702707.Google Scholar
Selye, H. (1956). The stress of life. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Schell, L. M., & Lieberman, L. S. (1981). Noise stress and cancer. In Hammer, K. & Newberry, B. H. (Eds.), Stress and cancer (pp. 202232). Toronto: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Sokolov, E. N. (1963). Perception and the conditioned reflex. Oxford: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
Steptoe, A., & Vögele, C. (1991). Methodology of mental stress testing in cardiovascular research. Circulation, 83, 1424.Google ScholarPubMed
Thayer, J. F., & Siegle, G. J. (2002). Neurovisceral integration in cardiac and emotional regulation. IEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology, 21, 2429.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turpin, G. (1986). Effects of stimulus intensity on autonomic responding: The problem of differentiating orienting and defense reflexes. Psychophysiology, 23, 1, 114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turpin, G, & Siddle, D. A. (1978). Cardiac and forearm plethysmographic responses to high intensity auditory stimulation. Biological Psychology, 6, 267281.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turpin, G., & Siddle, D. A. (1983). Effects of stimulus intensity on cardiovascular activity. Psychophysiology, 20, 6, 611624.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vera, M. N., Vila, J., & Godoy, J. F. (1994). Cardiovascular effects of traffic noise: The role of negative self-statements. Psychological Medicine, 24, 817827.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vila, J. (1995). Cardiac psychophysiology and health. In Rodríguez-Marín, J. (Ed.), Health psychology and quality of life research (pp. 520540). Murcia: University of Alicante Press.Google Scholar
Vila, J. (2002). Cardiac defense and emotion: Psychophysiological and clinical implications. In Bäckman, L. & von Hofsten, C. (Eds.), Psychology at the turn of the millennium (Vol.1, pp. 413439). Hove, UK: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Vila, J., & Fernández, M. C. (1989). The cardiac defense response in humans: Effects of predictability and adaptation period. Journal of Psychophysiology, 3, 245258.Google Scholar
Vila, J., Fernández, M. C., & Godoy, J. (1992). The cardiac defense response in humans: Effects of stimulus modality and gender differences. Journal of Psychophysiology, 6, 140154.Google Scholar
Vila, J., Fernández, M. C., Pérez, M. N., & Reyes, G. (1996). Autonomic mechanisms underlying the cardiac defense response in humans. Psychophysiology, 33, S87.Google Scholar
Vila, J., Pérez, M. N., Fernández, M. C., Pegalajar, J., & Sánchez, M. (1997). Attentional modulation of the cardiac defense response in humans. Psychophysiology, 34, 482487.Google ScholarPubMed
Vila, J., Sánchez, M. B., Ramírez, I., & Fernández, M. C. (1997). Effect of stimulus intensity, rise time, and duration on the cardiac defense response. Psychophysiology, 34, S85.Google Scholar
Watson, J. B., & Rayner, R. (1920). Conditioned emotional reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 3, 114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar