Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-x5gtn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-19T07:30:06.366Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 2 - How We Know What We Think We Know about Stone Tools

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 October 2016

John J. Shea
Affiliation:
Stony Brook University, State University of New York

Summary

Type
Chapter
Information
Stone Tools in Human Evolution
Behavioral Differences among Technological Primates
, pp. 10 - 19
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Of each thing, ask what is it in itself, in its own constitution? What is its substance and material, and how did it come to be? What does it do in the world, and how long does it persist?

Marcus Aurelius Antonius (AD 167 Meditations 8.11)

This chapter explores the sources of our current knowledge about stone tools, namely ethnographic and ethological observations, experimental archaeology, and contextual clues from the archaeological record. These sources allow us to develop a “Pre-Industrial Model” of human/hominin stone tool use. Contrasts between this Pre-Industrial Model and observations of non-human primate tool use allow testable predictions (hypotheses) about changes and variability in hominin stone tool production and use.

Sources of Information about Lithic Technology

Most of what archaeologists think we know about prehistoric stone tool technology is based on either actualistic or contextual information. Actualistic information comes from ethnoarchaeology, experimental archaeology, and any circumstances in which stone tool production, use, and discard have actually been observed. Contextual information is archaeological evidence interpreted according to generally accepted principles of geology, biology, chemistry, and other scientific disciplines.

Actualistic Information

Ethnoarchaeology

Ethnoarchaeology observes how living humans create an archaeological record. Most systematic ethnoarchaeological studies of stone tool technology involve people who otherwise use metal tools, or people who are lithic craft specialists, producing stone knives, axes, and grinding stones (Binford Reference Binford1986, Toth, Clark, and Ligabue Reference Toth, Clark and Ligabue1992, Searcy Reference Searcy2011). There are relatively few ethnoarchaeological studies of people who habitually make and use stone tools as routine parts of their daily lives. Ethnoarchaeological accounts of stone tool production and use are unevenly distributed around the world. The most detailed accounts of stone tool use by hunter-gatherers are from Australia (Hayden Reference Hayden1979, Gould 1971, Reference Gould1980; for recent overviews, see Holdaway and Douglas Reference Holdaway and Douglas2012, McCall Reference McCall2012). Stone tool use by horticulturalists, pastoralists, and former hunter-gatherers is more widely documented in Africa, Southeast Asia, and the Americas (Nelson Reference Nelson and Hodge1916, White Reference White, Bordes and de Sonneville-Bordes1968, Gallagher Reference Gallagher1977, Miller Reference Miller and Hayden1979, Clark and Kurashina Reference Clark, Kurashina, Gould and Schiffer1981, Brandt and Weedman Reference Brandt, Weedman, Fukui, Kuimoto and Shigeta1997, Weedman Reference Weedman2006).

Experimental Archaeology

“Experimental archaeology” describes a wide range of activities from contrived mechanical experiments to more holistic “experiential” activities. Experiments can often demonstrate probable sources of variability (Eren et al. Reference Eren, Lycett, Patten, Buchanan, Pargeter and O’Brien2016), while experiential activities more typically suggest possible sources of variation (Shea Reference Shea2015a). Many rocks used to make knapped stone tools fracture conchoidally, and mechanical experiments have investigated this property (for a review, see Dibble and Rezek Reference Dibble and Rezek2009, Lin et al. Reference Lin, Rezek, Braun and Dibble2013). The mechanics of groundstone technology have been less extensively researched, possibly due to the wide range of rocks shaped by abrasion (Dubreuil and Savage Reference Dubreuil and Savage2014). Hobby/craft knappers have documented experiential approaches to investigating rock fracture (Johnson Reference Johnson1978, Whittaker Reference Whittaker1994) to such an extent that archaeologists use their terms for stone tool production rather than terms from fracture mechanics (Crabtree Reference Crabtree1972, Inizan et al. Reference Inizan, Reduron-Ballinger, Roche, Tixier and Féblot-Augustins1999). Other experiential activities include teaching stone tool production and use to others and using stone tools for various purposes (Shea Reference Shea and Shott2015b). Some of these tool use experiments focus on identifying microwear patterns diagnostic of certain activities (Keeley Reference Keeley1980, Odell Reference Odell1981). Others aim for subjective assessments of how well stone tools with different qualities perform in one or another task (e.g., Jones Reference Jones1981, Schick and Toth Reference Schick and Toth1993: 147–186, Shea, Davis, and Brown Reference Shea, Davis and Brown2001, Shea, Brown, and Davis Reference Shea, Brown, Davis and Mathieu2002). Still other experiments simulate trampling and geological sources of damage in order to develop criteria for recognizing their effects in the archaeological record (e.g., McBrearty et al. Reference McBrearty, Bishop, Plummer, Dewar and Conard1998, Pargeter and Bradfield Reference Pargeter and Bradfield2012).

Contextual Information

Contextual information is inferred from the archaeological record. Such information includes stratigraphic associations, artifact-refitting patterns, stone tool cut-marks, microwear, and residues. Because contextual information is based on inferences rather than direct observations, it has to be used cautiously.

Stratigraphic Associations

Stone tools occur together with other artifacts and ecofacts (natural objects), and these associations can inspire hypotheses about the motivations of prehistoric tool makers. Yet, such spatial and stratigraphic associations can also be misleading, or at least interpretively complex (Schiffer Reference Schiffer1987). The geological “principle of association” holds that artifacts enclosed in the same sediment were buried at the same time in a broad sense, but this does not mean they were buried simultaneously or as the result of the same activity. Tools in the same archaeological assemblage may have been deposited days, weeks, months, years, even centuries apart from one another. The most common targets of archaeological excavations – caves/rockshelters and landforms near water sources – are perennial magnets for human occupation. If humans are in the vicinity, they reside there. Assemblages from such sites likely combine the traces of multiple occupations near to one another in time. More valuable sources of insights come from small sites where sediments were deposited quickly, and in which the possibility of multiple separate human/hominin occupations is low. Because such “mini-sites” lack the archaeological visibility of larger lithic accumulations, they form a tiny fraction of the global record for hominin stone tool use.

Artifact Refitting

Matching fracture surfaces on stone tools can allow one to reconstruct how artifacts were shaped and detached from one another, but not all such matches provide the same information. Fractures caused during core reduction and artifact shaping (refits) and edge resharpening (modifications) shed light on tool production and maintenance. Fragments separated by other causes (breaks) may reflect trampling, soil compaction, and other depositional processes, even excavation-related damage. Spatial analysis of artifact refitting patterns can provide insights into toolmaking strategies as well as about site formation processes (for overviews, see Cziesla Reference Cziesla, Cziesla, Eickhoff, Arts and Winter1990, Laughlin and Kelly Reference Laughlin and Kelly2010). Refitting studies frequently assume that all flakes struck from a particular rock were detached near to one another in time, by the same person, or at least by members of the same co-residential community. This is not necessarily true. The ethnographic record offers up numerous accounts of stone tools being made from artifacts found eroding from abandoned habitations and archaeological sites.

Stone-tool Cut-marks and Percussion Damage

After stone tools, vertebrate fossils are the second most common things found at prehistoric archaeological sites. Some of these fossils preserve cut-marks and fractures caused by stone tool use (Shipman Reference Shipman1981, Blumenschine and Pobiner Reference Blumenschine, Pobiner and Ungar2007). Sites with exceptional preservation can also preserve wood with wear traces from stone tool use. Such discoveries are revolutionizing archaeologists’ views about earlier hominin technological strategies (e.g., Conard et al. Reference Conard, Serangeli, Böhner, Starkovich, Miller, Urban and Van Kolfschoten2015). Needless to say, however, such evidence suffers from the problem of “false negative” findings. The absence of preserved bone, wood, cordage, and other organic artifacts does not necessarily indicate hominins overlooked these materials’ potential value as tools.

Microwear

Microwear (fractures, abrasion, and polish) on stone tool edges and surfaces furnishes clues about the use of particular stone tools, about the parts of the tools that were used, the motions employed, and the nature of the worked materials (for reviews, see Semenov Reference Semenov1964, Shea Reference Shea1992, Odell Reference Odell2004: 152). Activities involving high loading forces, prolonged use, and work on relatively hard materials leave more distinctive wear traces than tasks involving low loading forces, brief use, and cutting softer materials. Microwear traces are relatively small-scale phenomena that can be altered by post-depositional mechanical wear, such as flowing water, soil movement, trampling, and other factors (Shea and Klenck Reference Shea and Klenck1993, Levi-Sala Reference Levi-Sala1996).

Residues

Organic residues, including blood, hair, starch particles, and inorganic substances, such as red ochre (iron oxide), can sometimes be detected on stone tool edges and surfaces. The principal challenge in interpreting this evidence lies in differentiating between residues bonded to the tool surface as the result of cutting; residues from handles, fibers, and mastic use to attach tools to handles; and residues from sediments enclosing the tools. Though archaeologists have gained considerable expertise in visual identifications of residues, many such residues are ambiguous, and interpreting them can require physical or chemical analysis (for a recent review, see Monnier, Ladwig, and Porter Reference Monnier, Ladwig and Porter2012).

A Pre-industrial Model of Lithic Technology

Insights from actualistic and contextual sources allow one to construct a model for lithic technology, one that differs from early archaeologists’ Industrial Model. This “Pre-Industrial Model” contrasts with its Industrial counterpart in recognizing a greater range of variability in stone tool production, use, and discard (Table 2.1). Inasmuch as this model pulls together evidence from both historical and present-day (ethnographic) sources, it is discussed below using the “ethnographic present” tense.

Table 2.1 Industrial, pre-industrial, and non-human primate models of stone tool use

ActivityIndustrial ModelPre-industrial ModelNon-human Primate Model
Raw material procurementQuarried and transported in-bulk to production sitesMixed strategies of quarrying, embedded procurement, recyclingAggregates of individually transported objects
AgencyAdultsAdults (males >females), children (?)Mainly females and children
TransmissionFormal apprenticeshipImitation and directed learningImitation
Production methodsStandardizedSome stereotyped production, but little standardization, much variabilityTools used without modification
Artifact designsSize varies, but form and function are strongly correlatedSize varies, and form–function correlations are variableLarge tools, whose form and function are strongly correlated
Context of productionAt special-purpose workshops at or near habitation sitesNot necessarily correlated with habitation sitesNot applicable
Artifact repairYes, typically by specialistsYes, but variable, context-dependentNo
Artifact discardDistant from production sitesVariable, both near and far from production sites, habitation sites, resource extraction sitesAt location where used to extract food resources
Change through timeSubstitutionAdditionLittle/no change? (Inadequate evidence)

Pre-industrial Stone Tool Production

Pre-industrial humans’ choices of lithic raw material are governed by a combination of practical mechanical considerations such as ease of access, fracture properties, hardness, and silica content (Binford Reference Binford1980, Gould Reference Gould1980). Nevertheless, subjective qualities, such as aesthetics or perceived symbolic associations, play roles as well. Lithic raw material procurement is frequently “embedded” in daily foraging tasks carried out within a few kilometers of habitation sites. The most common lithic raw materials at any archaeological site are usually those available in the immediate geological substrate or within a 10–30 km radius of the site. Quarrying operations (“direct procurement”) occur, too, but these usually involve small work parties or individuals visiting a geological source. At such sources, people replace worn-out tools on the spot and reduce raw rock into small, efficiently transportable packages. Sedentary groups supplying long-term residential sites sometimes undertake in-bulk quarrying operations. Abandoned habitation sites and archaeological sites are also used as sources of tool materials. Lithic materials are also procured socially, through exchanges among individuals. Such exchanges range from straightforward commercial transactions involving producers, consumers, and middlemen to more complex ones guided by symbolic/supernatural motivations. Many individuals deploy more than one such raw material procurement strategy simultaneously.

Unlike Industrial Era factory work, ethnographic flintknapping occurs on an irregular schedule, either as tools are needed or during “down-time” between other activities. Most ethnographic tool makers use more than one distinct way of obtaining cutting edges. Indeed, stone tools continued to be made and used thousands of years after the appearance of metallurgy in the Near East and in Africa (Rosen Reference Rosen1997, Mitchell Reference Mitchell2002). To this day, some indigenous populations in Africa, Asia, and Australia continue to use stone tools alongside metal ones.

While there are instances of craft specialization in the ethnographic record, much stone tool production involves non-specialists. In ethnographic literature, adult males do most of the tool making, but this may reflect reporting bias by adult male ethnographers (but see Gould Reference Gould1980). There is no reason to assume prehistoric stone tool production or use was invariably organized along gender lines (Gero Reference Gero, Gero and Conkey1991, Arthur Reference Arthur2010). For evidence of prehistoric stone tool use by women, one need look no further than Exodus 4: 25–26,

Now it came about at the lodging place on the way that the Lord met (Moses) and sought to put him to death. Then Zipporah took a flint and cut off her son’s foreskin and threw it at Moses’ feet, and she said, “You are indeed a bridegroom of blood to me.” So He let him alone.

There are few detailed ethnographic accounts of children making or using stone tools, but it is reasonable to suppose prehistoric children did so, either for their own tool needs or in learning by imitating adult activities (Shea Reference Shea2006a). Only recent post-Industrial parents systemically keep children away from sharp implements “for their own good.”

Ethnographic flintknapping can be either highly stereotyped or widely variable, but there is little standardization. Even when ethnographic knappers work toward the production of similar artifacts, complex patterns of similarities and differences emerge (Binford Reference Binford1986, Toth et al. Reference Toth, Clark and Ligabue1992). The same variability can be seen in the work of craft/hobby flintknappers, some of whom can identify one another’s work by simple visual inspection of the artifacts (Whittaker Reference Whittaker2004).

Pre-industrial Stone Tool Use

Ethnographic humans use stone tools as projectile armatures, as tools for butchery, pulverizing seeds and nuts, carpentry, hide-working, flintknapping, carving stone, bone, and other hard materials, and for symbolic/ritual purposes (e.g., scarification, bloodletting). Though ethnographic documentation for stone tool use in some of these activities is actually rather sparse, many tasks performed today with metal tools were undoubtedly done in earlier times using stone tools. This being said, there are at least three activities for which ethnographic humans use metal tools for which there is little ethnographic or archaeological evidence of stone tool use. These activities include immediate pre-oral food processing (cutting food into small pieces immediately prior to eating them), digging in soil, and as weapon armatures for fishing. Inuit and other Arctic peoples used abraded-edge cutting knives (ulus) to cut up food prior to ingesting it, but other than this, the ethnographic record offers few other examples of stone tools (or indeed metal ones either) specifically dedicated to this activity. Wear traces and plant and animal tissues preserved on archaeological stone tools suggest simple flakes may have been used for these tasks, but it is also possible that much pre-oral food processing with stone tools was so brief that it left little detectable archaeological evidence. A few stone tools from North American contexts preserve “hoe polish” from tilling silty sediments (Witthoft Reference Witthoft1967), but such wear traces are otherwise rare. That so few stone tools are used to dig in soil or as tips for fishing spears probably reflects the fact that these tasks involve high risks of tool breakage due to collisions with rocks in the geological substrate.

Nearly every major category of stone tool that has either been subjected to microwear and/or residue analysis or whose use has been observed ethnographically exhibits functional variability (Odell Reference Odell2004). Mobility also affects form–function relationships (Binford Reference Binford1979, Parry and Kelly Reference Parry, Kelly, Johnson and Morrow1987). Transported stone implements are often design compromises, tools that perform adequately in the wide range of tasks their owners might encounter while on the move. Sedentary populations maintained larger and more functionally specialized toolkits.

Some findings from ethnoarchaeology contradict common archaeological assumptions about retouch and stone tool use. For example, archaeologists often equate retouch (small fractures along tool edges) with evidence of use. Ethnographic humans routinely use tools without putting such edge damage on them. They also retouch tools that they do not end up using. Predictably, many retouched archaeological stone tools preserve no traces of microwear or residues. Most archaeologists assume that unretouched flakes/flake fragments shorter than 2–3 cm in any dimension were too small to have been used as cutting implements, but ethnographic stone cutting tools include many such small unretouched artifacts.

Pre-industrial Stone Tool Discard

Stone tools are sharp. Understandably, much ethnographic stone tool production takes place away from high-foot-traffic areas in habitation sites. When lithic production happens at residential sites it can be accompanied by in-bulk removal and re-deposition of lithic artifacts away from living spaces (Gallagher Reference Gallagher1977). Thus, archaeological sites where residues of stone tool production occur in great quantities may not have been habitation sites at all, but rather workshops or dumping areas located some distance from where people were actually living at the time. Juxtaposed concentrations of flintknapping debris and habitation traces (hearths, architecture) may reflect reuse of former habitation sites as impromptu stone tool procurement/production sites.

Ethnographic stone tool discard behavior varies widely and in response to several circumstances. Where raw materials are abundant, the threshold for discarding them is often relatively low. That is, tools are discarded with minimal effort to retouch or resharpen them. Where raw materials are scarce, tools are often heavily resharpened, but this can vary widely, too. Because of the considerable amount of time and energy needed to remove and replace tools attached to handles, these artifacts are often heavily retouched and resharpened (Binford Reference Binford1979, Keeley Reference Keeley1982). Hand-held tools are often discarded as soon as their cutting edges become perceptibly dull.

Non-human Primate Stone Tool Use

Chimpanzees, bonobos, gorillas, orangutans, and macaques (Macaca), as well as more phylogenetically distant monkeys (e.g., capuchin monkeys Cebus spp.) use tools in the wild (Panger et al. Reference Panger, Brooks, Richmond and Wood2003, Haslam et al. Reference Haslam, Hernandez-Aguilar, Ling, Carvalho, de la Torre, DeStefano, Du, Hardy, Harris, Marchant, Matsuzawa, McGrew, Mercader, Mora, Petraglia, Roche, Visalberghi and Warren2009, Toth and Schick Reference Toth and Schick2009). For research on the stone tool evidence, however, chimpanzees and bonobos are most directly relevant due to their morphological similarity to humans. Table 2.1 summarizes some of the ways in which stone tool use by these non-human primates differs from both Industrial and Pre-Industrial models of human stone tool use.

Non-human Primate Raw Material Procurement and Stone Tool Production

Non-human primate raw material procurement involves transporting rocks from localities where they are available on the surface to places where they are used. Tool materials are neither excavated from bedrock deposits nor pre-shaped for transportation.

Wild-living non-human primates use stone in its natural form. They select stones for use as tools based on size, shape, and compositional criteria, but raw material choice is largely governed by the local geological substrate. Chimpanzees have been observed transporting stones several kilometers (Boesch and Boesch Reference Boesch and Boesch1984a), but these are extreme examples for that species. They rarely carry stones more than a few hundred meters.

Two captive bonobos have been taught to make and use stone cutting tools in laboratory settings (Toth et al. Reference Toth, Schick, Savage-Rumbaugh, Sevcik and Rumbaugh1993), but spontaneous and systematic flaked stone cutting tool production has not been observed among apes living outside captivity.

Non-human Primate Stone Tool Use

The overwhelming majority of observations of non-human primate stone tool use report hand-held stone percussors being used to crack open nuts (Haslam et al. Reference Haslam, Hernandez-Aguilar, Ling, Carvalho, de la Torre, DeStefano, Du, Hardy, Harris, Marchant, Matsuzawa, McGrew, Mercader, Mora, Petraglia, Roche, Visalberghi and Warren2009) (Figure 2.1). These artifacts are relatively larger in comparison to their users’ body size than the stone tools humans use for similar tasks. Monkeys also use stone tools to fracture the shells of crabs and molluscs as well as to dig in sediment. Neither edges nor abrasive stone surfaces are used to modify other materials. Non-human primates may bring stone tools into juxtaposition with one another, but they do not attach them to other non-lithic materials as multi-component tools. Nut-cracking stones are rarely repurposed for other tasks.

Figure 2.1 Chimpanzee stone tool use.

(Photograph provided by Tetsuro Matsuzawa of the Kyoto University Primate Research Center, used with permission.)

Adult females chimpanzees and their dependent offspring use stone tools more often than adult males (Boesch and Boesch Reference Boesch and Boesch1984b). Juveniles learn techniques for cracking nuts with stone tools mainly by observing and imitating adults over prolonged periods (i.e., years).

Non-human Primate Stone Tool Discard

In those cases where stone tools have broken during use, non-human primates may continue to use them, but they do not attempt to restore their functionality by further physical modification. Non-human primates usually abandon stone tools where they are used. Consequently, after many individual episodes of stone transport, use, and deposition large accretional accumulations of stones can appear at non-human primate tool use locations (Mercader, Panger, and Boesch Reference Mercader, Panger and Boesch2002).

Why Compare Human vs. Non-human Primate Lithic Technology?

Anthropologists have long viewed non-human primate behavior as a model for the behavior of human ancestors (Kinzey Reference Kinzey1986), but doing this risks ignoring significant evolutionary differences between extant primates and extinct hominins as well as differences in their habitats (Potts Reference Potts and Kinzey1987). Just as comparing human vs. non-human primate anatomy inspires hypotheses about sources of morphological variation among hominin fossils, comparing differences in human vs. non-human stone tool use can help us develop hypotheses about how evolutionarily derived human behaviors influenced variation in the stone tool record. Before we can make these comparisons and generate the hypotheses that can be tested with prehistoric lithic evidence, we need to change some of the ways archaeologists describe that evidence. These changes are outlined in the next chapter.

Figure 0

Table 2.1 Industrial, pre-industrial, and non-human primate models of stone tool use

Figure 1

Figure 2.1 Chimpanzee stone tool use.

(Photograph provided by Tetsuro Matsuzawa of the Kyoto University Primate Research Center, used with permission.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×