Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-qxdb6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T09:02:59.363Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Systemic inflammation and intelligence in early adulthood and subsequent risk of schizophrenia and other non-affective psychoses: a longitudinal cohort and co-relative study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 April 2018

Nils Kappelmann*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
Golam M Khandaker
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK Cambridgeshire and Peterborough NHS Foundation Trust, Cambridge, UK
Henrik Dal
Affiliation:
Centre for Epidemiology and Community Medicine, Stockholm County Council, Stockholm, Sweden
Jan Stochl
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK Department of Kinanthropology, Charles University, Prague, Czech Republic
Kyriaki Kosidou
Affiliation:
Centre for Epidemiology and Community Medicine, Stockholm County Council, Stockholm, Sweden Department of Public Health Sciences, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
Peter B Jones
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK Cambridgeshire and Peterborough NHS Foundation Trust, Cambridge, UK
Christina Dalman
Affiliation:
Centre for Epidemiology and Community Medicine, Stockholm County Council, Stockholm, Sweden Department of Public Health Sciences, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
Håkan Karlsson
Affiliation:
Department of Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
*
Author for correspondence: Nils Kappelmann, E-mail: n.kappelmann@gmail.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Schizophrenia is associated with impaired neurodevelopment as indexed by lower premorbid IQ. We examined associations between erythrocyte sedimentation rate (ESR), a marker of low-grade systemic inflammation, IQ, and subsequent schizophrenia and other non-affective psychoses (ONAP) to elucidate the role of neurodevelopment and inflammation in the pathogenesis of psychosis.

Methods

Population-based data on ESR and IQ from 638 213 Swedish men assessed during military conscription between 1969 and 1983 were linked to National Hospital Discharge Register for hospitalisation with schizophrenia and ONAP. The associations of ESR with IQ (cross-sectional) and psychoses (longitudinal) were investigated using linear and Cox-regression. The co-relative analysis was used to examine effects of shared familial confounding. We examined mediation and moderation of effect between ESR and IQ on psychosis risk.

Results

Baseline IQ was associated with subsequent risk of schizophrenia (adjusted HR per 1-point increase in IQ = 0.961; 95% confidence interval (CI) 0.960–0.963) and ONAP (adjusted HR = 0.973; 95% CI 0.971–0.975). Higher ESR was associated with lower IQ in a dose-response fashion. High ESR was associated with increased risk for schizophrenia (adjusted HR = 1.14; 95% CI 1.01–1.28) and decreased risk for ONAP (adjusted HR = 0.85; 95% CI 0.74–0.96), although these effects were specific to one ESR band (7–10 mm/hr). Familial confounding explained ESR-IQ but not ESR-psychoses associations. IQ partly mediated the ESR-psychosis relationships.

Conclusions

Lower IQ is associated with low-grade systemic inflammation and with an increased risk of schizophrenia and ONAP in adulthood. Low-grade inflammation may influence schizophrenia risk by affecting neurodevelopment. Future studies should explore the differential effects of inflammation on different types of psychosis.

Type
Original Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018

Introduction

The neurodevelopmental hypothesis of schizophrenia posits that abnormal neurodevelopment contributes to the pathogenesis of the illness (Murray & Lewis, Reference Murray and Lewis1987; Weinberger, Reference Weinberger1987). This hypothesis is supported by population-based longitudinal studies showing an association of schizophrenia and other non-affective psychoses (ONAP) with lower IQ during childhood/premorbid period (Jones et al. Reference Jones1994; Crow et al. Reference Crow, Done and Sacker1995; David et al. Reference David1997; Davidson et al. Reference Davidson1999; Cannon et al. Reference Cannon2000, Reference Cannon2002; Zammit et al. Reference Zammit2004; Reichenberg et al. Reference Reichenberg2005; Kendler et al. Reference Kendler2015). These findings have been summarised in a meta-analysis, which shows that there is a linear association between premorbid IQ and risk of adult schizophrenia and related psychoses; for every one-point decrease in IQ risk for schizophrenia increases by 3.7% (Khandaker et al. Reference Khandaker2011).

Co-relative control analysis is a way to examine familial confounding. This approach compares effect sizes for a particular risk factor-disease association between the general population and groups of relatives with different levels of relatedness (i.e. full siblings, half-siblings, cousins). If the effect size decreases in groups of relatives, compared with general population, this indicates that the association may be explained by shared familial confounding. Using co-relative control analysis, Kendler and colleagues reported that lower cognitive functioning in schizophrenia and ONAP is unlikely to be explained by familial confounding. Furthermore, premorbid cognitive deficits in psychosis may be the result of unique environmental factors (Kendler et al. Reference Kendler2015, Reference Kendler2016a, Reference Kendlerb). These findings provide important insights into potential causal models of psychosis and warrant exploration of unique environmental factors that may contribute to lower cognitive functioning in psychosis.

Emerging evidence indicates a role for low-grade inflammation in the pathogenesis of psychotic disorders (Khandaker & Dantzer, Reference Khandaker and Dantzer2015; Khandaker et al. Reference Khandaker2015a, Reference Khandaker, Dantzer and Jones2017a). Concentrations of circulating inflammatory markers such as interleukin 6 (IL-6) and C-reactive protein (CRP) are elevated during acute psychotic episodes, which tend to normalise after recovery but continue to be elevated in treatment-resistant patients (Potvin et al. Reference Potvin2008; Miller et al. Reference Miller2011; Goldsmith et al. Reference Goldsmith, Rapaport and Miller2016). Elevated serum concentrations of IL-6 and CRP in childhood/ adolescence are associated with increased risks of psychotic symptoms or diagnosis of schizophrenia subsequently in adulthood (Khandaker et al. Reference Khandaker2014; Metcalf et al. Reference Metcalf2017), which indicates that reverse causality is an unlikely explanation for the observed association between low-grade inflammation and psychosis. A functional genetic variant in the IL-6R gene known to dampen down inflammation (Ferreira et al. Reference Ferreira2013) is protective for psychosis and/or severe depression (Khandaker et al. Reference Khandaker2017b) and physical illness commonly comorbid with psychosis such as coronary heart disease (IL6R Genetics Consortium & Emerging Risk Factors Collaboration, 2012) as shown using Mendelian randomisation. This suggests that the inflammation–psychosis relationship is unlikely to be explained by confounding fully.

Despite a great deal of cross-sectional case-control studies linking low-grade inflammation with psychosis (Goldsmith et al. Reference Goldsmith, Rapaport and Miller2016), population-based longitudinal studies are relatively rare (Gardner et al. Reference Gardner2013; Wium-Andersen et al. Reference Wium-Andersen, Orsted and Nordestgaard2013; Khandaker et al. Reference Khandaker2014; Metcalf et al. Reference Metcalf2017). To our knowledge, no longitudinal study has examined the association between erythrocyte sedimentation rate (ESR), a surrogate marker of systemic inflammation and particularly acute phase reaction (Harrison, Reference Harrison2015), and subsequent risk of schizophrenia and related psychosis.

Low-grade inflammation can influence cognitive function across the life-course in the healthy population and in people with schizophrenia. We have reported that elevated CRP is associated with lower IQ in childhood in the ALSPAC birth cohort (MacKinnon et al. Reference MacKinnon2017). We have also reported that there is an inverse relationship between ESR and IQ based on a sample of healthy Swedish adolescents/young-adults (Karlsson et al. Reference Karlsson2010). Similarly, higher levels of CRP are associated with a lower cognitive function in patients with schizophrenia (Dickerson et al. Reference Dickerson2007). It is plausible that low-grade inflammation influences psychosis risk by affecting neurodevelopment. However, studies examining whether IQ mediates and/or moderates the association between ESR and subsequent psychotic disorders or whether they are independent risk factors are scarce.

The present study aimed to examine the relationship between inflammation (indexed by ESR), neurodevelopment (indexed by IQ), and subsequent risk of hospitalisation with non-affective psychoses in adulthood in a Swedish population-based sample of 638 213 male conscripts. First, we examine the relationships between lower cognitive functioning at 18–20 years and subsequent diagnosis of psychoses (Jones et al. Reference Jones1994; Zammit et al. Reference Zammit2004; Khandaker et al. Reference Khandaker2011; Kendler et al. Reference Kendler2015). Second, we examine the association between inflammation and IQ reported previously from our smaller population-based Swedish sample (Karlsson et al. Reference Karlsson2010). Third, we test the relationship between inflammation and subsequent diagnosis of psychotic illness. In addition, we examine whether the ESR-psychoses relationship and ESR-IQ association could be explained by shared familial factors using co-relative analysis. We examine mediation and moderation between ESR and IQ with regards to risk of psychoses.

Materials and methods

Study population and diagnosis of psychosis

The present study is based on Swedish men conscripted into military between 1969 and 1983. In Sweden, all men underwent obligatory military conscription between ages 18 and 20 years bar certain exceptions, so this representative sample included approximately 97% of the male population (Karlsson et al. Reference Karlsson2010). The conscripts underwent various assessments over 2 days that tested their suitability for military service. The data provide a unique opportunity for epidemiological investigations; see Ahlborg et al. (Reference Ahlborg1973). The conscription data were linked to the Swedish National Hospital Discharge Register until end of 2011, resulting in an average follow-up period of 35 years (range: 21–42 years) after conscription (mean age at follow-up: 54 years; range: 45–68 years). The discharge register contains medical diagnoses defined by the International Classification of Diseases (ICD; World Health Organization, 2004). This allowed the identification of individuals first hospitalised with a discharge diagnosis of schizophrenia (ICD-10: F20; ICD-9 and ICD-8: 295) or ONAP (ICD-10: F21 = Schizotypal Disorder, F22 = Persistent Delusional Disorder, F23 = Acute and Transient Psychotic Disorders, F24 = Induced Delusional Disorder, F25 = Schizoaffective Disorder, F28 = Other Non-organic Psychotic Disorders, F29 = Unspecified Non-organic Psychosis; corresponding diagnostic codes in ICD-9: 297, 298.2-9, 291.3, 291.5, 292.1; ICD-8: 297.0-9, 298.2-3, 298.9, 291.2-3). See online Supplementary Fig. 1 for sample selection process.

Measurements of IQ and ESR

Intelligence was measured during military conscription by tests on logic/general intelligence, verbal intelligence, visuospatial perception, and mechanical skills in solving mathematical or physical problems; see Karlsson et al. (Reference Karlsson2010). Results of these tests were aggregated on a nine-point scale that approximately corresponds to Wechsler adult IQ bands <74, 74–81, 82–89, 90–95, 96–104, 105–110, 111–118, 119–126, and >126. For visibility and comparison purposes, we rescaled IQ scores from this 9-point scale to a continuous score with mean = 100 (SD = 15).

ESR was measured from venous blood samples taken at conscription. ESR is the rate of sedimentation of red blood cells measured over an hour and is a marker of inflammation, particularly a surrogate marker of acute phase reaction (Harrison, Reference Harrison2015). As with our previous Swedish study (Karlsson et al. Reference Karlsson2010), ESR was corrected for the erythrocyte volume in the blood (i.e. haematocrit), according to the formula ESR × (haematocrit /45), and grouped into four bands, 0–3, 4–6, 7–10, and ⩾11 mm/h. The distribution of ESR was skewed (mean = 3.38; median = 2.18; IQR = 1.87–4.00), so this categorisation allowed better investigation of the effect of high ESR levels on IQ/psychosis. In addition, we carried out sensitivity analyses: (1) by grouping ESR into quartiles; (2) by grouping ESR into deciles; (3) using ESR as a continuous variable; and (4) after excluding participants with very high ESR that may be indicative of an infection; according to Mayo clinic, in men ESR>22 mm/h indicates acute infection.

Covariates

Migration status (either/both parents born outside Sweden), winter birth (December–May), parental history of schizophrenia and/ or ONAP, household crowding, and maximum parental socioeconomic status at child's age of 8–12 years (worker, white collar professional, business owner, and unknown) were included as covariates.

Statistical analysis

First, to analyse the association of IQ and ESR with schizophrenia or ONAP, Cox regression was used to estimate hazard ratios (HRs) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) for each one-point increase in IQ (and separately, for individuals in the higher ESR bands compared with ESR band 0–3 mm/h). Further analyses were carried out adjusting for potential confounders, and after excluding 622 participants hospitalised within 2 years of conscription to minimise reverse causality. Second, we performed co-relative analyses to determine whether HRs for the association between ESR and schizophrenia or ONAP changed as a function of family relatedness. To this end, we applied stratified Cox regression models on respective outcomes in the general population, cousin, half-sibling, and full-sibling pairs. For analyses of relative pairs, robust standard errors were calculated to adjust for family relatedness, that is, we included a cluster effect of each family pair. Furthermore, relative pairs falling into equal ESR bands were excluded to allow identifiability/convergence of the statistical model. We tested the proportionality assumption across all models. We also performed co-relative analyses of the ESR-IQ association. Due to the continuous outcome variable and cross-sectional association, we estimated linear mixed-effects models with outcome IQ and categorical ESR predictors as before (again excluding relative pairs with equal ESR). The models included a random intercept for relative pairs to stratify analyses. Third, we tested the association between ESR (predictor) and IQ (outcome) using linear regression before and after adjusting for potential confounders.

Fourth, we tested whether IQ mediated or moderated the relationship between ESR and schizophrenia or ONAP. For these analyses, we used logistic and linear regression models. We tested mediation by calculating direct and indirect effects of ESR on psychoses. Mediation analysis of the ESR-psychoses relationship by IQ was not conducted for outcome schizophrenia as the unadjusted main analysis did not reach statistical significance, which is a prerequisite for testing mediation. However, mediation analysis for the outcome of ONAP was conducted using the subgroups of individuals with ESR of 0–3 mm/h or 7–10 mm/h, in which main analyses had shown an association between ESR and psychosis. We were also able to run mediation analyses of these ESR groups for both schizophrenia and ONAP when adjusting for covariates. Moderation was tested by including an interaction term (IQxESR) in logistic regression models of schizophrenia and ONAP.

Results

Baseline characteristics

Of the total sample of 638 213 Swedish men, at baseline 470 923 (73.79%) had an ESR level of 0–3 mm/h, 112 089 (17.56%) of 4–6 mm/h, 35 023 (5.49%) of 7–10 mm/h, and 20 178 (3.16%) of ⩾11 mm/h. ESR groups differed significantly in age at conscription, migration status, winter birth, parental socioeconomic status, and household crowding (all p < 0.05; Table 1). In total, 5398 (0.85%) and 5133 (0.80%) individuals in this population-based sample developed schizophrenia or ONAP until follow-up, respectively.

Table 1. Baseline characteristics of the sample

a p values are based on ANOVA tests for continuous and χ2 tests for categorical variables.

Associations between IQ, schizophrenia, and ONAP

With each one-point increase in IQ, risk decreased for schizophrenia (unadjusted HR = 0.963, 95% CI 0.961–0.964; adjusted HR = 0.961, 95% CI 0.960–0.963), and for ONAP (unadjusted HR = 0.974; 95% CI 0.972–0.976; adjusted HR = 0.973, 95% CI 0.971–0.975). Evidence for these associations remained after excluding individuals hospitalised within 2 years after conscription (online Supplementary Table 1).

Association between ESR and IQ

Higher ESR was associated with lower IQ at the time of conscription in a dose-response function (Table 2). When compared with ESR of 0–3 mm/h, there was a small but robust decrease in IQ for groups with higher ESR levels. After adjusting for potential confounders these associations attenuated, but remained significant. We explored whether the dose–response relationship observed using ESR as a categorical variable could be more parsimoniously explained using ESR as a continuous variable. Comparing these models using a log-likelihood test revealed no significant difference between two models (p = 0.62) suggesting that categorical classification of ESR is not superior to a linear description of the ESR-IQ association (online Supplementary Table 2).

Table 2. Association between ESR and IQ at age 18 years

a The mean difference was calculated using the group with ESR = 0–3 mm/h as a reference.

b Regression models were adjusted for household crowding, winter birth, parental socioeconomic status at 8–12 years, migration status, and parental history of non-affective psychoses.

Associations between ESR, schizophrenia, and ONAP

The risk of future hospitalisation with schizophrenia was higher in the group with ESR of 7–10 mm/h at baseline, compared with 0–3 mm/h, although this association only reached statistical significance after adjusting for confounding variables (see Table 3; Figure 1; online Supplementary Table 3). Conversely, the risk for ONAP was significantly lower in the group with ESR of 7–10 mm/h, compared with 0–3 mm/h, both before and after adjusting for confounders. There were no significant differences between ESR bands of 4–6 mm/h and ⩾11 mm/h as compared with the reference group (0–3 mm/h). Exclusion of prodromal cases did not change the pattern of results.

Fig. 1. Association between ESR and schizophrenia (left), and other non-affective psychoses (right) (the group with ESR of 0–3 mm/h was used as the reference for all analyses).

Table 3. Association between ESR and subsequent schizophrenia and other non-affective psychoses

a Cox regression models were adjusted for household crowding, winter birth, parental socioeconomic status at 8–12 years, migration status, and parental history of non-affective psychoses.

b Cases of other non-affective psychosis were excluded from the analysis of schizophrenia and vice versa.

* p < 0.05

Co-relative control analysis of the ESR-psychoses association

The HRs for schizophrenia and ONAP for the group with ESR levels 7–10 mm/h, compared with 0–3 mm/h, was similar for the general population and full-sibling pairs (who share on average 50% of their genomes and presumably the largest number of environmental factors among the investigated groups of co-relatives): higher ESR increased risk of schizophrenia, but decreased risk of ONAP (Table 4). The results offer some support that these specific ESR and psychoses relationships may not be fully confounded by shared familial factors.

Table 4. Co-relative analyses for association between ESR, schizophrenia and other non-affective psychoses

* The group with ESR 0–3 mm/h has been used as the reference category for all analyses

Co-relative control analysis of the ESR-IQ association

Co-relative control analysis of the ESR-IQ association, using linear mixed-effects regression and a random intercept for relative pairs, showed evidence of familial confounding (online Supplementary Table 4). The mean difference in IQ for higher ESR levels (compared with ESR 0–3 mm/h) were between 0.5 and 1.5 IQ points in the general population (ESR 4–6 mm/h = −0.48, 95% CI −0.58 to −0.38; ESR 7–10 mm/h = −0.95, 95% CI −1.12 to −0.79; ESR ⩾11 mm/h = −1.57, 95% CI −1.78 to −1.36). However, this difference markedly decreased in a gradual manner with increasing family relatedness (results for full-sibling pairs: ESR 4–6 mm/h = −0.02, 95% CI −0.17 to 0.14; ESR 7–10 mm/h = −0.21, 95% CI −0.44 to 0.03; ESR ⩾11 mm/h = −0.68, 95% CI −0.98 to −0.38.

Mediation and moderation of the ESR and psychoses association by IQ

We tested whether IQ mediated the association of ESR (7–10 mm/h) with schizophrenia and ONAP. For schizophrenia, the indirect effect (mediated by IQ) was significant in the adjusted model (β = 0.004, se < 0.001, p < 0.001). Similarly, for ONAP, the indirect effect was significant in both unadjusted (β = 0.003, se < 0.001, p < 0.001) and adjusted models (β = 0.003, se < 0.001, p < 0.001; see online Supplementary Table 5 and online Supplementary Fig. 2). However, there was no evidence for an interaction between ESR and IQ on regression models for schizophrenia or ONAP (online Supplementary Table 6).

Sensitivity analyses of ESR and psychoses association

We re-examined the ESR-psychosis association using ESR as a continuous variable, using ESR quartiles, and using ESR deciles before and after excluding participants with a suspected acute infection (ESR >22 mm/h). After adjusting for potential confounders and excluding both potentially prodromal cases of psychosis and those with suspected infection, there was trend level association indicating that each one-point increase in ESR increased the risk of schizophrenia (p = 0.070), but decreased the risk of ONAP (p = 0.053); see online Supplementary Table 7.

Using ESR as quartiles, there was no association with schizophrenia (online Supplementary Table 8). However, those in the 3rd quartile of ESR had an increased risk of ONAP compared with 1st quartile; unadjusted HR 1.09 (95% CI, 1.01, 1.17); p = 0.029; evidence for this association attenuated after adjusting for potential confounders and exclusions (p = 0.074) (online Supplementary Table 9).

After adjusting for potential confounders and exclusions, there was a trend level association indicating that those in the top decile of ESR, compared with bottom decile, had an increased risk of schizophrenia (HR = 1.12; 95% CI, 1.00, 1.27); p = 0.056 (online Supplementary Table 10), but decreased risk of ONAP (HR = 0.89; 95% CI, 0.78, 1.01); p = 0.074 (online Supplementary Table 11).

Discussion

The present study investigated potential roles of inflammation and neurodevelopment in the pathogenesis of psychotic disorders in a large Swedish population-based male sample. We showed that lower IQ is robustly associated with subsequent diagnoses of schizophrenia and ONAP. We also observed a cross-sectional association between ESR and IQ that followed a linear pattern; co-relative control analyses showed this association was partly due to familial factors. There was some, albeit weak, evidence for an association between ESR and increased risk for schizophrenia. Contrary to our hypotheses, higher ESR was associated with decreased risk for ONAP. Co-relative control analyses suggested that respective associations between ESR and subsequent risk of psychosis might not be explained fully by shared familial factors. With regard to the interplay between inflammation and neurodevelopment, there was some evidence that IQ partly mediated the association between ESR and psychotic disorders. There was no evidence for interaction between ESR and IQ with regard to the risk of psychoses.

The association between premorbid IQ and psychotic disorders has been studied extensively. Our findings are consistent with previous studies reporting lower premorbid IQ in future patients of schizophrenia and ONAP (Jones et al. Reference Jones1994; Crow et al. Reference Crow, Done and Sacker1995; David et al. Reference David1997; Davidson et al. Reference Davidson1999; Cannon et al. Reference Cannon2000, Reference Cannon2002; Zammit et al. Reference Zammit2004; Reichenberg et al. Reference Reichenberg2005; Kendler et al. Reference Kendler2015). Adjusting for potential confounders and exclusion of suspected prodromal cases had minimal impact on this association. Previous studies indicate unique environmental factors may contribute to lower premorbid IQ in psychotic disorders (Kendler et al. Reference Kendler2015, Reference Kendler2016a). Our work indicates that low-grade inflammation may be one such factor.

We have presented separate analyses for schizophrenia and ONAP to cover the entire spectrum of psychotic disorders. In young people, diagnosis can change over time, e.g. from other NAP to schizophrenia (Fusar-Poli et al. Reference Fusar-Poli2016). Longitudinal studies have reported that lower premorbid IQ (Kendler et al. Reference Kendler2016b), prenatal/ childhood infections (Brown et al. Reference Brown2004; Dalman et al. Reference Dalman2008; Blomstrom et al. Reference Blomstrom2016) are associated with schizophrenia and ONAP suggesting impaired neurodevelopment and immune-related risk factors are relevant for all of these disorders.

The results for an association between ESR and IQ are consistent with our previous Swedish population-based study based on a smaller sample of 49 321 participants (Karlsson et al. Reference Karlsson2010). The findings indicate that low-grade systemic inflammation, as indexed by ESR, might affect cognitive development/functioning. However, the cross-sectional nature of the ESR-IQ association means that reverse causality cannot be excluded (Luciano et al. Reference Luciano2009). In future, longitudinal studies are required. Cognitive impairments associated with acute inflammatory conditions observed in experimental studies of humans and animals may be part of a ‘sickness response’, which is mediated by effector molecules such as cytokines (Reichenberg et al. Reference Reichenberg2001). Chronic low-grade inflammation, as indexed by elevated concentrations of inflammatory markers, has also been associated with cognitive impairments in elderly individuals (Gorelick, Reference Gorelick2010) and with lower IQ in children (MacKinnon et al. Reference MacKinnon2017). In the elderly, such effects may involve vascular changes in the brain parenchyma induced by subtle but chronic elevations in inflammatory mediators associated with aging and other factors (Casserly & Topol, Reference Casserly and Topol2004). In children, however, potential mechanisms are less clear, and might involve effects of inflammatory cytokines on brain development and functioning (Spencer et al. Reference Spencer, Heida and Pittman2005; Asp et al. Reference Asp2009; McAfoose & Baune, Reference McAfoose and Baune2009; Liu et al. Reference Liu2014; Khandaker & Dantzer, Reference Khandaker and Dantzer2015).

Low-grade inflammation had an opposing effect on different types of psychotic disorders; higher ESR increased the risk of schizophrenia but decreased the risk of ONAP. A Previous study has shown that higher levels of IL-6 at age 9 are associated with increased risk for psychotic symptoms at 18 years (Khandaker et al. Reference Khandaker2014). Other studies have reported an association between higher levels of CRP at baseline and subsequent risk for schizophrenia at follow-up (Wium-Andersen et al. Reference Wium-Andersen, Orsted and Nordestgaard2013; Metcalf et al. Reference Metcalf2017). These findings are consistent with our results that ESR levels in the upper normal range are associated with increased risk for schizophrenia. Protective effect of high ESR for ONAP in our sample might be due to diverse diagnoses included in this group. In a Finnish longitudinal cohort study higher CRP in adolescence was associated with increased risk of schizophrenia, but not ONAP, in adulthood (Metcalf et al. Reference Metcalf2017). We previously reported an association between lower concentrations of acute phase proteins in neonatal blood spots and diagnosis of non-affective psychosis including schizophrenia in adulthood; but did not have power to examine schizophrenia and other psychosis separately (Gardner et al. Reference Gardner2013). Taken together, the current studies indicate that low-grade inflammation may increase the risk of schizophrenia, but its association with ONAP is less clear, indicating that further studies looking at specific types of psychoses are required.

Results from co-relative control analyses further refine our results of the ESR-psychoses and ESR-IQ associations. The association between ESR and schizophrenia was comparable between individuals in the general population and between members of full-sibling pairs, suggesting that shared familial factors are unlikely to explain these associations fully. This suggests unique environmental exposures rather than genetic or shared environmental factors may be important. This is consistent with a Danish population-based study that reported that polygenic risk scores for schizophrenia and a history of infections were independent risk-factors for schizophrenia (Benros et al. Reference Benros2016). As the results for cousin and half-sibling pairs were not significant, this analysis requires replication in a different, possibly even larger sample. The ESR-IQ association, however, clearly highlighted familial influence on this association. Population-based studies do suggest that susceptibility to infection may have a genetic contribution (Cooke & Hill, Reference Cooke and Hill2001; Obel et al. Reference Obel2010; Hwang et al. Reference Hwang2012), so the association of systemic inflammation and cognitive functioning might be largely based on genetic, but also shared environmental factors. Thus, our findings provide an interesting distinction: the association of low-grade inflammation with cognitive functioning might be due to shared familial factors, but its association with psychosis may be due to unique, environmental factors.

The results of our mediation analysis indicate that inflammation may influence the risk of psychosis by affecting neurodevelopment. Thus, immune and neurodevelopmental mechanisms for pathogenesis of psychosis could be potentially intertwined. ESR increased the risk for both schizophrenia and ONAP (indirectly via IQ), yet also lowered risk (directly) for ONAP. Statistically, this phenomenon has been described as suppression (MacKinnon et al. Reference MacKinnon, Krull and Lockwood2000), in which both risk and protective factors operate together. Suppression hints towards the presence of different, opposing biological pathways, where only the sum of the direct and indirect effects of a risk factor reflects its final contribution towards causation of an illness.

The results of this study need to be considered in light of its limitations. ESR is a marker of systemic inflammation and it represents activation of acute phase response due to various stimuli (e.g., infection, cancer). In future, studies of more sensitive and direct measures of innate immune activity, such as IL-6 or CRP, might be helpful. We carried out a number of sensitivity analyses to explore the ESR-psychosis relationships further. There was some evidence for an association between ESR and schizophrenia for participants in the top decile of ESR distribution but effects were not robust. The analyses presented here are based on male conscripts and might not be generalisable to women. However, previous studies did not indicate any notable sex difference for the association between inflammation and cognition (Khandaker et al. Reference Khandaker2015b; Gale et al. Reference Gale2016), or for the association between inflammation and psychoses (Khandaker et al. Reference Khandaker2014). Strengths of this study include a large population-based sample with data from reliable general population registers, long follow-up period, and innovative co-relative analyses that allowed exploration of shared familial confounding.

In conclusion, we report that lower IQ is associated with low-grade inflammation (cross-sectional), and with an increased risk of psychotic disorders (longitudinal) in adulthood. There is some evidence that inflammation may increase the risk of schizophrenia by affecting processes involved in cognitive development.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291718000831

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Dr Kenneth Kendler, Virginia Commonwealth University, USA, and Dr Henrik Ohlsson, Lund University, Sweden, for their advice on co-relative control analysis.

The authors have no financial interests to disclose with regard to the content of this study. PBJ received an honorarium, that he donated to his department, from Roche (UK) for taking part in an advisory board to advise on education about schizophrenia for psychiatrists. GMK is supported by an Intermediate Clinical Fellowship from the Wellcome Trust (201486/Z/16/Z) and a Clinical Lecturer Starter Grant from the Academy of Medical Sciences, UK (grant no. 80354). PBJ acknowledges grant support from the Wellcome Trust (095844/Z/11/Z & 088869/Z/09/Z) and NIHR (RP-PG-0616-20003, Cambridge Biomedical Research Centre and CLAHRC East of England). HK acknowledges support from the Stanley Medical Research Institute. The funding bodies had no role in design, conduct, analysis or reporting of this study.

Footnotes

*

Joint first authors

References

Ahlborg, B, et al. (1973) Ett hälso-och kapacitetsundersökningssystem för personalselektion. Försvarsmedicin 9, 3652.Google Scholar
Asp, L, et al. (2009) Neonatal infection with neurotropic influenza A virus affects working memory and expression of type III Nrg1 in adult mice. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity 23, 733741.Google Scholar
Benros, ME, et al. (2016) Influence of polygenic risk scores on the association between infections and schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry 80, 609616.Google Scholar
Blomstrom, A, et al. (2016) Associations between maternal infection during pregnancy, childhood infections, and the risk of subsequent psychotic disorder-A Swedish cohort study of nearly 2 million individuals. Schizophrenia Bulletin 42, 125133.Google Scholar
Brown, AS, et al. (2004) Serologic evidence of prenatal influenza in the etiology of schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 61, 774780.Google Scholar
Cannon, M, et al. (2002) Evidence for early-childhood, pan-developmental impairment specific to schizophreniform disorder: results from a longitudinal birth cohort. Archives of General Psychiatry 59, 449456.Google Scholar
Cannon, TD, et al. (2000) Childhood cognitive functioning in schizophrenia patients and their unaffected siblings: a prospective cohort study. Schizophrenia Bulletin 26, 379.Google Scholar
Casserly, I and Topol, EJ (2004) Convergence of atherosclerosis and Alzheimer's disease: inflammation, cholesterol, and misfolded proteins. The Lancet 363, 11391146.Google Scholar
Cooke, GS and Hill, AVS (2001) Genetics of susceptibility to human infectious disease. Nature Reviews Genetics 2, 967977.Google Scholar
Crow, TJ, Done, DJ and Sacker, A (1995) Childhood precursors of psychosis as clues to its evolutionary origins. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences 245, 6169.Google Scholar
Dalman, C, et al. (2008) Infections in the CNS during childhood and the risk of subsequent psychotic illness: a cohort study of more than One million Swedish subjects. American Journal of Psychiatry 165, 5965.Google Scholar
David, AS, et al. (1997) IQ and risk for schizophrenia: a population-based cohort study. Psychological Medicine 27, 13111323.Google Scholar
Davidson, M, et al. (1999) Behavioral and intellectual markers for schizophrenia in apparently healthy male adolescents. American Journal of Psychiatry 156, 13281335.Google Scholar
Dickerson, F, et al. (2007) C-reactive protein is associated with the severity of cognitive impairment but not of psychiatric symptoms in individuals with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 93, 261265.Google Scholar
Ferreira, RC, et al. (2013) Functional IL6R 358Ala allele impairs classical IL-6 receptor signaling and influences risk of diverse inflammatory diseases. PLoS Genetics 9, e1003444.Google Scholar
Fusar-Poli, P, et al. (2016) Diagnostic stability of ICD/DSM first episode psychosis diagnoses: meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Bulletin 42, 13951406.Google Scholar
Gale, SD, et al. (2016) Infectious disease burden and cognitive function in young to middle-aged adults. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity 52, 161168.Google Scholar
Gardner, RM, et al. (2013) Neonatal levels of acute phase proteins and later risk of non-affective psychosis. Macmillan Publishers Limited Translational Psychiatry 3, e228.Google Scholar
Goldsmith, DR, Rapaport, MH and Miller, BJ (2016) A meta-analysis of blood cytokine network alterations in psychiatric patients: comparisons between schizophrenia, bipolar disorder and depression. Molecular Psychiatry, epub ahead of print 21, 16961709.Google Scholar
Gorelick, PB (2010) Role of inflammation in cognitive impairment: results of observational epidemiological studies and clinical trials. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1207, 155162.Google Scholar
Harrison, M (2015) Erythrocyte sedimentation rate and C-reactive protein. NPS MedicineWise Australian Prescriber 38, 9394.Google Scholar
Hwang, AE, et al. (2012) Evidence of genetic susceptibility to infectious mononucleosis: a twin study. Epidemiology and Infection 140, 20892095.Google Scholar
IL6R Genetics Consortium and Emerging Risk Factors Collaboration (2012) Interleukin-6 receptor pathways in coronary heart disease: a collaborative meta-analysis of 82 studies. The Lancet 379, 12051213.Google Scholar
Jones, P, et al. (1994) Child developmental risk factors for adult schizophrenia in the British 1946 birth cohort. The Lancet 344, 13981402.Google Scholar
Karlsson, H, et al. (2010) Association between erythrocyte sedimentation rate and IQ in Swedish males aged 18–20. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity 24, 868873.Google Scholar
Kendler, KS, et al. (2016 a) Observed cognitive performance and deviation from familial cognitive aptitude at Age 16 years and ages 18 to 20 years and risk for schizophrenia and bipolar illness in a Swedish national sample. JAMA Psychiatry 73, 465471.Google Scholar
Kendler, KS, et al. (2016 b) A Swedish national prospective and Co-relative study of school achievement at Age 16, and risk for schizophrenia, other nonaffective psychosis, and bipolar illness. Schizophrenia Bulletin 42, 7786.Google Scholar
Kendler, KS, et al. (2015) IQ and schizophrenia in a Swedish national sample: their causal relationship and the interaction of IQ with genetic risk. American Journal of Psychiatry 172, 259265.Google Scholar
Khandaker, GM, et al. (2011) A quantitative meta-analysis of population-based studies of premorbid intelligence and schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 132, 220227.Google Scholar
Khandaker, GM, et al. (2015 a) Inflammation and immunity in schizophrenia: implications for pathophysiology and treatment. . Elsevier The Lancet Psychiatry 2, 258270.Google Scholar
Khandaker, GM and Dantzer, R (2015) Is there a role for immune-to-brain communication in schizophrenia? Psychopharmacology 233, 15591573.Google Scholar
Khandaker, GM, Dantzer, R and Jones, PB (2017 a) Immunopsychiatry: important facts. Psychological Medicine 47, 22292237.Google Scholar
Khandaker, GM, et al. (2014) Association of serum interleukin 6 and C-reactive protein in childhood with depression and psychosis in young adult life: a population-based longitudinal study. JAMA Psychiatry 71, 11211128.Google Scholar
Khandaker, GM, et al. (2015 b) A population-based prospective birth cohort study of childhood neurocognitive and psychological functioning in healthy survivors of early life meningitis. Annals of Epidemiology 25, 236242.Google Scholar
Khandaker, GM, et al. (2017 b). Association between a functional interleukin 6 receptor genetic variant and risk of depression and psychosis in a population-based birth cohort. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity 69, 264272.Google Scholar
Liu, XC, et al. (2014) Behavioral disturbances in adult mice following neonatal virus infection or kynurenine treatment–role of brain kynurenic acid. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity 36, 8089.Google Scholar
Luciano, M, et al. (2009) Reverse causation in the association between C-reactive protein and fibrinogen levels and cognitive abilities in an aging sample. Psychosomatic Medicine 71, 404409.Google Scholar
MacKinnon, DP, Krull, JL and Lockwood, CM (2000) Equivalence of the mediation, confounding and suppression effect. Prevention Science 1, 173.Google Scholar
MacKinnon, N, et al. (2017) Association between childhood infection, serum inflammatory markers and intelligence: findings from a population-based prospective birth cohort study. Epidemiology and Infection 146, 256264.Google Scholar
McAfoose, J and Baune, BT (2009) Evidence for a cytokine model of cognitive function. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 33, 355366.Google Scholar
Metcalf, SA, et al. (2017) Serum C-reactive protein in adolescence and risk of schizophrenia in adulthood: a prospective birth cohort study. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity 59, 253259.Google Scholar
Miller, BJ, et al. (2011) Meta-analysis of cytokine alterations in schizophrenia: clinical status and antipsychotic effects. Biological Psychiatry 70, 663671.Google Scholar
Murray, RM and Lewis, SW (1987) Is schizophrenia a neurodevelopmental disorder? British Medical Journal (Clinical Research Ed.) 295, 681682.Google Scholar
Obel, N, et al. (2010) Genetic and environmental influences on risk of death due to infections assessed in Danish twins, 1943–2001. American Journal of Epidemiology 171, 10071013.Google Scholar
Potvin, S, et al. (2008) Inflammatory cytokine alterations in schizophrenia: a systematic quantitative review. Biological Psychiatry 63, 801808.Google Scholar
Reichenberg, A, et al. (2005) Elaboration on premorbid intellectual performance in schizophrenia: premorbid intellectual decline and risk for schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 62, 12971304.Google Scholar
Reichenberg, A, et al. (2001) Cytokine-associated emotional and cognitive disturbances in humans. Archives of General Psychiatry 58, 445452.Google Scholar
Spencer, SJ, Heida, JG and Pittman, QJ (2005) Early life immune challenge--effects on behavioural indices of adult rat fear and anxiety. Behavioural Brain Research 164, 231238.Google Scholar
Weinberger, DR (1987) Implications of normal brain development for the pathogenesis of schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 44, 660669.Google Scholar
Wium-Andersen, MK, Orsted, DD and Nordestgaard, BG (2013) Elevated C-reactive protein associated With late- and very-late-onset schizophrenia in the general population: a prospective study. Schizophrenia Bulletin 40, 11171127.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (2004) International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems, vol. 1. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization.Google Scholar
Zammit, S, et al. (2004) A longitudinal study of premorbid IQ score and risk of developing schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, severe depression, and other nonaffective psychoses. Archives of General Psychiatry 61, 354360.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Baseline characteristics of the sample

Figure 1

Table 2. Association between ESR and IQ at age 18 years

Figure 2

Fig. 1. Association between ESR and schizophrenia (left), and other non-affective psychoses (right) (the group with ESR of 0–3 mm/h was used as the reference for all analyses).

Figure 3

Table 3. Association between ESR and subsequent schizophrenia and other non-affective psychoses

Figure 4

Table 4. Co-relative analyses for association between ESR, schizophrenia and other non-affective psychoses

Supplementary material: File

Kappelmann et al. supplementary material

Kappelmann et al. supplementary material 1

Download Kappelmann et al. supplementary material(File)
File 176.5 KB