Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-5g6vh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T02:37:36.809Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

12 - Indirect behavioral indicators and their uses in conservation and management

from Part IV - Behavioral indicators

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 April 2016

Oded Berger-Tal
Affiliation:
Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Israel
David Saltz
Affiliation:
Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Israel
Oded Berger-Tal
Affiliation:
Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Israel
David Saltz
Affiliation:
Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Israel
Get access

Summary

INTRODUCTION

Animals inhabit environments that are rapidly changing due to anthropogenic activities, such as the destruction and fragmentation of habitats, the introduction of exotic species and the alteration of local and global climate regimes. These changes are stretching the capacity of animals to cope, with conditions potentially outside the bounds of those experienced over the recent evolutionary history of the species. For managers of protected areas and endangered populations to respond in time to the threats posed by changing environments, these threats must be recognized early on, when still relatively benign, so as to be able to mitigate or counteract the adverse consequences of the altered environment. Coping takes place most immediately through behavioral responses, perhaps followed at a later stage by adjustments in physiology, and maybe over generational scales by shifts in morphology. This means that changes in animal behavior are potentially sensitive indicators that can provide the necessary early warning. Such behaviors must be documented in such a way that the changes will be revealed. In Chapter 11, Kotler et al. describe in detail how managers can use the behavior of animals as an indicator for their population's status and as a monitoring tool for the success of management programs aimed to assist these populations. However, behavioral indicators can tell us even more.

Animals in the wild do not live their life in isolation. They are a part of a complex ecosystem that includes many different species as well as various abiotic features. All species in a given system interact, to some extent, either directly or indirectly. There are many types of biological interactions between species: The most common ones are competition (either direct competition through interference or indirect through the utilization of shared resources), predation, parasitism and mutualism (e.g. pollination or seed dispersal). In addition, animals interact with their abiotic environment. The environment provides resources such as food and shelter, as well as constraints that may limit the behavior of animals (e.g. barriers that limit movement, the chemical composition of the water that can affect the behavior of aquatic species or noise that can limit communication between individuals). There is a constant feedback between animals and their environment, which, depending on existing conditions, may be either positive or negative.

Type
Chapter
Information
Conservation Behavior
Applying Behavioral Ecology to Wildlife Conservation and Management
, pp. 352 - 375
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ale, S.B. and Brown, J.S. 2009. Prey behavior leads to predator: a case study of the himalayan tahr and the snow leopard in Sagarmatha (Mt. Everest) National Park, Nepal. Israel Journal of Ecology and Evolution, 55:315–327.Google Scholar
Apaloo, J., Brown, J.S. and Vincent, T.L. 2009. Evolutionary game theory: ESS, convergence stability, and NIS. Evolutionary Ecology Research, 11:489–515.Google Scholar
Baum, K.A. and Grant, W.E. 2001. Hummingbird foraging behavior in different patch types: simulation of alternative strategies. Ecological Modelling, 137:201–209.Google Scholar
Bedoya-Perez, M.A., Carthy, A.J.R., Mella, V.S.A., McArthur, C. and Banks, P.B. 2013. A practical guide to avoid giving up on giving-up densities. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 67:1541–1553.Google Scholar
Berger-Tal, O., Mukherjee, S., Kotler, B.P. and Brown, J.S. 2009. Look before you leap: is risk of injury a foraging cost?Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 63:1821–1827.Google Scholar
Berger-Tal, O., Polak, T., Oron, A., Lubin, Y., Kotler, B.P. and Saltz, D. 2011. Integrating animal behavior and conservation biology: a conceptual framework. Behavioral Ecology, 22:236–239.Google Scholar
Binkley, D., Moore, M.M., Romme, W.H. and Brown, P.M. 2006. Was Aldo Leopold right about the Kaibab deer herd?Ecosystems, 9:227–241.Google Scholar
Boyce, M.S. and McDonald, L.L. 1999. Relating populations to habitats using resource selection functions. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 14:268–272.Google Scholar
Boyce, M.S., Vernier, P.R., Nielsen, S.E. and Schmiegelow, F.K.A. 2002. Evaluating resource selection functions. Ecological Modelling, 157:281–300.Google Scholar
Brown, J.S. 1988. Patch use as an indicator of habitat preference, predation risk, and competition. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 22:37–47.Google Scholar
Brown, J.S. 1989a. Desert rodent community structure: a test of 4 mechanisms of coexistence. Ecological Monographs, 59:1–20.Google Scholar
Brown, J.S. 1989b. Coexistence on a seasonal resource. American Naturalist, 133:168–182.Google Scholar
Brown, J.S. 1992. Patch use under predation risk. I. Models and predictions. Annales Zoologici Finnici, 29:301–309.Google Scholar
Brown, J.S. 1999. Vigilance, patch use and habitat selection: foraging under predation risk. Evolutionary Ecology Research, 1:49–71.Google Scholar
Brown, J.S., Arel, Y., Abramsky, Z. and Kotler, B.P. 1992. Patch use by gerbils (Gerbillus allenbyi) in sandy and rocky habitats. Journal of Mammalogy, 73:821–829.Google Scholar
Brown, J.S., Kotler, B.P. and Mitchell, W.A. 1994. Foraging theory, patch use, and the structure of a Negev Desert granivore community. Ecology, 75:2286–2300.Google Scholar
Brown, J.S., Laundré, J.W. and Gurung, M. 1999. The ecology of fear: optimal foraging, game theory, and trophic interactions. Journal of Mammalogy, 80:385–399.Google Scholar
Brown, J.S. and Kotler, B.P. 2004. Hazardous duty pay and the foraging cost of predation. Ecology Letters, 7:999–1014.Google Scholar
Brown, J.S. and Kotler, B.P. 2007. Foraging and the ecology of fear. In Stephens, D.W., Brown, J.S. and Ydenberg, R.C. (eds.), Foraging: Behavior and Ecology, pp. 437–482. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Cagnacci, F., Boitani, L., Powell, R.A. and Boyce, M.S. 2010. Challenges and opportunities of using GPS-based location data in animal ecology. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 365:2157–2312.Google Scholar
Charnov, E.L. 1976. Optimal foraging, marginal value theorem. Theoretical Population Biology, 9:129–136.Google Scholar
Ciuti, S., Northrup, J.M., Muhly, T.B., Simi, S., Musiani, M., Pitt, J.A. and Boyce, M.S. 2012. Effects of humans on behaviour of wildlife exceed those of natural predators in a landscape of fear. PLoS ONE, 7:e50611.Google Scholar
Courchamp, F., Berec, L. and Gascoigne, J. 2008. Allee Effects in Ecology and Conservation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Cressman, R. and Křivan, V. 2010. The ideal free distribution as an evolutionarily stable state in density-dependent population games. Oikos, 119:1231–1242.Google Scholar
Dalke, P.D. and Sime, P.R. 1941. Food habits of the eastern and New England cottontails. Journal of Wildlife Management, 5:216–228.Google Scholar
Dall, S.R.X., Kotler, B.P. and Bouskila, A. 2001. Attention, apprehension, and gerbils searching in patches. Annales Zoologica Fennica, 38:15–23.Google Scholar
Davis, J.M. and Stamps, J.A. 2004. The effect of natal experience on habitat preferences. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 19:411–416.Google Scholar
Druce, D.J., Brown, J.S., Castley, J.C., Kerley, G.I.H., Kotler, B.P. and Slotow, R. 2006. Scale-dependent foraging costs: habitat use by rock hyraxes (Procavia capensis) determined by giving-up densities. Oikos, 115:513–525.Google Scholar
Druce, D.J., Brown, J.S., Kerley, G.I.H., Kotler, B.P., Mackey, R.A. and Slotow, R. 2009. Spatial and temporal scaling in habitat utilization by klipspringers (Oreotragus oreotragus) determined using giving-up densities. Austral Ecology, 34:577–587.Google Scholar
Dwernychuk, L.W. & Boag, D.A. 1972. Ducks nesting in association with gulls – an ecological trap?Canadian Journal of Zoology, 50:559–563.Google Scholar
Emerson, S.E. and Brown, J.S. 2012. Using giving up densities to test for dietary preferences in primates: an example with samango monkeys (Cercopithecus (nictitans) mitis erythrarchus). International Journal of Primatology, 33:1420–1438.Google Scholar
Errington, P.L. 1963. Muskrat Populations. Iowa State University Press, Ames, Iowa, USA.664 pp.
Fortin, D., Morris, D.W. and McLoughlin, P.D. 2008. Habitat selection and the evolution of specialists in heterogeneous environments. Israel Journal of Ecology & Evolution, 54:311–328.Google Scholar
Fortin, D., Fortin, M.E., Beyer, H.L., Duchesne, T., Courant, S. and Dancose, K. 2009. Group-size-mediated habitat selection and group fusion-fission dynamics of bison under predation risk. Ecology, 90:2480–2490.Google Scholar
Fretwell, S.D. and Lucas, H.L. Jr. 1969. On territorial behavior and other factors influencing habitat distribution in birds. Acta Biotheoretica, 19:16–36.Google Scholar
Fryxell, J.M, Vamosi, S.M., Walton, R.A. and Doucet, C.M. 1994. Retention time and the functional response of beavers. Oikos, 71:207–214.Google Scholar
Gill, J.A., Sutherland, W.J. and Norris, K. 2001. Depletion models can predict shorebird distribution at different spatial scales. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences, 268:369–376.Google Scholar
Gilliam, J.F. and Fraser, D.F. 1987. Habitat selection under predation hazard: a test of a model with foraging minnows. Ecology, 68:1856–1862.Google Scholar
Grafen, A. 1982. How not to measure inclusive fitness. Nature, 298:425–426.Google Scholar
Gregory, S.D. and Courchamp, F. 2010. Safety in numbers: extinction arising from predator-drive Allee effects. Journal of Animal Ecology, 79:511–514.Google Scholar
Green, C.M. and Stamps, J.A. 2001. Habitat selection at low population densities. Ecology, 82:2091–2100.Google Scholar
Greenberg, J. 2014. A Feathered River across the Sky: The Passenger Pigeon's Flight to Extinction.New York: Bloomsbury Publishing.
Gyimesi, A., Varghese, S., De Leeuw, J. and Nolet, B.A. 2012. Net energy intake rate as a common currency to explain swan spatial distribution in a shallow lake. Wetlands, 32:119–127.Google Scholar
Hawlena, D., Saltz, D., Abramsky, Z. and Buskila, A. 2010. Ecological trap for desert lizards caused by anthropogenic changes to habitat structure that favor predator activity. Conservation Biology, 24:803–809.Google Scholar
Hochman, V. and Kotler, B.P. 2006. Effects of food quality, diet preference and water on patch use by Nubian ibex. Oikos, 112:547–554.Google Scholar
Hoffmeister, D.F. 1989. Mammals of Illinois. Champaign: University of Illinois Press.
Houston, A.I. and McNamara, J.M. 1999. Models of Adaptive Behavior. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Huntly, N.J. 1987. Influence of refuging consumers (pikas – ochotona-princeps) on sub-alpine meadow vegetation. Ecology, 68:12–26.Google Scholar
Iribarren, C. and Kotler, B.P. 2012a. Patch use and vigilance behavior by Nubian ibex: the role of the effectiveness of vigilance. Evolutionary Ecology Research, 14:223–234.Google Scholar
Iribarren, C. and Kotler, B.P. 2012b. Foraging patterns of habitat use reveal Nubian ibex’ landscape of fear. Wildlife Biology, 18:194–201.Google Scholar
Kjellander, P. and Nordström, J. 2003. Cyclic voles, prey switching in red fox, and roe deer dynamics – a test of the alternative prey hypothesis. Oikos, 101:338–344.Google Scholar
Kokko, H. and Sutherland, W.J. 2001. Ecological traps in changing environments: ecological and evolutionary consequences of a behaviorally mediated Allee effect. Evolutionary Ecology Research, 3:537–551.Google Scholar
Kotler, B.P. 1985. Owl predation on desert rodents which differ in morphology and behavior. Journal of Mammalogy, 66:824–828.Google Scholar
Kotler, B.P. and Brown, J.S. 1990. Rates of seed harvest by two species of gerbilline rodents. Journal of Mammalogy, 71:591–596.Google Scholar
Kotler, B.P., Brown, J.S. and Hasson, O. 1991. Factors affecting gerbil foraging behavior and rates of owl predation. Ecology, 72:2249–2260.Google Scholar
Kotler, B.P., Brown, J.S and Mitchell, W.A. 1993. Environmental factors affecting patch use in gerbilline rodents. Journal of Mammalogy, 74:614–620.Google Scholar
Kotler, B.P., Brown, J.S. and Subach, A. 1993. Mechanisms of coexistence of optimal foragers: temporal partitioning in two species of sand dune dwelling gerbils. Oikos, 67:548–556.Google Scholar
Kotler, B.P., Gross, J.E. and Mitchell, W.A. 1994. Applying patch use in Nubian ibex to measure resource assessment ability, diet selection, indirect interactions between food plants, and predatory risk. Journal of Wildlife Management, 58:300–308.Google Scholar
Kotler, B.P., Dickman, C.R. and Brown, J.S. 1998. The effects of water on patch use in arid-zone birds and rodents in the Simpson Desert, central Australia. Australian Journal of Ecology, 23:574–578.Google Scholar
Kotler, B.P., Brown, J.S., Oldfield, A., Thorson, J. and Cohen, D. 2001. Patch use in three species of gerbils in a risky environment: the role of escape substrate and foraging substrate. Ecology, 82:1781–1790.Google Scholar
Kotler, B.P., Brown, J.S., Dall, S.R.X., Gresser, S., Ganey, D. and Bouskila, A. 2002. Foraging games between owls and gerbils: temporal dynamics of resource depletion and apprehension in gerbils. Evolutionary Ecology Research, 4:495–518.Google Scholar
Kotler, B.P. and Brown, J.S. 2007. Community Ecology. In Stephens, D.W., Brown, J.S. and Ydenberg, R.C. (eds.) Foraging: Behavior and Ecology, pp. 397–436. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Kotler, B.P., Morris, D.W. and Brown, J.S. 2007. Behavioral indicators and conservation: wielding “the biologist's tricorder.”Israel Journal of Ecology and Evolution, 53:237–244.Google Scholar
Kotler, B.P., Brown, J.S., Mukherjee, S., Berger-Tal, O. and Bouskila, A.. 2010. Moonlight avoidance in gerbils reveals a sophisticated interplay among time allocation, vigilance, and state-dependent foraging. Proceeding of the Royal Society of London B, 277:1469–1474.Google Scholar
Kramer, A.M. and Drake, J.M. 2010. Experimental demonstration of population extinction due to a predator-driven Allee effect. Journal of Animal Ecology, 79:633–639.Google Scholar
Laundre, J.W., Hernandez, L. and Altendorf, K.B. 2001. Wolves, elk, and bison: re-establishing the “landscape of fear” in Yellowstone National Park, USA. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 79:1401–1409.Google Scholar
Laundre, J.W. 2010. Behavioral response races, predator–prey shell games, ecology of fear, and patch use of pumas and their ungulate prey. Ecology, 91:2995–3007.Google Scholar
Lucas, J.R. and Grafen, A. 1985. Partial prey consumption by ambush predators. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 113:455–473.Google Scholar
Mabry, K.E. and Stamps, J.A. 2008. Dispersing brush mice prefer habitat like home. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 275:543–548.Google Scholar
Manly, B.F. J., McDonald, L.L., Thomas, D.L., McDonald, T.L. and Wallace, P.E. 2002. Resource Selection by Animals: Statistical Design and Analysis for Field Studies, 2nd Edition. Boston, MA: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
McArthur, C., Orlando, P., Banks, P.B. and Brown, J.S. 2012. The foraging tight rope between predation risk and plant toxins: a matter of concentration. Functional Ecology, 26:74–83.Google Scholar
McLoughlin, P.D., Morris, D.W., Fortin, D., VanderWal, E. and Contasti, A.L. 2010. Considering ecological dynamics in resource selection functions. Journal of Animal Ecology, 79:4–12.Google Scholar
Messier, F., Virgl, J.A. and Marinelli, L. 1990. Density-dependent habitat selection in muskrats – a test of the ideal free distribution model. Oecologia, 84:380–385.Google Scholar
Mitchell, W.A. and Valone, T.J. 1990. The optimization research program: studying adaptations by their functions. Quarterly Review of Biology, 65:43–52Google Scholar
Morris, D.W. 1987. Tests of density dependent habitat selection in a patchy environment. Ecological Monographs, 57:269281.Google Scholar
Morris, D.W. 1988. Habitat dependent population regulation and community structure. Evolutionary Ecology, 2:253269.Google Scholar
Morris, D.W. 1999. Has the ghost of competition passed?Evolutionary Ecology Research, 1:3–20.Google Scholar
Morris, D.W. 2002. Measuring the Allee effect: positive density dependence in small mammals. Ecology, 83: 14–20.Google Scholar
Morris, D.W. 2003a. Toward an ecological synthesis: a case for habitat selection. Oecologia, 136: 1–13.Google Scholar
Morris, D.W. 2003b. How can we apply theories of habitat selection to wildlife conservation and management?Wildlife Research, 30:303–319.Google Scholar
Morris, D.W. 2011. Adaptation, habitat selection, and the eco-evolutionary process. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 278:2401–2411.Google Scholar
Morris, D.W. 2014. Can foraging behaviour reveal the eco-evolutionary dynamics of habitat selection?Evolutionary Ecology Research, 16:1–18.Google Scholar
Morris, D.W. and Mukherjee, S. 2007. Can we measure carrying capacity with foraging behavior?Ecology, 88:597–604.Google Scholar
Morris, D.W., Kotler, B.P., Brown, J.S., Ale, S.B. and Sundararaj, V. 2009. Behavioral indicators for conserving mammal diversity. The Year in Ecology and Conservation Biology, Annals of The New York Academy of Sciences, 1162:334–356.Google Scholar
Morris, D.W., Moore, D. E., Ale, S. B. and Dupuch, A. 2011. Forecasting ecological and evolutionary strategies to global change: an example from habitat selection by lemmings. Global Change Biology, 17:1266–1276.Google Scholar
Morris, D.W. and Dupuch, A. 2012. Habitat change and the scale of habitat selection: shifting gradients used by coexisting Arctic rodents. Oikos, 121:975–984.Google Scholar
Morris, D.W., Dupuch, A. and Halliday, W.D. 2012. Climate induced habitat selection predicts future evolutionary strategies of lemmings. Evolutionary Ecology Research, 14:689–705.Google Scholar
Nelson, R., Keller, C. and Ratnaswamy, M. 2005. Estimating the extent of Delmarva fox squirrel habitat using an airborne LiDAR profiler. Remote Sensing of Environment, 96:292–301.Google Scholar
Nolet, B.A., 2006. The use of a flexible patch leaving rule under exploitative competition: a field test with swans. Oikos, 112: 342–352.Google Scholar
Nolet, B.A., Gyimesi, A. and Klaassen, R.H. 2006. Prediction of bird‐day carrying capacity on a staging site: a test of depletion models. Journal of Animal Ecology, 75:1285–1292.Google Scholar
Nolet, B.A. and Klaassen, M. 2009. Retrodicting patch use by foraging swans in a heterogeneous environment using a set of functional responses. Oikos, 118:431–439.Google Scholar
Olsson, O., Wiktander, U., Holmgren, N.M.A. and Nilsson, S.G. 1999. Gaining ecological information about Bayesian foragers through their behaviour. II. A field test with woodpeckers.Oikos, 87:264–276.Google Scholar
Olsson, O., Molokwyu, M. and Ngozi, M. 2007. On the missed opportunity cost, GUD, and estimating environmental quality. Israel Journal of Ecology and Evolution, 53:263–278.Google Scholar
Olsson, O., Brown, J.S. and Heft, K.L. 2008. A guide to central place effects in foraging. Theoretical Population Biology, 74:22–33.Google Scholar
Oyugi, J.O., Brown, J.S. and Whelan, C.J. 2012. Foraging behavior and coexistence of two sunbird species in a Kenyan woodland. Biotropica, 44:262–269.Google Scholar
Patten, M.A. and Kelly, J.F. 2010. Habitat selection and the perceptual trap. Ecological Applications, 20: 2148–2156.Google Scholar
Perrin, M.R. and Kotler, B.P. 2005. A test of five mechanisms of species coexistence between rodents in a southern African savanna. African Zoology, 40:55–61.Google Scholar
Pulliam, H.R. 1974. On the theory of optimal diets. American Naturalist, 108:59–74.Google Scholar
Reid, C. 2004. Habitat suitability and behavior of springbok (Antidorcas marsupialis) at Augrabies Falls National Park, South Africa. University of Port Elizabeth, M. Sc.thesis. pp.
Rieuceu, G., Vickery, W.L. and Doucet, G.J. 2009. A patch use model to separate effects of foraging costs on giving-up densities: an experiment with white-tailed deer (Odocoileus virginianus). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 63:891–897.Google Scholar
Rosenzweig, M.L. 1979. Optimal habitat selection in two-species competitive systems. Fortschritte der Zoologie, 25:283–293.Google Scholar
Rosenzweig, M.L. 1981. A theory of habitat selection. Ecology, 62:327–335.Google Scholar
Sanchez, F. 2006. Harvest rates and patch-use strategy of Egyptian fruit bats in artificial food patches. Journal of Mammalogy, 87:1140–1144.Google Scholar
Sanchez, F., Kotler, B.P., Korine, C. and Pinshow, B. 2008a. Sugars are complementary resources to ethanol in foods consumed by Egyptian fruit bats. Journal of Experimental Biology, 211: 1475–1481.Google Scholar
Sanchez, F, Kotler, B.P., Korine, C. and Pinshow, B. 2008b. Ethanol and the foraging behavior of Egyptian fruit bats, Rousettus aegyptiacus. Naturwissenschaften, 95: 561–567.Google Scholar
Schlaepfer, M.A., Runge, M.C. and Sherman, P.W. 2002. Ecological and evolutionary traps. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 17:474–480.Google Scholar
Schmidt, K.A., Brown, J.S. and Morgan, R.A. 1998. Plant defenses as complementary resources: a test with squirrels. Oikos, 81:130–142.Google Scholar
Shrader, A.M, Kotler, B.P., Brown, J.S. and Kerley, . 2008. Providing water for goats in arid landscapes: effects of feeding effort with regards to time period, herd size, and secondary compounds. Oikos, 117:446–472.Google Scholar
Shochat, E., Patten, M.A., Morris, D.W., Reinking, D.L., Wolfe, D.H. and Sherrod, S.K. 2005. Ecological traps in isodars: effects of tallgrass prairie management on bird nest success. Oikos, 111:159–169.Google Scholar
Sih, A. 1980. Optimal behavior – can foragers balance 2 conflicting demands?Science, 210:1041–1043.Google Scholar
Speziale, K.L., Lambertucci, S.O. and Olsson, O. 2008. Disturbance from roads negatively affects Andean condor habitat use. Biological Conservation, 141:1765–1772.Google Scholar
Stamps, J.A. 2001. Habitat selection by dispersers: integrating proximate and ultimate approaches. In Clober, J., Danchin, E., Dhondt, A.A. and Nichols, J.D. (eds.) Dispersal, pp. 230–242, New York: Oxford University Press.
Stephens, D.W. and Krebs, J.R. 1986. Foraging Theory.Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Sutherland, W.J. 1996. From Individual Behavior to Population Ecology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Tadesse, S.A. and Kotler, B.P. 2012. Impact of tourism on Nubian ibex revealed through assessment of behavioral indicators. Behavioral Ecology, 23:1257–1262.Google Scholar
Tadesse, S.A. and Kotler, B.P. 2013. Habitat use by mountain nyala Tragelaphus buxtoni determined using stem bite diameters at point of browse, bite rates, and time budgets in the Bale Mountains National Park, Ethiopia. Current Zoology, 59:707–717.Google Scholar
Tilman, T. 1988. Plant Strategies and the Dynamics and Structure of Plant Communities. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
van der Merwe, M. and Brown, J.S. 2007. Foraging ecology of North American tree squirrels on cacheable and less cacheable foods: a comparison of two urban habitats. Evolutionary Ecology Research, 9:705–716.Google Scholar
van der Merwe, M. and Brown, J.S. 2008. Mapping the landscape of fear of the cape ground squirrel (Xerus inauris). Journal of Mammalogy, 89:1162–1169.Google Scholar
van Gils, J.A., Edelaar, P., Escudero, G. and Pierma, T. 2004. Carrying capacity models should not use fixed prey density thresholds: a plea for using more tools of behavioral ecology. Oikos, 104:197–204.Google Scholar
van Gils, J.A., Kraan, C., Dekinga, A., Drent, J., de Goeij, P. and Pierma, T. 2009. Reversed optimality and predictive ecology: burrowing depth forecasts population change in a bivalve. Biology Letters, 5:5–8.Google Scholar
van Horne, B. 1983. Density as a misleading indicator of habitat quality. Journal of Wildlife Management, 47:893–901.Google Scholar
Vickery, W.L., Rieucau, G. and Doucet, G.J. 2010. Comparing habitat quality within and between environments using giving up densities: an example based on the winter habitat of white-tailed deer Odocoileus virginianus.Oikos, 120:999–1004.Google Scholar
Watkinson, A.R. and Sutherland, W.J. 1995. Sources, sinks and pseudo-sinks. Journal of Animal Ecology, 64:126–130.Google Scholar
Wecker, S.C. 1963. The role of early experience in habitat selection by the prairie deer mouse, Peromyscus maniculatus baridi.Ecological Monographs, 33:307–325.Google Scholar
Yip, S.J.S., Rich, M.A. and Dickman, C.R. 2015. Diet of the feral cat, Felis catus, in central Australian grassland habitats during population cycles of its principal prey. Mammal Research, 60:39–50.Google Scholar
Ziv, Y., Abramsky, Z., Kotelr, B.P. and Subach, A. 1993. Interference competition and temporal and habitat partitioning in two gerbil species. Oikos, 66:237–246.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×