Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-wzw2p Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-17T13:14:52.804Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

2 - The development of body representations

the integration of visual-proprioceptive information

from Part I - The bodily self

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 October 2011

Virginia Slaughter
Affiliation:
University of Queensland
Celia A. Brownell
Affiliation:
University of Pittsburgh
Get access

Summary

Body representations may be considered in terms of both their first- and third-person characteristics. By first-person, we mean those aspects of body awareness and knowledge that are typically exclusively available to the “owner” of the body – the self. By third-person, we mean those aspects of body awareness and knowledge that are at least potentially available to any observer. Although there is overlap between first- and third-person characteristics – I can observe my own hand movements as I type just as anyone who happens to be watching me type can also observe these movements – there are also some qualitatively distinct characteristics. Most importantly, the owner of the body gets various kinds of information in a qualitatively different form and in some sense “privately.” For example, the proprioceptive sense yields a type of first-person information that allows for awareness of one’s movement and the relative position of various parts of one’s body. Thus, even without visual information, it is possible to determine fairly accurately both dynamic and spatial aspects of one’s body parts through proprioception. And so, in the dark, I know a lot more about my body than you do.

At least in humans, however, bodies are represented simultaneously in terms of both first- and third-person characteristics. Bodies have both externally observable properties and internal conditions. As adults, our experience and representation of our own bodies, for the most part, consist of integrated multisensory input. That is, when we perform an arm movement, we do not perceive the visual and proprioceptive information as separate yet correlated, but instead as unified and integrated. Similarly, the observed movement of our faces when we look in a mirror is perceived in integration with felt movement of our face. This integration depends upon the perfect temporal coincidence of the relevant information. In illustration, we know that adults will misperceive a fake rubber hand as their own under conditions where they are presented with a tight temporal correlation between first person information (either tactile or proprioceptive) from their own real hand and “third person” visual information from the rubber hand (e.g. Botvinick and Cohen, 1998; Dummer et al., 2009).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Amsterdam, B. 1972 Mirror self-image reactions before age twoDevelopmental Psychobiology 5 297Google Scholar
Bahrick, L. E.Lickliter, R. 2000 Intersensory redundancy guides attentional selectivity and perceptual learning in infancyDevelopmental Psychology 36 190Google Scholar
Bahrick, L. E.Watson, J. S. 1985 Detection of intermodal proprioceptive-visual contingency as a potential basis of self-perception in infancyDevelopmental Psychology 21 963Google Scholar
Barresi, J.Moore, C. 1996 Intentional relations and social understandingBehavioral and Brain Sciences 19 107Google Scholar
Barth, J.Povinelli, D.Cant, J. 2004 Bodily origins of SELFBeike, D.Lampinen, J.Behrend, D.The Self and MemoryNew YorkPsychology Press
Botvinick, M.Cohen, J. 1998 Rubber hands “feel” touch that eyes seeNature 391 756Google Scholar
Brownell, C.Zerwas, S.Ramani, G. 2007 So big”: The development of body self-awareness in toddlersChild Development 78 1,426Google Scholar
Collins, S.Moore, C. 2008 The Temporal Parameters of Visual-proprioceptive Perception in InfancyPoster presented at the International Conference on Infant Studies, VancouverCanada
Dummer, T.Picot-Annand, A.Neal, T.Moore, C. 2009 Movement and the rubber hand illusionPerception 38 271Google Scholar
Hiraki, K. 2006 Detecting contingency: A key to understanding development of self and social cognitionJapanese Psychological Research 48 204Google Scholar
Leube, D. T.Knoblich, G.Erb, M.Grodd, W.Bartels, M.Kricher, T. T. 2003 The neural correlates of perceiving one’s own movementsNeuroImage 20 2,084Google Scholar
Lewis, M.Brooks-Gunn, J. 1979 Social Cognition and the Acquisition of SelfNew York; LondonPlenum Press
Lewkowicz, D. J. 1996 Perception of auditory-visual temporal synchrony in human infantsJournal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 22 1,094Google Scholar
Lewkowicz, D. J. 2000 The development of intersensory temporal perception: An epigenetic systems/limitations viewPsychological Bulletin 126 281Google Scholar
Mitchell, R. W. 1993 Mental models of mirror-self-recognition: Two theoriesNew Ideas in Psychology 11 295Google Scholar
Miyazaki, M.Hiraki, K. 2006 Delayed intermodal contingency affects young children’s recognition of their current selfChild Development 77 736Google Scholar
Moore, C. 2006 The Development of Commonsense PsychologyMahwah, NJLawrence Erlbaum Associates
Moore, C. 2007 Understanding self and other in the second yearBrownell, C. A.Kopp, C. B.Transitions in Early Socioemotional Development: The Toddler YearsNew YorkGuilford Press
Moore, C.Mealiea, J.Garon, N.Povinelli, D. 2007 The development of body self-awarenessInfancy 11 157Google Scholar
Papousek, H.Papousek, M. 1974 Mirror image and self-recognition in young human infants: I. A method of experimental analysisDevelopmental Psychobiology 7 149Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. 1995 The unduplicated selfRochat, P.The Self in Infancy: Theory and ResearchAmsterdam, NetherlandsNorth-Holland/Elsevier Science Publishers
Povinelli, D.  J.Simon, B.  B. 1998 Young children’s reactions to briefly versus extremely delayed images of the self: Emergence of the autobiographical stanceDevelopmental Psychology 43 188Google Scholar
Povinelli, D.Landau, K.Perilloux, H. 1996 Self-recognition in young children using delayed versus live feedback: Evidence of a developmental asynchronyChild Development 67 1,540Google Scholar
Rochat, P.Morgan, R. 1995 Spatial determinants in the perception of self-produced leg movements by 3- to 5-month-old infantsDevelopmental Psychology 31 626Google Scholar
Rochat, P.Striano, T. 2000 Perceived self in infancyInfant Behavior and Development 23 513Google Scholar
Rovee-Collier, C. 1987 Learning and memory in infancyOsofsky, J. D.Handbook of Infant DevelopmentOxford, UKJohn Wiley & Sons
Schmuckler, M. A. 1996 Visual-proprioceptive intermodal perception in infancyInfant Behavior and Development 19 221Google Scholar
Spelke, E. 1976 Infants’ intermodal perception of eventsCognitive Psychology 8 553Google Scholar
Spelke, E. 1979 Perceiving bimodally specified events in infancyDevelopmental Psychology 15 626Google Scholar
Spelke, E.Owsley, C. 1979 Intermodal exploration and knowledge in infancyInfant Behavior and Development 2 13Google Scholar
Watson, J.Ramey, C. 1972 Reactions to response-contingent stimulation in early infancyMerrill–Palmer Quarterly 18 219Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×