Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-9pm4c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T13:49:20.942Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

3 - Trade-offs in ecological experimentation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2015

Nelson G. Hairston
Affiliation:
University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill
Get access

Summary

Introduction

Ecological experimentation has some characteristics in common with mathematical modeling in ecology, which has been stated by Levins (1968) to face the impossibility of simultaneously maximizing precision, realism, and generality [see a discussion of this problem by Hunt & Doyle (1984)]. Ecologists conducting experiments face analogous choices, in that providing confidence in a relevant ecological process may preclude its general application, or the requirements of a sophisticated experimental design may severely decrease realism, or the use of an elaborate design in the field may put the necessary amount of replication beyond the resources of the investigator. In each case, the ecologist faces a difficult choice. All of the choices should be made deliberately, because to let them be made by default can lead to a misinterpretation or to such an unfortunate effect as replication insufficient to yield a convincing (that is, statistically significant) result.

Generality versus confidence

Conclusions reached from ecological studies increase in value as they can be extended to more and more situations. Therefore, any factors reducing this generality should also incorporate sufficient benefits to offset the loss. Experience over a wide range of habitats might be offset by a thorough knowledge of the ecological processes taking place in one or a few. Anyone undertaking ecological experiments is faced with this dilemma. The experiments are needed to test hypotheses about the processes, but they necessarily limit the results of the tests to one or a few localities.

Type
Chapter
Information
Ecological Experiments
Purpose, Design and Execution
, pp. 52 - 66
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 1989

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×