Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-r6qrq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T06:12:08.659Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

20 - Rethinking culture and the self: some basic principles and their implications

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Chi-Yue Chiu
Affiliation:
Nanyang Technological University, China, and University of Illinois, USA
Young-hoon Kim
Affiliation:
University of Illinois, USA
Fons J. R. van de Vijver
Affiliation:
Universiteit van Tilburg, The Netherlands
Athanasios Chasiotis
Affiliation:
Universiteit van Tilburg, The Netherlands
Seger M. Breugelmans
Affiliation:
Universiteit van Tilburg, The Netherlands
Get access

Summary

Culture and the self is one of the most widely researched topics in cross-cultural and cultural psychology. Based on the assumption that there are marked differences in the conception of the self in Eastern and Western cultures (Markus and Kitayama, 1991), research has examined and uncovered many East–West differences in basic self processes (Chiu and Hong, 2006, 2007; Lehman, Chiu and Schaller, 2004). These discoveries have led some investigators to conclude that self processes are culture-dependent (Heine et al., 1999). In response to this claim and to defend the universal nature of basic self processes, some investigators (Sedikides, Gaertner and Toguchi, 2003; Sedikides, Gaertner and Vevea, 2005, 2007) have initiated a dialogue on whether cultures differ only in the expressions of the same (universal) self processes.

This debate over whether culture leaves deep imprint on self processes is particularly germane to two fundamental questions in cross-cultural psychology. First, what is the relationship between culture and the individual? Does culture shape how individuals conceptualise their ontological existence and essences? Second, how deep are cross-cultural differences in psychological functioning? Do individuals fully internalise cultural norms? When individuals display culturally appropriate behaviours, are they just being themselves, or do they feel pressurised to conform to normative expectations?

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allport, G. (1955). Becoming: Basic considerations for a psychology of personality. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (2002). Social cognitive theory in cultural context. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 51, 269–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., and Tice, D. M. (1985). Self-esteem and responses to success and failure: Subsequent performance and intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality, 53, 450–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bempechat, J., and Drago-Severson, E. (1999). Cross-national differences in academic achievement: Beyond etic conceptions of children's understandings. Review of Educational Research, 69, 287–314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berry, J. W., Poortinga, Y. H., Segall, M. H., and Dasen, P. R. (2002). Cross-cultural psychology: Research and applications. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bond, M. H., and Cheung, T.-S. (1983). College students' spontaneous self-concept: The effect of culture among respondents in Hong Kong, Japan, and the United States. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 14, 153–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bond, M. H., Leung, K., and Wan, K.-c. (1982). The social impact of self-effacing attributions: The Chinese case. Journal of Social Psychology, 118, 157–166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brumann, C. (1999). Writing for culture: Why a successful concept should not be discarded. Current Anthropology, 40, 1–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, D. T. (1990). Levels of organization, downward causation, and the selection-theory approach to evolutionary epistemology. In Tobach, E. and Greenberg, G. (eds.), Scientific methodology in the study of mind: Evolutionary epistemology (pp. 1–17). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Campbell, D. T. (1994). How individual and face-to-face group selection undermine firm selection in organizational evolution. In Baum, J. A. C. and Singh, J. V. (eds.), Evolutionary dynamics of organizations (pp. 23–38). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Carver, C. S., Pozo, C., Harris, S. D., Noriega, V., Scheier, M. F., and Robinson, D. S. (1993). How coping mediates the effect of optimism on distress: A study of women with early stage breast cancer. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 375–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chang, E. C., Asakawa, K., and Sanna, L. J. (2001). Cultural variations in optimistic and pessimistic bias: Do Easterners really expect the worst and Westerners really expect the best when predicting future life events?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 476–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chao, M. M., Zhang, Z., and Chiu, C. (2008). Personal and collective culpability judgment: A functional analysis of East Asian-North American differences. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 39, 716–29.Google Scholar
Chaturvedi, A., Chiu, C.-y., and Viswanathan, M. (2009). Literacy, negotiable fate, and thinking style among low income women in India. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 40, 880–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, J., Chiu, C.-y., and Chan, F. S.-F. (in press). The cultural effects of job mobility and the belief in a fixed world: Evidence from performance forecast. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 851–65.CrossRef
Chirkov, V. L., and Ryan, R. M. (2001). Parent and teacher autonomy-support in Russian and U.S. adolescents: Common effects on well-being and academic motivation. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32, 618–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chirkov, V. L., Kim, Y., Ryan, R. M., and Kaplan, U. (2003). Differentiating autonomy from individualism and independence: A self-determination theory perspective on internalization of cultural orientations and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 97–110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chiu, C.-y., and Chao, M. M. (2009). Society, culture, and the person: Ways to personalize and socialize cultural psychology. In Wyer, R., Chiu, C.-y. and Hong, Y. (eds.), Understanding culture: Theory, research and application (pp. 456–66). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Chiu, C.-y., and Chen, J. (2004). Symbols and interactions: Application of the CCC model to culture, language, and social identity. In Ng, S.-h., Candlin, C. and Chiu, C.-y. (eds.), Language matters: Communication, culture, and social identity (pp. 155–82). Hong Kong: City University of Hong Kong Press.Google Scholar
Chiu, C.-y., and Hong, Y. (2006). Social psychology of culture. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Chiu, C.-y., and Hong, Y. (2007). Cultural processes: Basic principles. In Higgins, E. T. and Kruglanski, A. E. (eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 785–804). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Chiu, C.-y., Kim, Y.-h., and Chaturvedi, A. (2010). Collective evolution: Revisiting the Donald Campbell legacy. In Schaller, M., Norenzayan, A., Heine, S. J., Yamagishi, T. and Kameda, T. (eds.), Evolution, culture, and the human mind (pp. 39–47). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Chiu, C.-y., Kim, Y.-h., and Wan, W. N. (2008). Personality: Cross-cultural perspectives. In Boyle, G. J., Matthews, G. and Salofske, D. H. (eds.), Handbook of personality theory and testing, vol. I: Personality theory and testing (pp. 124–44). London: Sage.Google Scholar
Chiu, C.-y., Morris, M., Hong, Y., and Menon, T. (2000). Motivated cultural cognition: The impact of implicit cultural theories on dispositional attribution varies as a function of need for closure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 247–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Choi, I., and Choi, Y. (2002). Culture and self-concept flexibility. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1508–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, D., and Gunz, A. (2002). As seen by the other … Perspectives on the self in the memories and emotional perceptions of Easterners and Westerners. Psychological Science, 13, 55–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cousins, S. D. (1989). Culture and self-perception in Japan and the United States. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 124–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diener, E., and Diener, M. (1995). Cross-cultural correlates of life satisfaction and self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 653–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fu, H.-y., Morris, M. W., Lee, S.-l., Chao, M.-c., Chiu, C.-y., and Hong, Y.-y. (2007). Epistemic motives and cultural conformity: Need for closure, culture, and context as determinants of conflict judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 191–207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuller, S. (2008). Job mobility and wage trajectories for men and women in the United States. American Sociological Review, 73, 158–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gelfand, M. J., Higgins, M., Nishii, L. H., Rava, J. L., Dominquez, A., Murakami, F., Yamaguchi, S., and Toyama, M. (2002). Culture and egocentric perceptions of fairness in conflict and negotiation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 833–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gelfand, M. J., Spurlock, D., Sniezek, J. A., and Shao, L. (2000). Culture and social prediction: The role of information in enhancing confidence in social predictions in the United States and China. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 31, 498–516.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haberstroh, S., Oyserman, D., Schwarz, N., Kühnen, U., and Ji, L. (2002). Is the interdependent self more sensitive to question context than the independent self? Self-construal and the observation of conversational norms. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 323–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Han, S.-P., and Shavitt, S. (1994). Persuasion and culture: Advertising appeals in individualistic and collectivistic societies. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 30, 326–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heine, S. J., and Hamamura, T. (2007). In search of East Asian self-enhancement. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11, 4–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heine, S. J., and Lehman, D. R. (1995). Cultural variation in unrealistic optimism: Does the West feel more invulnerable than the East?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 595–607.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heine, S. J., and Renshaw, K. (2002). Interjudge agreement, self-enhancement, and liking: Cross-cultural divergences. Personality and Social Psychological Bulletin, 28, 442–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heine, S. J., Lehman, D. R., Markus, H. R., and Kitayama, S. (1999). Is there a universal need for positive self-regard?Psychological Review, 106, 766–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hernandez, M., and Iyengar, S. S. (2001). What drives whom? A cultural perspective on human agency. Social Cognition, 19, 269–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heylighen, F., and Campbell, D. T. (1995). Selection of organization at the societal level: Obstacles and facilitators of metasystem transitions. World Futures: The Journal of General Evolution, 45, 181–212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofstede, G. H. (2001). Culture's consequences: Comparing values, behaviors, institutions, and organizations across nations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Hong, Y.-y., Ip, G., Chiu, C.-y., Morris, M. W., and Menon, T. (2001). Cultural identity and dynamic construction of the self: Collective duties and individual rights in Chinese and American cultures. Social Cognition, 19, 251–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hong, Y., Morris, M., Chiu, C.-y., and Benet-Martinez, V. (2000). Multicultural minds: A dynamic constructivist approach to culture and cognition. American Psychologist, 55, 709–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iyengar, S. S., and DeVoe, S. E. (2003). Rethinking the value of choice: Cultural mediators of intrinsic motivation. In Murphy-Berman, and Berman, J. J. (eds.), Cross-cultural differences in perspectives on the self (pp. 129–74). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Iyengar, S. S., and Lepper, M. R. (1999). Rethinking the value of choice: A cultural perspective on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 349–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kanagawa, C., Cross, S. E., and Markus, H. R. (2001). ‘Who am I?’ The cultural psychology of the conceptual self. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 90–103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kashima, Y., Kashima, E., and Aldridge, J. (2001). Towards cultural dynamics of self-conceptions. In Sedikides, C. and Brewer, M. B. (eds.), Individual self, relational self, and collective self: Partners, opponents, and strangers (pp. 277–98). Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Kashima, Y., Kashima, E., Chiu, C.-y., Farsides, T., Gelfand, M., Hong, Y.-y., Kim, U., Strack, F., Worth, L., Yuki, M., and Yzerbyt, V. (2005). Culture, essentialism, and agency: Are individuals universally believed to be more real entities than groups?European Journal of Social Psychology, 35, 147–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keesing, R. M. (1974). Theories of culture. Annual Review of Anthropology, 3, 73–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, H. S., and Markus, H. R. (1999). Deviance or uniqueness, harmony or conformity? A cultural analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 785–800.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, J., Kim, M.-s., Kam, K. Y., and Shin, H.-c. (2003). Influence of self-construals on the perception of different self-presentation styles in Korea. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 6, 89–101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, K., and Markman, A. B. (2006). Differences in fear of isolation as an explanation of cultural differences: Evidence from memory and reasoning. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 350–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, Y.-H., Peng, S., and Chiu, C.-y. (2008). Explaining self-esteem differences between Chinese and North Americans: Dialectical self (vs. self-consistency) or lack of positive self-regards. Self and Identity, 7, 113–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, Y.-H., Chiu, C.-y., Peng, S., Cai, H., and Tov, W. (2010). Explaining East–West differences in the likelihood of making favorable self-evaluations: The role of evaluation apprehension and directness of expression. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 41, 62–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kitayama, S., and Karasawa, M. (1997). Implicit self-esteem in Japan: Name letters and birthday numbers. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 736–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., Matsumoto, H., and Norasakkunkit, V. (1997). Individual and collective processes of self-esteem management: Self-enhancement in the United States and self-depreciation in Japan. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 1245–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kniffin, K. M., and Wilson, D. S. (2005). Utilities of gossip across organizational levels: Multilevel selection, free-riders, and teams. Human Nature, 16, 278–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knight, J., and Yueh, L. (2004). Job mobility of residents and migrants in urban China. Journal of Comparative Economics, 32, 637–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kobayashi, C., and Greenwald, A. G. (2003). Implicit–explicit differences in self-enhancement for Americans and Japanese. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 34, 522–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kudo, E., and Numazaki, M. (2003). Explicit and direct self-serving bias in Japan: Reexamination of self-serving bias for success and failure. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 34, 511–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kurman, J. (2001). Self-enhancement: Is it restricted to individualistic cultures?Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 12, 1705–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kurman, J. (2003). Why is self-enhancement low in certain collectivistic cultures? An investigation of two competing explanations. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 34, 489–510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kurman, J., and Sriram, N. (1997). Self-enhancement, generality of self-evaluation, and affectivity in Israel and Singapore. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 28, 421–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kurman, J., and Sriram, N. (2002). Interrelationships among vertical and horizontal collectivism, modesty, and self-enhancement. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 33, 71–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kwan, V. S. Y., Bond, M. H., and Singelis, T. M. (1997). Pancultural explanations for life satisfaction: Adding relationship harmony to self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1038–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lalwani, A. K., Shavitt, S., and Johnson, T. (2006). What is the relation between cultural orientation and socially desirable responding?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 165–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lalwani, A. K., Shrum, L. J., and Chiu, C.-y. (2009). Motivated response styles: The role of cultural values, regulatory focus, and self-consciousness in socially desirable responding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 870–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lehman, D., Chiu, C.-y., and Schaller, M. (2004). Culture and psychology. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 689–714.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Markus, H. R., and Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markus, H. R., and Kitayama, S. (2003). Culture, self, and the reality of the social. Psychological Inquiry, 14, 277–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menon, T., Morris, M., Chiu, C.-y., and Hong, Y. (1999). Culture and the construal of agency: Attribution to individual versus group dispositions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 76, 701–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muramoto, Y. (2003). An indirect self-enhancement in relationship among Japanese. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 34, 552–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murray, S. L., Holmes, J. G., and Griffin, D. W. (1996). The self-fulfilling nature of positive illusions in romantic relationships: Love is not blind, but prescient. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 1155–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oishi, S., and Diener, E. (2001). Goals, culture, and subjective well-being. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 1674–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., and Kemmelmeier, M. (2002). Rethinking individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 3–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poortinga, Y. H. (2003). Coherence of culture and generalizability of data: Two questionable assumptions in cross-cultural psychology. In Murphy-Berman, V. and Berman, J. J. (eds.), Cross-cultural differences in perspectives on the self: Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (vol. XLIX, pp. 257–305). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Rhee, E., Uleman, J. S., Lee, H. K., and Roman, R. J. (1995). Spontaneous self-descriptions and ethnic identities in individualistic and collectivistic cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 142–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ryan, R. M., and Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 68–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schimmack, U., Radhakrishnan, P., Oishi, S., Dzokoto, V., and Ahadi, S. (2002). Culture, personality, and subjective well-being: Integrating process models of life satisfaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 582–593.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sedikides, C., and Strube, M. J. (1997). Self-evaluation: To thine own self be good, to thine own self be sure, to thine own self be true, and to thine own self be better. In Zanna, M. P. (ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 209–69). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Sedikides, C., Gaertner, L., and Toguchi, Y. (2003). Pancultural self-enhancement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 60–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sedikides, C., Gaertner, L., and Vevea, J. L. (2005). Pancultural self-enhancement reloaded: A meta-analytic reply to Heine (2005). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 539–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sedikides, C., Gaertner, L., and Vevea, J. L. (2007). Evaluating the evidence for pancultural self enhancement. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 10, 201–3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sedikides, C., Rudich, E. A., Gregg, A. P., Kumashiro, M., and Rusbult, C. (2004). Are normal narcissists psychologically healthy? Self-esteem matters. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 400–416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheldon, K. M., and McGregor, H. (2000). Extrinsic value orientation and the ‘tragedy of the commons’. Journal of Personality, 68, 383–411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shweder, R. A. (1990). Cultural psychology – what is it? In Stigler, J. W., Shweder, R. A. and Herdt, G. (eds.), Cultural theories: Essays on comparative human development (pp. 1–46). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sober, E., and Wilson, D. S. (1998). Unto others: The evolution and psychology of unselfish behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Spoor, J., and Williams, K. D. (2007). The evolution of an ostracism detection system. In Forgas, J. P., Haselton, M. G. and Hippel, W. (eds.), Evolution and the social mind: Evolutionary psychology and social cognition (pp. 279–92). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Suh, E. M. (2002). Culture, identity consistency, and subjective well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1378–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suh, E. M., Diener, E., Oishi, S., and Triandis, H. C. (1998). The shifting basis of life satisfaction judgments across cultures: Emotions versus norms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 482–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sui, J., Zhu, Y., and Chiu, C.-y. (2007). Bicultural mind, self-construal, and recognition memory: Cultural priming effects on self- and mother-reference effect. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 818–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tajfel, H., Billig, M. G., Bundy, R. P., and Flament, C. (1971). Social categorization and intergroup behavior. European Journal of Social Psychology, 1, 149–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Takata, T. (2003). Self-enhancement and self-criticism in Japanese culture: An experimental analysis. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 34, 542–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Triandis, H. C. (1989). The self and social behavior in differing cultural contexts. Psychological Review, 96, 506–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Triandis, H. C., Bontempo, R., Villareal, M. J., Asai, M., and Lucca, N. (1988). Individualism and collectivism: Cross-cultural perspective on self–group relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 323–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Triandis, H. C., Leung, K., Villareal, M. J., and Clack, F. U. (1985). Allocentric and idiocentric tendencies: Convergent and discriminant validation. Journal of Research in Personality, 19, 395–415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baaren, R. B., Maddux, W. W., Chartrand, T., Bouter, C., and Knippenberg, A. (2003). It takes two to mimic: Behavioral consequences of self-construals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 1093–102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wagar, B., and Cohen, D. (2003). Culture, memory, and the self: An analysis of the personal and collective self in long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 468–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wan, C., and Chiu, C.-y. (2009). Intersubjective consensus approach to culture. In Wyer, R., Chiu, C.-y., and Hong, Y. (eds.), Understanding culture: Theory, research and application (pp. 79–91). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Wan, C., Chiu, C., Tam, K., Lee, S., Lau, I. Y., and Peng, S. (2007). Perceived cultural importance and actual self-importance of values in cultural identification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 337–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, Q. (2001). Culture effects on adults' earliest childhood recollection and self-description: Implications for the relation between memory and the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 220–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, Q. (2004). The emergence of cultural self-constructs: Autobiographical memory and self-description in European American and Chinese children. Developmental Psychology, 40, 3–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, Y., Wiley, A., and Chiu, C.-y. (2008). Independence-supportive praise versus interdependence promoting praise. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 32, 13–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wheeler, C. H. (2008). Local market scale and the pattern of job changes among young men. Regional Science and Urban Economics, 38, 101–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wirtz, D., and Chiu, C.-y. (2008). Perspectives on the self in the East and West: Searching for the quiet ego. In Wayment, H. and Bauer, J. (eds.), Transcending self-interest: Psychological explorations of the quiet ego (pp. 149–58). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wirtz, D., Chiu, C.-y., Diener, E., and Oishi, S. (2009). What constitutes a good life? Cultural differences in the role of positive and negative affect in subjective well-being. Journal of Personality, 77, 1167–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wrong, D. (1976). The oversocialized conception of man. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Yamagishi, T., Hashimoto, H., and Schug, J. (2008). Preferences vs. strategies as explanations for culture-specific behavior. Psychological Science, 19, 579–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yamaguchi, S., Greenwald, A., Banaji, M., Murakami, F., Chen, D., Shiomura, K., Kobayashi, C., Cai, H., and Krendl, A. (2007). Apparent universality of positive implicit self-esteem. Psychological Science, 18, 498–500.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×