Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-ndmmz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-15T18:54:58.863Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2009

Maria Legerstee
Affiliation:
York University, Toronto
Get access
Type
Chapter
Information
Infants' Sense of People
Precursors to a Theory of Mind
, pp. 201 - 224
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2005

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adamson, L. B. and Bakeman, R. (1982). Affectivity and reference: Concepts, methods, and techniques in the study of communication development of 6– to 18-month-old infants. In Field, T. M. and Fogel, A. (eds.), Emotion and early interaction. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., and Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Astington, J. W. (2001). The paradox of intention: Assessing children's metarepresentational understanding. In Malle, B. F., Moses, L. J., and Baldwin, D. A. (eds.), Intentions and intentionality: Foundations of social cognition (pp. 85–103). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bahrick, L. R. (1988) Intermodal learning in infancy: Learning on the basis of two kinds of invariant relations in audible and visible events. Child Development, 59, 197–209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bahrick, L. R., Moss, L., and Fadil, C. (1996). Development of visual self-recognition in infancy. Ecological Psychology, 8, 189–208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bahrick, L. R., and Watson, J. S. (1985). Detection of intermodal proprioceptive-visual contingency as a potential basis of self-perception in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 21, 963–973.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baillargeon, R. (1986). Representing the existence and the location of hidden objects: Object permanence in 6–8-month-old infants. Cognition, 23, 21–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baillargeon, R.(1993). The object concept revisited: New directions in the investigation of infants' physical knowledge. In Granrud, C. (ed.), Visual perception and cognition in infancy (pp. 265–315). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bakeman, R., and Adamson, L. (1984). Coordinating attention to people and objects in mother–infant and peer–infant interaction. Child Development, 55, 1278–1289.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baldwin, D. A., and Moses, L. J. (1994). The mindreading engine: Evaluating evidence for modularity. Cahiers der Psychologie, 13, 553–560.Google Scholar
Baldwin, D. A., and Moses, L. J(1996). Early understanding of referential intent and attentional focus: Evidence from language and emotion. In Lewis, C. and Mitchell, P. (eds.), Children's early understanding of mind: Origins and development (pp. 133–156). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Baldwin, J. M. (1902). Social and ethical interpretations in mental development. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Ball, A. W. (1973). The perception of causality in the infant. Presented at the Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Philadelphia, PA.
Ball, A. W., and Tronick, E. (1971). Infant responses to impending collision: Optical and real. Science, 171, 818–820.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bandura, A. (1962). Social cognitive theory of social referencing. In Feinman, S. (ed.), Social learning through imitation. University of Nebraska Press: Lincoln, NE.Google Scholar
Banks, M. S., and Salapatek, P. (1983). Infant visual perception. In Haith, M. M. and Campos, J. J. (eds.), Infancy and developmental psychobiology (vol. II of P. H. Mussen [ed.], Handbook of Child Psychology), 4th edn. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Barna, J., and Legerstee, M. (2005). Nine and 12-month-old infants relate emotions to people's actions. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 53–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). Are autistic children “behaviorists”? An examination of their mental–physical and appearance–reality distinctions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 579–600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S.(1991). Precursors to a theory of mind: Understanding attention in others. In Whiten, A. (ed.), Natural theories of mind: Evolution, development and simulation of everyday mindreading. Cambridge, MA: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S.(1993). From attentional-goal psychology to belief-desire psychology. The development of theory of mind and its dysfunction. In Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., and Cohen, D. J. (eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism (pp. 59–82). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S.(1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Campbell, R., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Grant, J., and Walker, S. (1995). Are children with autism blind to the mentalistic significance of the eyes?British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13, 379–398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrera, M. E., and Maurer, D. (1981). Recognition of mother's photographed face by the three-month-old infant. Child Development, 52, 714–716.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barresi, J. (1984). Knowledge and representation: Transparent and opaque. Contemporary Psychology, 29, 160–161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barresi, J., and Moore, C. (1996). Intentional relations and social understanding. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 19, 107–154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, M. D. (1989). Early language development. In Slater, A. and Bremner, J. G. (eds.), Infant development (pp. 211–241). London: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bartsch, K., and Wellman, H. M. (1995). Children talk about the mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Baumwell, L., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., and Borstein, M. H. (1997). Maternal verbal sensitivity and child language comprehension. Infant Behavior and Development, 20, 247–258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bayley, N. (1969). Bayley scales of infant development. New York: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., March, J. E., and Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561–571.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beebe, B., Stern, D., and Jaffe, J. (1979). The kinesic rhythm of mother-infant interactions. In Siegman, A. W. & Feldstein, S. (eds.), Of speech and time: Temporal patterns in interpersonal contexts. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bellagamba, F. and Tomasello, M. (1999). Re-enacting intended acts: comparing 12– and 18-month-olds. Infant Behavior and Development, 22, 277–282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berthenthal, B. I., and Bai, D. L. (1989). Infants' sensitivity to optical flow for controlling posture. Developmental Psychology, 25, 936–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berkley, G. (1975). Philosophical works. Edited by Ayers, M. R.. London: Dent.Google Scholar
Bigelow, A. E. (1998). Infants' sensitivity to familiar imperfect contingencies in social interaction. Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 149–161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F. (2005). Children's thinking. Cognitive development and individual differences. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth/Thomson.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., and Lamb, M. E. (1992). Development in infancy: An introduction (3rd edn, xiv, pp. 560). New York, NY, England: Mcgraw-Hill Book Company.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., and Sigman, M. D. (1986). Continuity in mental development from infancy. Child Development, 57, 251–274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bornstein, M. H., and Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (1989a). Maternal responsiveness and cognitive development in children. In Bornstein, M. H. (ed.), Maternal responsiveness: Characteristics and consequences (pp. 49–61). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., and Tamis-LeMonda, C. S.(1989b). Habituation and maternal encouragement of attention in infancy as predictors of toddler language, play, and representational competence. Child Development, 60, 738–751.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., and Tamis-LeMonda, C. S.(1997). Maternal responsiveness and infant mental abilities: Specific predictive relations. Infant Behavior and Development, 20, 283–296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1951). Maternal care and mental health. London: HMSO; New York: Columbia University.Google ScholarPubMed
Bowlby, J.(1969). Attachment and loss. Vol. I: Attachment. Pelican Books, Penguin.Google Scholar
Bowlby, J.(1979). On knowing what you are not supposed to know and feeling what you are not supposed to feel. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 24, 403–408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brazelton, T. B., Koslowski, B., and Main, M. (1974). The origins of reciprocity: The early mother–infant interaction. In Lewis, M. and Rosenblum, L. (eds.), The effect of the infant on its caregiver (pp. 49–76). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bremner, J. G. (1988). Infancy. Cambridge, MA: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bremner, J. G.(1998). From perception to action: The early development of knowledge. In Simion, F. and Butterworth, G. (eds.), The development of sensory, motor and cognitive capacities in early infancy: From perception to cognition (pp. 239–255). Hove, England: Psychology Press/Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Brentano, F. (1874/1973). Psychology from an empirical standpoint. Trans. A. C. Rancurello, D. B. Terrel, and L. L. McAlister. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Bretherton, I. and Beeghly, M. (1982). Talking about internal states: The acquisition of an explicit theory of mind. Developmental Psychology, 18, 906–921.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1974). A report on longitudinal evaluations of pre-school programs, Vol. 2, Is early intervention effective? Washington, DC: D. H. W. Publications No. (OHD) 74–25.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1973). Organization of early skilled action. Child Development, 44, 11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruner, J. S.(1975). From communication to language: A psychological perspective. Cognition, 3, 255–287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. S.(1983). Child's talk: Learning to use language. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S.(1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, J.(1999). The intentionality of referring. In Zelazo, P. and Astington, J. W. (eds.), Developing theories of intention: Social understanding and self-control (pp. 329–339). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S., and Koslowski, B. (1972). Visually preadapted constituents of manipulatory action. Perception, 1, 3–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bushnell, I. W. R., Sai, F., and Mullin, J. T. (1989). Neonatal recognition of the mother's face. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7, 3–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butterworth, G. (1990). On reconceptualizing sensorimotor development in dynamic systems theory. In Bloch, H. and Berthenthal, B. J. (eds.), Sensorimotor organizations and development in infancy and early childhood (pp. 57–73). Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butterworth, G.(1991). The ontogeny and phylogeny of joint visual attention. In Whiten, A. (ed.), National theories of mind: Evolution, development, and simulation of everyday mind reading (pp. 223–232). Cambridge, MA: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Butterworth, G.(1994). Theories of mind and the facts of embodiment. In Lewis, C. and Mitchell, P. (eds.), Children's early understanding of mind: Origins and development (pp. 115–132). Sussex, England: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Butterworth, G.(1995). Factors in visual attention eliciting manual pointing in human infancy. In Raiblat, H. L. and Meyer, J. (eds.), Comparative approaches to cognitive science (pp. 329–338). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Butterworth, G., and Hicks, L. (1977). Visual proprioception and postural stability in infancy: A developmental study. Perception, 6, 255–262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butterworth, G., and Hopkins, B. (1988). Hand–mouth coordination in the new-born baby. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 6, 303–314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butterworth, G., and Jarrett, N. (1991). What minds have in common in space: Spatial mechanisms serving joint visual attention in infancy. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 55–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camaioni, L., Perucchini, P., Bellagamba, F., and Colonnesi, R. (2004), The role of declarative pointing in developing a Theory of Mind. Infancy, 5, 291–308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camaioni, L., Perucchini, P., Muratori, F., and Milone, A. (1997). Brief report: A longitudinal examination of the communicative gestures deficit in young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27, 715–725.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, S. B., Cohn, J., and Myers, T. (1995). Depression in first-time mothers: Mother–infant interaction and depression chronicity. Developmental Psychology, 31, 349–357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campos, J., and Sternberg, C. (1981). Perception, appraisal, and emotion: The onset of social referencing. In Lamb, M. and Sherrod, L. (eds.), Infant social cognition: Empirical and theoretical considerations (pp. 273–314). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual Change in Childhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Caron, A., Carfon, R., Mustelin, C., and Roberts, J. (1992). Infant responding to aberrant social stimuli. Infant Behavior and Development, 15, 335 (Special ICIS Conference Issue).Google Scholar
Carpenter, M., Nagell, L., and Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63 (4, serial no. 255).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chada, S. W. (1996). A comparison of mother–infant, father–infant and stranger–infant affect attunement: Characteristics and consequences of discrete parenting roles in infant affective development. Dissertation Abstracts International, 52 (12-B), 7060.Google Scholar
Chapman, M. (1992). Equilibration and the dialectic of organization. In Beiling, H. and Putfall, P. (eds.), Piaget's theory: Prospects and possibilities (pp. 39–59). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Baird, G., Cox, A., and Drew, A. (2000). Testing joint attention, imitation, and play as infancy precursors to language and theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 15, 481–498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charman, T., Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Baird, G., and Drew, A. (1997). Infants with autism: An investigation of empathy, pretend play, joint attention, and imitation. Developmental Psychology, 33, 781–789.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chi, M. T. H. (1988). Children's lack of access and knowledge reorganization: An example from the concept of animacy. In Weinert, F. E. and Perlmuter, M. (eds.), Memory development: Universal changes and individual differences (pp. 169–194). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Churchland, P. M. (1991). Folk-psychology and the explanation of human behavior. In Greenwood, J. D. (ed.), The future of folk psychology: Intentionality and cognitive science. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., and Schneider-Rosen, K. (1984). Toward a transactional model of childhood depression. New Directions for Child Development, 26, 5–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohn, J. F., and Tronick, E. Z. (1989). Specificity of infants' response to mothers' affective behavior. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 242–248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corkum, V., and Moore, C. (1995). Development of joint visual attention in infants. In Moore, C. and Dunham, P. J. (eds.), Joint attention: its origins and role in development (pp. 61–83). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Corkum, V., and Moore, C.(1998). The origin of joint visual attention in infants. Developmental Psychology, 34, 28–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coster, W. J., Gersten, M. S., Beegly, M., and Cicchetti, D. (1989). Communicative functioning in maltreated toddlers. Developmental Psychology, 25, 1020–1029.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crockenberg, S. (1983). Early mother and infant antecedents of Bayley Scale performance at 21 months. Developmental Psychology, 19, 727–730.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Csibra, G., Gergely, G., Biro, S., Koos, O., and Brockbank, M. (1999). Social attribution without agency cues: The perception of “pure reason” in infancy. Cognition, 72, 237–267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darwin, C. (1877). A biographical sketch of an infant. Mind, 2, 285–294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, K. (1947). Final note on a case of extreme isolation. American Journal of Sociology, 52, 432–437.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeCasper, A. J., and Carstens, A. A. (1981). Contingencies of stimulation: Effects on learning and emotion in neonates. Infant Behavior and Development, 4, 19–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeCasper, A. J., and Fifer, W. P. (1980). Of human bonding: Newborns prefer their mother's voice. Science, 208, 1174–1176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeCasper, A. J., Lecanuet, J. P., Busnell, M. C., Granier-Deferre, C., and Maugeais, R. (1994). Fetal reactions to recurrent maternal speech sounds. Infant Behavior and Development, 9, 133–150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeCharmes, R. (1968). Personal Causation: The Internal Affective Determinants of behavior. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Descartes, R. (1641/1985). The philosophical writings of Descartes (Vols. 1 and 2). Trans. J. Cottingham et al. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Desrochers, S., Morrissette, P., and Ricard, M. (1995). Two perspectives on pointing in infancy. In Moore, C. and Dunham, P. J. (eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 85–101). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Dodd, B. (1979). Lip reading in infants: Attention to speech present in- and out-of-synchrony. Cognitive psychology, 11, 478–484.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dondi, M., Simion, F., and Caltran, G. (1999). Can newborns discriminate between their own cry and the cry of another newborn infant?Developmental Psychology, 35, 418–426.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunham, P., and Dunham, F. (1990). Effects of mother–infant social interactions on infants' subsequent contingency task performance. Child Development, 61, 785–793.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunham, P., Dunham, F., Hurshman, A., and Alexander, T. (1989). Social contingency effects on subsequent perceptual-cognitive tasks in young infants. Child Development, 60, 1486–1496.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Emde, R. N. (1989). Early emotional development: New modes of thinking for research and intervention. In Warhol, J. G. (ed.), New perspectives in early emotional development (pp. 29–45). Johnson and Johnson Pediatric Institute.Google Scholar
Eimas, P. D., Siqueland, E. R., Juscyk, P., and Vigorito, J. (1971). Speech perception in infants. Science, 171, 303–306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P., and Friesen, W. V. (1975). Unmasking the face: A guide to recognizing emotions from facial clues. New York: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Ellsworth, C. P., Muir, D., and Hains, S. (1993). Social competence and person–object differentiation: An analysis of the still-face effect. Developmental Psychology, 39, 63–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fagan, J. F. (1972). Infants' recognition memory for faces. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 14, 453–476.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feinman, S. (1982). Social referencing in infancy. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 28, 445–470.Google Scholar
Feinman, S., and Lewis, M. (1983). Social referencing at 10-months. A second order effect on infants' responses to strangers. Child Development, 54, 878–887.Google Scholar
Field, T. (1984). Early interactions between infants and their postpartum depressed mothers. Infant Behaviour and Development, 7, 527–532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Field, T.(1990). Neonatal stress and coping in intensive care. Infant Mental Health Journal, 11, 57–65.3.0.CO;2-Y>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Field, T.(1992). Psychobiological attunement in close relationships. In Lerner, R., Featherman, D. L., and Perlmutter, M. (eds.), Life-span development and behavior (pp. 2–22). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Field, T.(1994). The effects of mother's physical and emotional unavailability on emotion regulation. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59 (23).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Field, T.(1995). Infant of depressed mothers (Presidential address). Infant Behavior and Development, 18, 1–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Field, T., Cohen, D., Garcia, , and Greenberg, R. (1985). Mother–stranger face discrimination by the newborn. Annual Progress in Child Psychiatry and Child Development, 3–10.Google Scholar
Field, T., Guy, L., and Umbel, V. (1985). Infants' responses to mothers' imitative behaviors. Infant Mental Health Journal, 6, 40–44.3.0.CO;2-L>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Field, T., Healy, B., Goldstein, S., Perry, S., Berry, D., Schanberg, S., Zimmerman, E. A., and Kuhn, C. (1998). Infants of depressed mothers show “depressed” behavior even with nondepressed adults. Child Development, 59, 1569–1579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Field, T., Woodson, R., Greenberg, R., and Cohen, D. (1982). Discrimination and imitation of facial expressions of neonates. Science, 218, 179–181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Finkelstein, N. W., and Ramey, C. T. (1977). Learning to control the environment in infancy. Child Development, 48, 806–819.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, K. W., Shaver, P. R., and Carnochan, P. (1990). How emotions develop and how they organize development. Cognition and Emotion, 4, 81–127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fisher, L., Ames, E. W., Chisholm, K., and Savoie, L. (1997). Problems reported by parents of Romanians orphans adopted to British Columbia. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 20, 67–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flavell, J. H. (1988). The development of the children's knowledge about the mind: From cognitive connections to mental representations. In Astington, J. W. and Harris, P. L. (eds.), Developing theories of mind. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Flavell, J. H.(1999). Cognitive development: Children's knowledge about the mind. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 21–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flavell, J. H., Flavell, E. R., Green, F. L., and Moses, L. J. (1990). Young children's understanding of false beliefs versus value beliefs. Child Development, 61, 915–928.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flavell, J. H., Green, F. L., and Flavell, E. R. (1986). Development of knowledge about the appearance–reality distinction. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 51, 1–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of the mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A.(1992). A theory of the child's theory of mind. Cognition, 44, 283–296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fogel, A. (1977). Temporal organization in mother–infant face-to-face interaction. In Schaffer, H. Rudolph (ed.), Studies in mother–infant interaction (pp. 5–56). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Fogel, A.(1993). Developing through relationships: Origins of communication, self and culture. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Fogel, A.(2001). The history (and future) of infancy. Blackwell handbook of infant development. Handbooks of developmental psychology (pp. 726–757). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishers.Google Scholar
Fogel, A., and Hannan, T. E. (1985). Manual actions of nine to fifteen-week-old human infants during face-to-face interaction with their mothers. Child Development, 56, 1271–1279.Google ScholarPubMed
Fogel, A., and Thelen, E. (1987). Development of early expressive and communicative action: Reinterpreting the evidence from a dynamic systems perspective. Developmental Psychology, 23, 747–761.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franco, F., and Butterworth, G. (1996). Pointing and social awareness: Declaring and requesting in the second year. Journal of Child Language, 23, 307–336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, W. J. (2000). Emotion is essential to all intentional behaviors. In Granic, I. and Lewis, M. (eds.), Emotion, development, and self-organization: Dynamic systems approaches to emotional development (pp. 209–235). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1949). An outline of psychoanalysis (rev. edn, Strachey). New York: Norton (original work published 1940).Google Scholar
Freud, S.(1961). The ego and the id. New York: W. W. Norton and Co.Google Scholar
Frye, D. (1981). Developmental changes in strategies of social interaction. In Lamb, M. and Sherrod, L. (eds.), Infant social cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gallagher, S. (1996). The moral significance of primitive self-consciousness: The response to Bermúdez. Ethics, 107, 129–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallup, G. G. (1982). Self-awareness and the emergence of mind in primates. American Journal of Primatology, 2, 237–248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gekoski, M. J., and Fagen, J. W. (1984). Noncontingent stimulation, stimulus familiarization, and subsequent learning in young infants. Child Development, 55, 2226–2233.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gelman, R., and Spelke, E. (1981). The development of thoughts about animate and inanimate objects: Implications for research on social cognition. In Flavell, J. H. and Ross, L. (eds.), Social cognition development: Frontiers and possible futures. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gelman, S. A., and Coley, J. D. (1990). The importance of knowing a dodo is a bird: Categories and inferences in 2-year-old children. Developmental Psychology, 26, 796–804.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gelman, S. A., Durgin, F., and Kaufman, L. (1995). Distinguishing between animates and inanimates: Not by notion alone. In Sperber, D. and Premack, D. (eds.), Causal cognition: A multidisciplinary debate (pp. 150–184). Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Gelman, S. A., and Markman, E. M. (1986). Categories and induction in young children. Cognition, 23, 183–209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gergely, G. (2001). Is early differentiation of human behavior a precursor to the 1-year-old's understanding of intentional action? Comment on Legerstee, Barna, and DiAdamo (2000). Developmental Psychology, 37, 579–582.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gergely, G.(2002). The development of understanding self and agency. In Goswamia, Usha (ed.), Blackwell's Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development. Malden, MA: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gergely, G., and Watson, J. (1996). The social biofeedback theory of parental affect-mirroring: The development of emotional self-awareness and self-control in infancy. International Journal of Psycho-Analysis, 77, 1181–1212.Google ScholarPubMed
Gergely, G., and Watson, J.(1999). Early socio-emotional development: Contingency perception and the social biofeedback model. In Rochat, P. (ed.), Early social cognition: Understanding others in the first months of life (pp. 101–136). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gergely, G., Nadasdy, Z., Csibry, G., and Biro, S. (1995). Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition, 56, 165–193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibson, E. J. (1969). Principles of perceptual learning and development. East Norwalk, US: Appleton Century Crofts.Google Scholar
Gibson, E. J.(1993). Ontogenesis of the perceived self. In Neisser, U. (ed.), The perceived self: Ecological and interpersonal sources of self-knowledge (pp. 25–42). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gibson, E. J.(1995). Are we automata? In Rochat, P. (ed.), The self in infancy: Theory and research. Advances in psychology (pp. 13–15). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Glick, J. (1978). Cognition and social cognition: an introduction. In Glick, J. L. and Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (eds.), The development of social understanding, (pp. 1–9). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Glick, J., and Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (eds.) (1978). The development of social understanding. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Goldberg, S., Lojkasek, M., Gartner, G., and Corter, C. (1989). Maternal responsiveness and social development in pre-term infants. In Bornstein, M. H. (ed.), Maternal responsiveness: Characteristics and consequences (pp. 89–103). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Goldfarb, W. (1943). Infant rearing and problem behavior. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 13, 249–265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldsmith, D. F., and Rogoff, B. (1997). Mothers' and toddlers' coordinated joint focus of attention: Variations with maternal dysphoric symptoms. Developmental Psychology, 33, 113–119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gopnik, A. (1995). How to understand beliefs. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18, 398–400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, A., and Meltzoff, A. N. (1997). Words, thoughts, and theories. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A., and Wellman, H. M. (1992). Why the child's theory of mind really is a theory. Mind and Language, 7, 145–171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goren, C. C., Sarty, M., and Wu, P. Y. K. (1975). Visual following and pattern discrimination of face-like stimuli by newborn infants. Pediatrics, 56, 544–549.Google ScholarPubMed
Gotlieb, S. J. (1991). Visual memory in neonates. Dissertation Abstracts International, 51 (10-B), 5049–5050.Google Scholar
Granic, I. (2000) Emotion, development, and self-organization: dynamic systems approaches to emotional development. In Lewis, M. D. and Granic, I. (eds.), The self-organization of parent–child relations: Beyond bidirectional models (pp. 267–297). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Grant, K. W., Ardell, L. H., Kuhl, P. K., and Sparks, D. W. (1986). The transmission of prosodic information via an electrotactile speedreading aid. Ear and Hearing, 7, 243–251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hains, S. M., and Muir, D. W. (1996). Effects of stimulus contingency in infant–adult interactions. Infant Behavior and Development, 19, 49–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haith, M. M. (1966). The response of the human newborn to visual movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Child Psychology, 3, 243–253.Google ScholarPubMed
Haith, M. M., Bergman, T., and Moore, M. J. (1977). Eye contact and face scanning in early infancy. Science, 198, 853–855.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harding, C. G. (1982). Development of the intention to communicate. Human Development, 25, 140–151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harlow, H. F. (1958). The nature of love. American Psychologist, 13, 673–685.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harlow, H. F.(1961). The development of affectional patterns in infant monkeys. In Foss, B. M. (ed.), Determinants of infant behaviour (Vol. I). London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Harlow, H. F. and Harlow, M. K. (1965). The affectional systems. In Schrier, A. M., Harlow, H. F., and Stollnitz, F. (eds.), Behavior of nonhuman primates (Vol. II, pp. 287–334). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Harlow, H. F., and Zimmermann, R. R. (1959). Affectional responses in the infant monkey. Science, 130, 421–432.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, P. L. (1989). Children and emotion: The development of psychological understanding. Oxford, England: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Harris, P. L.(1992). From simulation to folk psychology: The case for development. Mind Language, 7, 120–144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haviland, J. M., and Lewica, M. (1987). The induced affect response: 10-week-old infants' responses to three emotion expressions. Developmental Psychology, 23, 97–104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heyes, C. M. (1998). Theory of mind in nonhuman primates. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, bbsonline.org.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hobson, R. P. (1989). Beyond cognition: A theory of autism. In Dawson, G. (ed.), Autism, Nature, diagnosis and treatment (pp. 48–). New York, Guilford.Google Scholar
Hobson, R. P.(1989). On sharing experiences. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 197–203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobson, R. P.(1990). On the origins of self and the case of autism. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 163–181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobson, R. P.(1993). Autism and the development of mind. Hove: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hobson, R. P.(1998). The intersubjective foundations of thought. In Braten, S. (ed.), Intersubjective communication and emotion in early ontogeny: Studies in emotion and social interaction (2nd series, pp. 283–296). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hobson, R. P.(2002). The cradle of thought: Exploring the origins of thinking. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hoffman, M. (1981). Perspectives on the difference between understanding people and understanding things: the role of affect. In Flavell, H. and Ross, L. (eds.), Social cognitive development: Frontiers and possible futures, pp. 67–81. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hofsten, C. (1980). Predictive reaching for moving objects by human infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 30, 369–382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hood, B. M., Willen, J. D., and Driver, J. (1998). Adults' eyes trigger shifts of visual attention in human infants. Psychological Science, 9, 131–134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hornick, R., Risenhoover, N., and Gunnar, M. (1987). The effects of maternal positive, neutral, and negative affective communications on infant responses to new toys. Child Development, 58, 937–944.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hsu, H. C., and Fogel, A., (2003). Stability and transitions in mother–infant face-to-face communication during the first 6 months: A micro-historical approach. Developmental Psychology, 39, 1061–1082.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hsu, H. C., Fogel, A., and Messinger, D. S. (2001). Infant non-distress vocalization during mother–infant face-to-face interaction: Factors associated with quantitative and qualitative differences. Infant Behavior and Development, 24, 107–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hume, D. (1739/1888). A treatise of human nature. Ed. Selby-Bigge, L. A.. Oxford: Clarendon.Google Scholar
Izard, C. E. (1971). The face of emotion. East Norwalk: Appleton Century Crofts.Google Scholar
Izard, C. E.(1978). Emotions as motivations: An evolutionary-developmental perspective. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 26, 163–200.Google Scholar
Jaffe, J., Beebe, B., Feldstein, S., Crown, C. L., and Jasnow, M. D. (2001). Rhythms of dialogue in infancy. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 66, 2001.Google ScholarPubMed
James, W. (1890). Principles of psychology. New York: Holt.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. H., Dziurawiec, S., Ellis, H. D., and Morton, J. (1991). Newborns' preferential tracking of face-like stimuli and its subsequent decline. Cognition, 40, 1–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, M. H., and Morton, J. (1991). Biology and cognitive development: The case of face recognition. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Johnson, S. C., Booth, A., and O'Hearn, L. (2001). Inferring the goals of a non-human agent. Cognitive Development, 16, 637–656.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, S., Slaughter, V., and Carey, S. (1998). Whose gaze will infants follow? The elicitation of gaze following in 12-month-olds. Developmental Science, 1, 233–238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Juscyk, P. W., Kennedy, L. J., and Juscyk, A. (1995). Young infants' retention of information about syllables. Infant Behavior and Development, 18, 24–41.Google Scholar
Kagan, J. (1976). Resilience and continuity in psychological development. In Clarke, A. M. and Clarke, A. D. B. (eds.), Early experience: Myth and evidence (pp. 97–121). New York: The Free Press.Google Scholar
Kalins, I. V., and Bruner, J. S. (1973). The coordination of visual observation and instrumental behavior in early infancy. Perception, 2, 307–314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kant, I. (1781/1996). Critique of pure reason. Trans. W. S. Pluhar. Indianapolis: Hackett.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A.(1998). Is atypical development necessarily a window on the normal mind/brain? The case of Williams Syndrome. Developmental-Science, 1, 273–277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A., Klima, E., Bellugi, U., Grant, J., and Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Is there a social module? Language, face processing, and theory of mind in individuals with Williams syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 196–208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaye, K. (1982). Self-image formation in adopted children: The environment within. Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy, 13, 175–181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaye, K., and Fogel, A. (1980). The temporal structure of face-to-face communication between mothers and infants. Developmental Psychology, 16, 454–464.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaye, K., and Markus, J. (1981). Infant imitation: The sensory-motor agenda. Developmental Psychology, 3, 258–265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keil, C. K., Smith, W. C., Simons, D. J., and Levin, D. T. (1998). Two dogmas of conceptual empiricism: Implications for hybrid models of the structure of knowledge. Cognition, 65, 103–135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenny, J. (1988), The self. Marquette: Marquette University Press.Google Scholar
Killen, M., and Uzgiris, I. C. (1981). Imitation of action with objects: The role of social meaning. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 138, 219–229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koluchova, J. (1972). Severe deprivation in twins: A case study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 13, 107–114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuchuk, A., Vibbert, N. M., and Bornstein, M. H. (1986). The perception of smiling and its experiential correlates in three-month-old infants. Child Development, 57, 1054–1061.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhl, P., and Meltzoff, A. N. (1982). The bimodal perception of speech in infancy. Science, 218, 1138–1141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LaBarbera, J. D., Izard, C. E., Vietze, P., and Parisi, S. A. (1976). Four- and six-month-old infants' visual responses to joy, anger, and neutral expressions. Child Development, 47, 535–538.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Millar-Loncar, C. L., and Swank, P. R. (1998). The relation of change in maternal interactive styles to the developing social competence in full-term and pre-term children. Child Development, 69, 105–123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legerstee, M. (1990). Infants use multimodal stimulation to imitate speech sounds. Infant Behavior and Development, 13, 345–356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legerstee, M.(1991a). Changes in the quality of infant sounds as a function of social and nonsocial stimulation. First Language, 11, 327–343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legerstee, M.(1991b). The role of people and objects in early imitation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 51, 423–433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legerstee, M.(1992). A review of the animate/inanimate distinction in infancy: Implications for models of social and cognitive knowing. Early Development and Parenting, 1, 59–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legerstee, M.(1994a). Patterns of 4-month-old infant responses to hidden silent and sounding people and objects. Early Development and Parenting, 3, 71–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legerstee, M.(1994b). The role of familiarity and sound in the development of person and object permanence. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 12, 455–468.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legerstee, M.(1997a). Changes in social-conceptual development: domain specific structures, self-organization and indeterminism. In Fogel, A., Lyra, M. C. D. P., and Valsiner, J. (eds.), Dynamics and indeterminism in development and social processes (pp. 245–260). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Legerstee, M.(1997b). Contingency effects of people and objects on subsequent cognitive functioning in three-month-old infants. Social Development, 6, 307–321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legerstee, M.(1998). Mental and bodily awareness in infancy: Consciousness of self-existence. Journal of Consciousness, 5, 627–644.Google Scholar
Legerstee, M.(2001a). Domain specificity and the epistemic triangle in the development of animism in infancy. In Lacerda, F., Hofsten, C., and Heinemann, M. (eds.), Emerging cognitive abilities in early infancy (pp. 193–212). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Legerstee, M.(2001b). Six-month-old infants rely on explanatory inference when relating communication to people and manipulatory actions to inanimate objects: Reply to Gergely. Developmental Psychology, 5, 583–586.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legerstee, M., Anderson, D., and Schaffer, A. (1998). Five- and eight-month-old infants recognize their faces and voices as familiar and social stimuli. Child Development, 69, 37–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Legerstee, M., and Barillas, Y. (2003). Sharing attention and pointing to objects at 12 months: Is the intentional stance implied?Cognitive Development, 18, 91–110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legerstee, M., Barna, J., and DiAdamo, C. (2000). Precursors to the development of intention at 6 months: Understanding people and their actions. Developmental Psychology, 3, 627–634.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legerstee, M., and Bowman, T. (1989). The development responses to people and a toy in infants with Down syndrome. Infant Behaviour and Development, 12, 462–473.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legerstee, M., Bowman, T., and Fels, S. (1992). People and objects affect the quality of vocalizations in infants with Down syndrome. Early Development and Parenting, 1, 149–156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legerstee, M., Corter, C., and Kienapple, K. (1990). Hand, arm and facial actions of young infants to a social and nonsocial stimulus. Child Development, 61, 774–784.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Legerstee, M., Fahy, L., Blake, J., Fisher, T., and Markova, G. (May, 2004). Maternal Factors Contributing to Toddlers' Early Development of Internal State Language: A Longitudinal Study. Poster presented at 14th Biennial International Conference on Infant Studies (ICIS), Chicago, USA.Google Scholar
Legerstee, M., and Feider, H. (1986). The acquisition of person pronouns in French-speaking children. International Journal of Psychology, 21, 629–639.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legerstee, M., Fisher, T., and Markova, G. (2005). The development of attention during dyadic and triadic interactions: The role of affect attunement. Paper presented at 35th Annual Meeting of the Jean Piaget Society, Vancouver, Canada, June, 2005.
Legerstee, M., and Markova, G. (2005). Variation in imitation in 10-month-old infants: Awareness of intentional action. Paper presented at 35th annual meeting of the Jean Piaget Society, Vancouver, Canada, June, 2005.
Legerstee, M., Pasic, N., Barillas, Y., and Fahy, L. (2004). Social emotional development: The basis for mentalism. In Gallagher, S., Watson, S., LeBrun,, P. and Romanski, P.. (eds.) Ipseity and alterity: Interdisciplinary approaches to intersubjectivity (pp. 33–46). Rouen, France: Publications de l'Université de Rouen.Google Scholar
Legerstee, M., Pomerlau, A., Malacuit, G., and Feider, H. (1987). The development of infants' responses to people and a doll: Implications for research in communication. Infant Behavior and Development, 10, 81–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legerstee, M., Beek, Y., and Varghese, M. (2002). Effects of maintaining and redirecting infant attention on the production of referential communication in infants with and without Down syndrome. Journal of Child Language, 29, 23–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Legerstee, M., and Varghese, J. (2001). The role of affect mirroring on social expectancies in three-month-old infants. Child Development, 72, 1301–1313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Legerstee, M., and Weintraub, J. (1997). The integration of person and object attention in infants with and without Down Syndrome. Infant Behavior and Development, 20, 71–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leslie, A. M. (1984). Spatiotemporal continuity and the perception of causality in infants. Perception, 13, 287–305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leung, E. H., and Rheingold, H. L. (1981). Development of pointing as a social gesture. Developmental Psychology, 17, 215–220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, M. and Brooks-Gunn, J. (1979). Social cognition and the acquisition of self., New York, Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, M. D. (1997). Personality self-organization: Cascading constraints on cognition-emotion interaction. In Lyra, M. (ed.), et-al. Fogel, A. (ed.), Dynamics and indeterminism in developmental and social processes (pp. 193–216). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
Lewis, M. D.(2000). Emotional self-organization at three time scales, The dynamics of emotion-related behaviors in infancy (pp. 37–69). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lewis, M. D., and Granic, I. (2000). The dynamics of emotion-related behaviors in infancy. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Leyendecker, B., Lamb, M. E., Schölmerich, A., and Fricke, D. M. (1997). Contexts as moderators of observed interactions: A study of Costa Rican mothers and infants from differing socioeconomic backgrounds. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 21, 15–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lillard, J., and Flavell, J. H. (1990). Young children's preference for mental state versus behavioral descriptions of human actions. Child Development, 61, 731–741.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Locke, A. (1980). The Guided Reinvention of Language. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Locke, J. (1710/1975). An essay concerning human understanding (ed. with a foreword by P. H. Nidditsch). Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Lollis, S., and Kuczynski, L. (1997). Beyond one hand clapping: Seeing bidirectionality in parent–child relations. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 14, 441–461.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loveland, K. A. (1986). Discovering the affordances of a reflecting surface. Developmental Review, 6, 1–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lukesch, H., and Lukesch, M. (1976). Fragebogen zur Messung von Einstellungen zu Schwangerschaft, Sexualität und Geburt (SSG). Göttingen: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
MacKain, K., Studder-Kennedy, M., Spieker, S., and Stern, D. (1983). Infant intermodal speech perception is a left-hemisphere function. Science, 218, 1138–1141.Google Scholar
Mahler, M. S., Pine, F., and Bergman, A. (1975). Stages in the infant's separation from the mother. In Handel, G. and Whitchurch, G. G. (eds.), The psychosocial interior of the family (4th edn, pp. 419–448). Hawthorne, US: Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Malatesta, C. Z., and Izard, C. E. (1984). The ontogenesis of human social signals: From biological imperative to symbol utilization. In Fox, N. A. and Davidson, R. J. (eds.), The Psychobiology of Affective Development (pp. 161–206). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Mandler, J. M. (1992). How to build a baby: Conceptual primitives. Psychological Review, 99, 587–604.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maratos, O. (1973). The origin and development of imitation in the first six months of life. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Geneva.
Markova, G. (Winter, 2004). PhD student participant in Graduate seminar, “Development of Affect, Consciousness and Social Cognition,” (M. Legerstee). York University.
Markova, G., and Legerstee, M. (in press). Contingency, imitation or affect sharing? Foundations of infant's social awareness, Developmental Psychology.
Markova, G., and Legerstee, M.(2004). Foundations of infant's social awareness. Paper presented at the Conference on Developmental relations between Executive Functioning, Social Understanding, and Social Interaction, September, Vancouver, BC).
Markus, J., Mundy, P., Morales, M., Delgado, C. E. F., and Yale, M. (2000). Individual differences in infant skills as predictors of child–caregiver joint attention and language. Social Development, 9, 302–315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maurer, D., and Salapatek, P. (1976). Developmental changes in the scanning of faces by young infants. Child Development, 47, 523–527.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCall, R., and Kagan, J. (1970). Fixation time and tempo of play in infants. Developmental Psychology, 3 (3, pt. 1).Google Scholar
McGurk, H. and McDonald, J. (1976). Hearing lips and seeing voices. Nature, 264, 746–748.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Fradley, E., and Tuckey, M. (2001). Rethinking maternal sensitivity: Mothers' comments on infants' mental processes predict security attachment at 12 months. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Sciences, 42, 637–648.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Wainwright, R., Clark-Carter, D., Gupta, M. D., Fradley, E., and Tuckey, M. (2002). Maternal mind-mindedness and attachment security as predictors of theory of mind understanding. Child Development, 73, 1715–1726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Wainwright, R., Clark-Carter, D., Gupta, M. D., Fradley, E., and Tuckey, M.(2003). Pathways to understanding mind: Construct validity and predictive validity of maternal mind-mindedness. Child Development, 74, 1194–1211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A. N. (1985). Immediate and deferred imitation in fourteen- and twenty-four-month-old infants. Child Development, 56, 62–72.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N.(1988). Infant imitation and memory: Nine-month-olds in immediate and deferred tests. Child Development, 59, 217–225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A. N.(1990). Foundations for developing a concept of self: The role of imitation in relating self to other and the value of social mirroring, social modelling, and self practice in infancy. In Cicchetti, D. and Beeghly, M. (eds.), The self in transition: Infancy to childhood (pp. 139–164). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N.(1995). Understanding the intentions of others: Reenactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology, 31, 838–850.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N., and Borton, R. (1979). Intermodal matching of human neonates. Nature, 282, 403–404.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A. N., and Brooks, R. (2001). “Like me” as a building block for understanding other minds: Bodily acts, attention, and intention. In Malle, B. F. and Bertram, L. J. (eds.), Intentions and intentionality: Foundations of social cognition (pp. 171–195). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N., and Gopnik, A. (1993). The role of imitation in understanding persons and developing a theory of mind. In Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., and Cohen, D. J. (eds.), Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from Autism (pp. 335–365). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N., and Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures of human neonates. Science, 198, 75–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A. N., and Moore, M. K.(1983). The origins of imitation in infancy: Paradigm, phenomena, and theories. In Lipsitt, L. P. and Rovee-Collier, C. (eds.), Advances in infancy research, 2. Norwood: NJ, Ablex.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N., and Moore, M. K.(1997). Explaining facial imitation: A theoretical model. Early Development and Parenting, 6, 179–192.3.0.CO;2-R>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A. N., and Moore, M. K.(1999). Persons and representation: Why infant imitation is important for theories of human development. In Nadel, J. and Butterworth, G. (eds.), Imitation in infancy (pp. 9–35). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Merleau-Ponty, M. (1942/1963). The structure of behavior. Trans. Fisher, A. L.. USA: Beacon Press.Google Scholar
Messer, D. (1997). Referential communication: Making sense of the social and physical worlds. In Bremner, G., Slater, A., and Butterworth, G. (eds.), Infant development: Recent advances (pp. 291–309). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Messer, S. B., Kagan, J., and McCall, R. B. (1970). Fixation time and tempo of play in infants. Developmental Psychology, 3, 406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Millar, W. S. (1972). A study of operant conditioning under delayed reinforcements in early infancy. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 37, 1–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moerk, E. L. (1989). The fuzzy set called “imitation.” In Speidel, G. E. and Nelson, K. E. (eds.), The many faces of imitation in language learning (pp. 277–303). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Molina, M., Van de Walle, G. A., Condry, K., and Spelke, E. S. (in press). The animate/inanimate distinction in infancy: Developing sensitivity to constraints on human actions. Journal of Cognition and Development.
Montague, D. P. F., and Walker-Andrews, A. S. (2001). Peekaboo: A new look at infants' perception of emotion expression. Developmental Psychology, 37, 826–838.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, C. (1996). Theories of mind in infancy. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 14, 19–40. In C. Moore, and Dunham, P. J. (eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 251–271). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Moore, C., and Corkum, V. (1994). Social understanding at the end of the first year of life. Developmental Review, 14, 349–372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, C., and D' Entremont, B. (2001). Developmental changes in pointing as a function of attentional focus. Journal of Cognition and Development, 2, 109–129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, D. G., Hobson, R. P., Lee, A., and Anderson, M. (1992, September). IQ-independent person perception: Evidence from developmental psychopathology. Paper presented at the meeting of the 5th European Conference on Developmental Psychology, Seville, Spain.
Morrissette, P., Ricard, M., and Decarie, T. G. (1995). Joint visual attention and pointing in infancy: A longitudinal study of comprehension. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13, 163–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moses, L. J. (1993). Young children's understanding of belief constraints on intention. Cognitive Development, 8, 1–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moses, L. J., Baldwin, D. A., Rosicky, J. G., and Tidball, G. (2001). Evidence for referential understanding in the emotions domain at twelve and eighteen months. Child Development, 72, 718–735.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moses, L. J., and Flavell, J. H. (1990). Inferring false beliefs from actions and reactions. Child Development, 61, 929–945.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muir, D., Cao, Y., and Entremont, B. (1994, June). Infant social perception revealed during adult face-to-face interaction. Infant Behavior and Development, 17, 86.Google Scholar
Muir, D., and Hains, S. (1999). Young infants' perception of adult intentionality: Adult contingency and eye direction. In Rochat, P. (ed.), Early social cognition: Understanding others in the first months of life (pp. 155–187). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Munakata, Y. (2000). Challenges to the violation-of-expectation paradigm: Throwing the conceptual baby out with the perceptual processing bathwater?Infancy, 1, 471–477.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mundy, P., and Sigman, M. (1989). The theoretical implications of joint-attention deficits in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 173–183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murphy, C. M., and Messer, D. J. (1977). Mothers, infants, and pointing: A study of gesture. In Schaffer, H. R. (ed.), Studies in mother–infant interaction (pp. 325–354). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Murray, L., Fior-Cowley, A., Hooper, R., and Cooper, P. (1996). The impact of postnatal depression and associated adversity on early mother–infant interactions and later infant outcomes. Child Development, 67, 2512–2526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murray, L., and Trevarthen, C. (1985). Emotional regulations of interactions between two-month-olds and their mothers. In Field, T. M. and Fox, N. (eds.), Social perception in infants. New Jersey: Ablex.Google Scholar
Nadel, J., Carchon, I., Kervella, C., Marcelli, D., and Reserbat-Plantey, D. (1999). Expectancies for social contingency in 2-month-olds. Developmental Science, 2, 164–173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakano, S. and Kanaya, Y. (1993). The effects of mothers' teasing: Do Japanese infants read their mothers' play intention in teasing?Early Development and Parenting, 2, 7–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neisser, U. (1993). The self perceived. In Neisser, U. (ed.), The perceived self: Ecological and interpersonal sources of self-knowledge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Neisser, U.(1995). Criteria for an ecological self. In Rochat, P. (eds.), The Self in Infancy: Theory and Research (pp. 17–34). Amsterdam, Netherlands: North-Holland/ Elsevier Science Publishers.Google Scholar
Nelson, L. A. (1987). The recognition of facial expression in the first two years of life: Mechanisms of development. Child Development, 58, 889–909.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nicely, P., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., and Bornstein, M. H. (1999). Mothers' attuned responses to infant affect expressivity promote earlier achievement of language milestones. Infant Behavior and Development, 22, 557–568.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicely, P., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., and Grolnick, W. S. (1999). Maternal responses to infant affect: Stability and prediction. Infant Behavior and Development, 22, 103–117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. (1986). Local traditions and cultural transmission. In Smuts, B. B., Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Wrangham, R. W., and Struhsaker, T. T. (eds.), Primate Societies (pp. 462–471).Google Scholar
Papousek, M., Papousek, H., and Symmes, D. (1991). The meanings of melodies in motherese in tone and stress languages. Infant Behavior and Development, 14, 415–440.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pascual-Leone, J., and Johnson, J. (1998). A dialectical constructivist view of representation: The role of mental attention, executives, and symbols. In Sigel, I. E. (ed.), The development of representational thought: Theoretical perspectives (pp. 169–200). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Perry, J. C., and Stern, D. N. (1976). Gaze duration frequency distributions during mother–infant interaction. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 129, 45–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representational mind. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Phillips, A. T., Wellman, H. M., and Spelke, E. (2002). Infant's ability to connect gaze and emotional expression to intentional action. Cognition, 85, 53–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phillips, W., Baron-Cohen, S., and Rutter, M. (1992). The role of eye contact in goal detection: Evidence from normal infants and children with autism or mental handicaps. Development and Psychopathology, 4, 375–383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1929). The child's conception of the world. Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Piaget, J.(1952). Play, dreams and imitation. New York: W. W. Norton and Co. Inc.Google Scholar
Piaget, J.(1954). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Piaget, J.(1981). Intelligence and affectivity: Their relationship during child development. Oxford: Annual Reviews.Google Scholar
Poulin-Dubois, D. (1999). Infants' distinction between animate and inanimate objects: The origins of naïve psychology. In Rochat, P. (ed.), Early social cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J., and Eddy, T. J. (1996). What young chimpanzees know about seeing. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 61, (3, Serial No. 247).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Premack, D. (1990). The infant's theory of self-propelled objects. Cognition, 36, 1–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Premack, D.(1991). The infant's theory of self-propelled objects. In Frye, D. and Moore, C. (eds.), Children's theories of mind: Mental states and social understanding (pp. 39–48). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rader, N., and Stern, J. D. (1982). Visually elicited reaching in neonates. Child Development, 53, 1004–1007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rakison, D. H., and Poulin-Dubois, D. (2000). Developmental origin of the animate/inanimate distinction. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 209–226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramey, C. T., and Finkelstein, N. W. (1978). Contingent stimulation and infant competence. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 3, 89–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raver, C. C., and Leadbeater, B. J. (1995). Factors influencing joint attention between socioeconomically disadvantaged adolescent mothers and their infants. In Moore, C. and Dunham, P. (eds.), Joint attention: Its origin and role in development (pp. 251–271). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Reddy, V. (1991). Playing with others' expectations: Teaching and mucking about in the first year. In Whitten, A. (ed.), Natural theories of mind: Evolution, development and simulation of everyday mindreading. Cambridge, MA: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Reddy, V.(1999). Prelinguistic communication. In Barrett, M. (ed.), The development of language. Studies in developmental psychology (pp. 25–50). Philadelphia: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Reddy, V.(2003). On being the object of attention: Implications for self–other consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 397–402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reddy, V., Hay, D., Murray, L., and Trevarthen, C. (1997). Communication in infancy: Mutual regulation of affect and attention. In Bremner, G., Slater, A., and Butterworth, G. (eds.), Infant development: Recent advances (pp. 247–273). Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Reed, E. S. (1995). Becoming a self. In Rochat, P. (ed.), The self in infancy: Theory and research. Amsterdam, Netherlands: North-Holland/Elsevier Science Publishers.Google Scholar
Repacholi, B. M. (1998). Infants' use of attentional cues to identify the referent of another person's emotional expression. Developmental Psychology, 34, 1017–1025.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Repacholi, B. M., and Gopnik, A. (1997). Early reasoning about desires: Evidence from 14– and 18-month-olds. Developmental Psychology, 33, 12–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rheingold, H. L. (1961). The effect of environmental stimulation upon social and exploratory behaviour in the human infant. In Foss, B. M. (ed.), Determinants of infant behavior (pp. 143–171). Oxford, England: Wiley.Google Scholar
Ribble, M. (1944). Infant experience in relation to personality development. In Hunt, M. V. (ed.), Personality and the Behavior Disorders (pp. 621–651). Ronald Press.Google Scholar
Rogers, S. J., and Pennington, B. F. (1991). A theoretical approach to the deficits in infantile autism. Development and Psychopathology, 3, 137–162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothbart, M. K. (1981). IBQ. The Infant Behavior Questionnaire: Measurement of temperament in infancy. Child Development, 52, 569–578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rovee-Collier, C. K., and Fagan, J. W. (1981). The retrieval of memory in early infancy. Advances in Infancy Research, 1, 225–254.Google Scholar
Ruddy, M. G., and Bornstein, M. H. (1982). Cognitive correlates of infant attention and maternal stimulation over the first year of life. Child Development, 53, 183–188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M. (1971). Parent–child separation: Psychological effects on the children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 12, 233–260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rutter, M.(1972). Maternal deprivation reassessed. Harmondsworth: Penguin.Google Scholar
Salapatek, P., and Kessen, W. (1966). Visual scanning of triangles by the human newborn. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 3, 155–167.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saxon, T. F., Frick, J. E., and Colombo, J. (1997). A longitudinal study of maternal interactional styles and infant visual attention. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 43, 48–66.Google Scholar
Scaife, M., and Bruner, J. S. (1975). The capacity for joint visual attention in the infant. Nature, 253, 265–266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schaffer, H. R. (1971). The Growth of Sociability. Harmondsworth: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Schaffer, H. R.(1984). The child's entry into a social world. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Schaffer, H. R., Collins, G. M., and Parsons, G. (1977). Vocal interchange and visual regard in verbal and preverbal children. In Schaffer, H. R. (ed.), Studies in mother–infant interaction (pp. 291–324). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Schmuckler, M. (1981). Visual habituation programme. Ontario: University of Toronto.Google Scholar
Schore, A. N. (2000). Emotion, development, and self-organization: Dynamic systems approaches to emotional development. In Lewis, M. D. and Granic, I. (eds.), Neurobiological perspectives: The self-organization of the right brain and the neurobiology of emotional development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Schultz, S., and Wellman, H. M. (1997). Explaining human movements and actions: Children's understanding of the limits of psychological explanation. Cognition, 62, 291–324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1983). Intentionality: An essay in the philosophy of mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seifer, R., Schiller, M., Sameroff, A. J., Resnick, S., and Riordan, K. (1996). Attachment, maternal sensitivity, and infant temperament during the first year of life. Developmental Psychology, 32, 12–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Helplessness: On depression, development and death. San Francisco: Freeman.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1948). Concurrent operants. American Psychologist, 3, 359.Google Scholar
Smith, P. B., and Pederson, D. R. (1988). Maternal sensitivity and patterns of infant–mother attachment. Child Development, 59, 1097–1101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snow, C. (1977). The development of conversation between mothers and babies. Child Language, 4, 1–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Soken, N. H., and Pick, A. D. (1992). Intermodal perception of happy and angry expressive behaviors by seven-month-old infants. Child Development, 63, 787–795.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spelke, E. S. (1981). The infant's acquisition of knowledge of bimodally specified events. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 31, 179–299.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spelke, E. S.(1985). Preferential-looking methods as tools for the study of cognition in infancy. In Gotlieb, G. and Krasnegor, N. A. (eds.), Measurement of audition and vision in the first year of postnatal life: A methodological overview. Westport, US: Ablex Publishing.Google Scholar
Spelke, E. S.(1988). Where perceiving ends and thinking begins: The apprehension of objects in infancy. In Yonas, A. (ed.), Perceptual development in infancy. The Minnesota symposia on child psychology (vol. 20, pp. 197–234). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Spelke, E. S., Breinlinger, K., Macomber, J., and Jacobson, K. (1992). Origins of knowledge. Psychological Review, 99, 605–632.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spelke, E. S., and Cortelyou, A. (1981). Perceptual aspects of social knowing: Looking and listening in infancy. In Lamb, M. E. and Sherrod, L. R. (eds.), Infant social cognition: Empirical and theoretical considerations. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Spelke, E. S., Phillips, A. T., and Woodward, A. L. (1995). Infants, knowledge of object motion and human action. In Sperber, D., Premack, D., and Premack, A. J. (eds.), Causal cognition: A multi-disciplinary debate (pp. 44–78). Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Sperber, D., Premack, D., and Premack, A. J. (1995). Causal cognition: A multi-disciplinary debate. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Spitz, R. (1963). Hospitalism: An inquiry into the genesis of psychiatric conditions in early childhood. Psychoanalytic Study of Child, 1, 53–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stankov, L. (1983). Attention and intelligence. Educational Psychology, 75, 471–490.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stern, D. N. (1977). The first relationship: Infant and mother. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Stern, D. N.(1984). Affect attunement. In Call, J. D., Galenson, E., and Tyson, R. T. (eds.), Frontiers of infant psychiatry (Vol. II, pp. 3–14). New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Stern, D. N.(1985). The interpersonal world of the infant. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Stern, D. N.(1995). Self/other distinction in the domain of intimate socio-affective interaction: Some considerations. In Rochat, P. (ed.), The self in infancy: Theory and research. Advances in psychology. Amsterdam: North Holland-Elsevier.Google Scholar
Sullivan, J. W., and Horowitz, F. D. (1983). Infant intermodal perception and maternal multimodal stimulation: Implications for language development. In Lipsitt, L. P. and Rovee-Collier, C. K. (eds.), Advances in infancy research (Vol. 2). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Summerfeld, Q. (1979). Use of visual information for phonetic perception. Phonetica, 36, 314–331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Szajnberg, N. M., Skrinjaric, J., and Moore, A. (1989). Affect attunement, attachment, temperament and zygosity: A twin study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 249–253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tager-Flusberg, H. (1989). An analysis of discourse ability and internal state lexicons in a longitudinal study of autistic children. Paper presented at Biennial Meeting of Society for Research in Child Development, Kansas City.
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., and Bornstein, M. H. (1989). Habituation and maternal encouragement of attention in infancy as predictors of toddler language, play, and representational competence. Child Development, 60, 738–751.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bornstein, M. H., and Baumwell, L. (2001). Maternal responsiveness and children's achievement of language milestones. Child Development, 72, 748–767.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tarabulsy, G. M., Tessier, R., and Kappas, A. (1996). Contingency detection and the contingent organization of behavior in interactions: Implications for socioemotional development in infancy. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 25–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teo, T. (1997). Developmental psychology and the relevance of a critical metatheoretical reflection. Human Development, 40, 195–210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tizard, B., and Rees, J. (1974). A comparison of the effects of adoption, restoration, to the natural mother, and continued institutionalization of the cognitive development of four-year-old children. Child Development, 45, 92–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In Moore, C. and Dunham, P. (eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 103–130). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M.(1996). Do apes ape? In Heyes, C. M. and Galef, B. G. (eds.), Social learning in animals: The roots of culture (pp. 319–346). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M.(1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M.(2000). Culture and cognitive development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 37–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M.(January, 2003), personal communication.
Tomasello, M., and Call, J. (1997). Primate cognition. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., and Camaioni, L. (1997). A comparison of the gestural communication of apes and human infants. Human Development, 40, 7–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., and Farrar, M. J. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development, 57, 1454–1463.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C., and Ratner, H. H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 495–552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trehub, S. (1976). The discrimination of foreign speech contrasts by infants and adults. Child Development, 47, 466–472.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in early infancy. A description of primary intersubjectivity. In Bullowa, M. (ed.), Before speech: The beginning of human communication (pp. 321–347). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C.(1991). The self born in intersubjectivity: The psychology of an infant communicating. In Neisser, U. (ed.), The perceived self: Ecological and interpersonal sources of self-knowledge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C., and Hubley, P. (1978). Secondary intersubjectivity: Confidence, confiding and acts of meaning in the second year. In Lock, A. (ed.), Action, gesture and symbol: The emergence of language (pp. 183–229). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Tronick, E. Z. (1989). Emotions and emotional communication in infants. American Psychologist, 44, 112–119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tronick, E. Z.(2002). A model of infant mood states: Long lasting organizing affective states and emotional representational processes without language or symbols. Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 12, 73–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tronick, E. Z.(2003). Things still to be done on the still-face effect. Infancy, 4, 475–482.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tronick, E. Z.(2004). Selective coherence and dyadic states of consciousness. In Nadel, J. and Muir, D. (eds.), Developmental studies of emotions (pp. 293–315). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valsiner, J. (1997). Culture and the development of children's action. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Boom, D. C. (1994). The influence of temperament and mothering on attachment and exploration: An experimental manipulation of sensitive responsiveness among lower-class mothers with irritable infants. Child Development, 65, 1457–1477.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vecera, S. P., and Johnson, M. H. (1995). Gaze detection and the cortical processing of faces: Evidence from infants and adults. Visual Cognition, 2, 59–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vedeler, D. (1994). Infant intentionality as object-directedness: A method for observation. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 35, 343–366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verschueren, K., Marcoen, A., and Schoef, V. (1996). The internal working model of the self, attachment, and competence in five year olds. Child Development, 67, 2493–2511.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofsten, C. (1982). Eye–hand coordination in newborns. Developmental Psychology, 18, 450–461.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. (Edited and translated by Haufmann, Eugenia and Vakar, Gertrude.) Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S.(1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. In Cole, M., John-Steiner, V., Criner, S., and Souberman, E. (eds.), Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Walden, T. A., and Ogan, T. A. (1988). The development of social referencing. Child Development, 59, 1230–1240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walker-Andrews, A. S. (1997). Infants' perception of expressive behaviors: Differentiation of multimodal information. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 437–456.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, J. B. (1928). Psychological care of infant and child. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Watson, J. S. (1972). Smiling, cooing, and “the game.”Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 18, 323–339.Google Scholar
Watson, J. S.(1979). Perception of contingency as a determinant of social responsiveness. In Thoman, E. B. (ed.), The origins of the infant's social responsiveness (pp. 33–64). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Watson, J. S.(1985). Contingency perception in early social development. In Field, T. M. and Fox, N. A. (eds.), Social perception (pp. 157–176). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing.Google Scholar
Watson, J. S., and Ramey, C. T. (1972). Reactions to response-contingent stimulation in early infancy. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 18, 219–227.Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M. (1990). The child's theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M.(1993). Early understanding of mind: The normal case. In Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., and Cohen, D. J. (eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism (pp. 10–39). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M., and Banerjee, M. (1991). Mind and emotion: Children's understanding of the emotional consequences of beliefs and desires. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 191–224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wellman, H. M., and Estes, D. (1986). Early understanding of mental entities: A reexamination of childhood realism. Child Development, 57, 910–923.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wellman, H. M., Harris, P. L., Banerjee, M., and Sinclair, A. (1995). Early understanding of emotion. Evidence from natural language. Cognition and Emotion, 9, 117–149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wellman, H. M., and Woolley, J. P. (1990). From simple desires to ordinary beliefs: The early development of everyday psychology. Cognition, 35, 245–275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Werker, J. F., and McLeod, P. J. (1989). Infant preference for both male and female infant-directed talk: A developmental study of attentional and affective responsiveness. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 43, 230–246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wimmer, H., and Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103–128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolff, P. H. (1966). The causes, controls, and organization of behavior in the neonate. Psychological Issue, 5 (Monograph 17).Google ScholarPubMed
Wolff, P. H.(1987). The development of behavioral states and the expression of emotions in early infancy: New proposals for investigation. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Woodward, A. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach. Cognition, 69, 1–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yonas, A., Petterson, L, and Lockman, J. J. (1979). Young infants' sensitivity to optical information for collision. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 33, 268–276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zeedyk, S. M. (1996). Developmental accounts of intentionality: Toward integration. Developmental Review, 16, 416–461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adamson, L. B. and Bakeman, R. (1982). Affectivity and reference: Concepts, methods, and techniques in the study of communication development of 6– to 18-month-old infants. In Field, T. M. and Fogel, A. (eds.), Emotion and early interaction. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., and Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Astington, J. W. (2001). The paradox of intention: Assessing children's metarepresentational understanding. In Malle, B. F., Moses, L. J., and Baldwin, D. A. (eds.), Intentions and intentionality: Foundations of social cognition (pp. 85–103). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bahrick, L. R. (1988) Intermodal learning in infancy: Learning on the basis of two kinds of invariant relations in audible and visible events. Child Development, 59, 197–209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bahrick, L. R., Moss, L., and Fadil, C. (1996). Development of visual self-recognition in infancy. Ecological Psychology, 8, 189–208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bahrick, L. R., and Watson, J. S. (1985). Detection of intermodal proprioceptive-visual contingency as a potential basis of self-perception in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 21, 963–973.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baillargeon, R. (1986). Representing the existence and the location of hidden objects: Object permanence in 6–8-month-old infants. Cognition, 23, 21–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baillargeon, R.(1993). The object concept revisited: New directions in the investigation of infants' physical knowledge. In Granrud, C. (ed.), Visual perception and cognition in infancy (pp. 265–315). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bakeman, R., and Adamson, L. (1984). Coordinating attention to people and objects in mother–infant and peer–infant interaction. Child Development, 55, 1278–1289.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baldwin, D. A., and Moses, L. J. (1994). The mindreading engine: Evaluating evidence for modularity. Cahiers der Psychologie, 13, 553–560.Google Scholar
Baldwin, D. A., and Moses, L. J(1996). Early understanding of referential intent and attentional focus: Evidence from language and emotion. In Lewis, C. and Mitchell, P. (eds.), Children's early understanding of mind: Origins and development (pp. 133–156). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Baldwin, J. M. (1902). Social and ethical interpretations in mental development. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Ball, A. W. (1973). The perception of causality in the infant. Presented at the Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Philadelphia, PA.
Ball, A. W., and Tronick, E. (1971). Infant responses to impending collision: Optical and real. Science, 171, 818–820.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bandura, A. (1962). Social cognitive theory of social referencing. In Feinman, S. (ed.), Social learning through imitation. University of Nebraska Press: Lincoln, NE.Google Scholar
Banks, M. S., and Salapatek, P. (1983). Infant visual perception. In Haith, M. M. and Campos, J. J. (eds.), Infancy and developmental psychobiology (vol. II of P. H. Mussen [ed.], Handbook of Child Psychology), 4th edn. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Barna, J., and Legerstee, M. (2005). Nine and 12-month-old infants relate emotions to people's actions. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 53–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). Are autistic children “behaviorists”? An examination of their mental–physical and appearance–reality distinctions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 579–600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S.(1991). Precursors to a theory of mind: Understanding attention in others. In Whiten, A. (ed.), Natural theories of mind: Evolution, development and simulation of everyday mindreading. Cambridge, MA: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S.(1993). From attentional-goal psychology to belief-desire psychology. The development of theory of mind and its dysfunction. In Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., and Cohen, D. J. (eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism (pp. 59–82). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S.(1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Campbell, R., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Grant, J., and Walker, S. (1995). Are children with autism blind to the mentalistic significance of the eyes?British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13, 379–398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrera, M. E., and Maurer, D. (1981). Recognition of mother's photographed face by the three-month-old infant. Child Development, 52, 714–716.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barresi, J. (1984). Knowledge and representation: Transparent and opaque. Contemporary Psychology, 29, 160–161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barresi, J., and Moore, C. (1996). Intentional relations and social understanding. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 19, 107–154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, M. D. (1989). Early language development. In Slater, A. and Bremner, J. G. (eds.), Infant development (pp. 211–241). London: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bartsch, K., and Wellman, H. M. (1995). Children talk about the mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Baumwell, L., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., and Borstein, M. H. (1997). Maternal verbal sensitivity and child language comprehension. Infant Behavior and Development, 20, 247–258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bayley, N. (1969). Bayley scales of infant development. New York: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., March, J. E., and Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561–571.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beebe, B., Stern, D., and Jaffe, J. (1979). The kinesic rhythm of mother-infant interactions. In Siegman, A. W. & Feldstein, S. (eds.), Of speech and time: Temporal patterns in interpersonal contexts. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bellagamba, F. and Tomasello, M. (1999). Re-enacting intended acts: comparing 12– and 18-month-olds. Infant Behavior and Development, 22, 277–282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berthenthal, B. I., and Bai, D. L. (1989). Infants' sensitivity to optical flow for controlling posture. Developmental Psychology, 25, 936–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berkley, G. (1975). Philosophical works. Edited by Ayers, M. R.. London: Dent.Google Scholar
Bigelow, A. E. (1998). Infants' sensitivity to familiar imperfect contingencies in social interaction. Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 149–161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F. (2005). Children's thinking. Cognitive development and individual differences. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth/Thomson.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., and Lamb, M. E. (1992). Development in infancy: An introduction (3rd edn, xiv, pp. 560). New York, NY, England: Mcgraw-Hill Book Company.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., and Sigman, M. D. (1986). Continuity in mental development from infancy. Child Development, 57, 251–274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bornstein, M. H., and Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (1989a). Maternal responsiveness and cognitive development in children. In Bornstein, M. H. (ed.), Maternal responsiveness: Characteristics and consequences (pp. 49–61). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., and Tamis-LeMonda, C. S.(1989b). Habituation and maternal encouragement of attention in infancy as predictors of toddler language, play, and representational competence. Child Development, 60, 738–751.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., and Tamis-LeMonda, C. S.(1997). Maternal responsiveness and infant mental abilities: Specific predictive relations. Infant Behavior and Development, 20, 283–296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1951). Maternal care and mental health. London: HMSO; New York: Columbia University.Google ScholarPubMed
Bowlby, J.(1969). Attachment and loss. Vol. I: Attachment. Pelican Books, Penguin.Google Scholar
Bowlby, J.(1979). On knowing what you are not supposed to know and feeling what you are not supposed to feel. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 24, 403–408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brazelton, T. B., Koslowski, B., and Main, M. (1974). The origins of reciprocity: The early mother–infant interaction. In Lewis, M. and Rosenblum, L. (eds.), The effect of the infant on its caregiver (pp. 49–76). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bremner, J. G. (1988). Infancy. Cambridge, MA: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bremner, J. G.(1998). From perception to action: The early development of knowledge. In Simion, F. and Butterworth, G. (eds.), The development of sensory, motor and cognitive capacities in early infancy: From perception to cognition (pp. 239–255). Hove, England: Psychology Press/Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Brentano, F. (1874/1973). Psychology from an empirical standpoint. Trans. A. C. Rancurello, D. B. Terrel, and L. L. McAlister. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Bretherton, I. and Beeghly, M. (1982). Talking about internal states: The acquisition of an explicit theory of mind. Developmental Psychology, 18, 906–921.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1974). A report on longitudinal evaluations of pre-school programs, Vol. 2, Is early intervention effective? Washington, DC: D. H. W. Publications No. (OHD) 74–25.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1973). Organization of early skilled action. Child Development, 44, 11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruner, J. S.(1975). From communication to language: A psychological perspective. Cognition, 3, 255–287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. S.(1983). Child's talk: Learning to use language. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S.(1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, J.(1999). The intentionality of referring. In Zelazo, P. and Astington, J. W. (eds.), Developing theories of intention: Social understanding and self-control (pp. 329–339). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S., and Koslowski, B. (1972). Visually preadapted constituents of manipulatory action. Perception, 1, 3–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bushnell, I. W. R., Sai, F., and Mullin, J. T. (1989). Neonatal recognition of the mother's face. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7, 3–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butterworth, G. (1990). On reconceptualizing sensorimotor development in dynamic systems theory. In Bloch, H. and Berthenthal, B. J. (eds.), Sensorimotor organizations and development in infancy and early childhood (pp. 57–73). Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butterworth, G.(1991). The ontogeny and phylogeny of joint visual attention. In Whiten, A. (ed.), National theories of mind: Evolution, development, and simulation of everyday mind reading (pp. 223–232). Cambridge, MA: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Butterworth, G.(1994). Theories of mind and the facts of embodiment. In Lewis, C. and Mitchell, P. (eds.), Children's early understanding of mind: Origins and development (pp. 115–132). Sussex, England: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Butterworth, G.(1995). Factors in visual attention eliciting manual pointing in human infancy. In Raiblat, H. L. and Meyer, J. (eds.), Comparative approaches to cognitive science (pp. 329–338). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Butterworth, G., and Hicks, L. (1977). Visual proprioception and postural stability in infancy: A developmental study. Perception, 6, 255–262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butterworth, G., and Hopkins, B. (1988). Hand–mouth coordination in the new-born baby. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 6, 303–314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butterworth, G., and Jarrett, N. (1991). What minds have in common in space: Spatial mechanisms serving joint visual attention in infancy. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 55–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camaioni, L., Perucchini, P., Bellagamba, F., and Colonnesi, R. (2004), The role of declarative pointing in developing a Theory of Mind. Infancy, 5, 291–308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camaioni, L., Perucchini, P., Muratori, F., and Milone, A. (1997). Brief report: A longitudinal examination of the communicative gestures deficit in young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27, 715–725.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, S. B., Cohn, J., and Myers, T. (1995). Depression in first-time mothers: Mother–infant interaction and depression chronicity. Developmental Psychology, 31, 349–357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campos, J., and Sternberg, C. (1981). Perception, appraisal, and emotion: The onset of social referencing. In Lamb, M. and Sherrod, L. (eds.), Infant social cognition: Empirical and theoretical considerations (pp. 273–314). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual Change in Childhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Caron, A., Carfon, R., Mustelin, C., and Roberts, J. (1992). Infant responding to aberrant social stimuli. Infant Behavior and Development, 15, 335 (Special ICIS Conference Issue).Google Scholar
Carpenter, M., Nagell, L., and Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63 (4, serial no. 255).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chada, S. W. (1996). A comparison of mother–infant, father–infant and stranger–infant affect attunement: Characteristics and consequences of discrete parenting roles in infant affective development. Dissertation Abstracts International, 52 (12-B), 7060.Google Scholar
Chapman, M. (1992). Equilibration and the dialectic of organization. In Beiling, H. and Putfall, P. (eds.), Piaget's theory: Prospects and possibilities (pp. 39–59). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Baird, G., Cox, A., and Drew, A. (2000). Testing joint attention, imitation, and play as infancy precursors to language and theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 15, 481–498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charman, T., Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Baird, G., and Drew, A. (1997). Infants with autism: An investigation of empathy, pretend play, joint attention, and imitation. Developmental Psychology, 33, 781–789.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chi, M. T. H. (1988). Children's lack of access and knowledge reorganization: An example from the concept of animacy. In Weinert, F. E. and Perlmuter, M. (eds.), Memory development: Universal changes and individual differences (pp. 169–194). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Churchland, P. M. (1991). Folk-psychology and the explanation of human behavior. In Greenwood, J. D. (ed.), The future of folk psychology: Intentionality and cognitive science. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., and Schneider-Rosen, K. (1984). Toward a transactional model of childhood depression. New Directions for Child Development, 26, 5–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohn, J. F., and Tronick, E. Z. (1989). Specificity of infants' response to mothers' affective behavior. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 242–248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corkum, V., and Moore, C. (1995). Development of joint visual attention in infants. In Moore, C. and Dunham, P. J. (eds.), Joint attention: its origins and role in development (pp. 61–83). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Corkum, V., and Moore, C.(1998). The origin of joint visual attention in infants. Developmental Psychology, 34, 28–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coster, W. J., Gersten, M. S., Beegly, M., and Cicchetti, D. (1989). Communicative functioning in maltreated toddlers. Developmental Psychology, 25, 1020–1029.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crockenberg, S. (1983). Early mother and infant antecedents of Bayley Scale performance at 21 months. Developmental Psychology, 19, 727–730.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Csibra, G., Gergely, G., Biro, S., Koos, O., and Brockbank, M. (1999). Social attribution without agency cues: The perception of “pure reason” in infancy. Cognition, 72, 237–267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darwin, C. (1877). A biographical sketch of an infant. Mind, 2, 285–294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, K. (1947). Final note on a case of extreme isolation. American Journal of Sociology, 52, 432–437.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeCasper, A. J., and Carstens, A. A. (1981). Contingencies of stimulation: Effects on learning and emotion in neonates. Infant Behavior and Development, 4, 19–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeCasper, A. J., and Fifer, W. P. (1980). Of human bonding: Newborns prefer their mother's voice. Science, 208, 1174–1176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeCasper, A. J., Lecanuet, J. P., Busnell, M. C., Granier-Deferre, C., and Maugeais, R. (1994). Fetal reactions to recurrent maternal speech sounds. Infant Behavior and Development, 9, 133–150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeCharmes, R. (1968). Personal Causation: The Internal Affective Determinants of behavior. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Descartes, R. (1641/1985). The philosophical writings of Descartes (Vols. 1 and 2). Trans. J. Cottingham et al. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Desrochers, S., Morrissette, P., and Ricard, M. (1995). Two perspectives on pointing in infancy. In Moore, C. and Dunham, P. J. (eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 85–101). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Dodd, B. (1979). Lip reading in infants: Attention to speech present in- and out-of-synchrony. Cognitive psychology, 11, 478–484.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dondi, M., Simion, F., and Caltran, G. (1999). Can newborns discriminate between their own cry and the cry of another newborn infant?Developmental Psychology, 35, 418–426.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunham, P., and Dunham, F. (1990). Effects of mother–infant social interactions on infants' subsequent contingency task performance. Child Development, 61, 785–793.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunham, P., Dunham, F., Hurshman, A., and Alexander, T. (1989). Social contingency effects on subsequent perceptual-cognitive tasks in young infants. Child Development, 60, 1486–1496.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Emde, R. N. (1989). Early emotional development: New modes of thinking for research and intervention. In Warhol, J. G. (ed.), New perspectives in early emotional development (pp. 29–45). Johnson and Johnson Pediatric Institute.Google Scholar
Eimas, P. D., Siqueland, E. R., Juscyk, P., and Vigorito, J. (1971). Speech perception in infants. Science, 171, 303–306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P., and Friesen, W. V. (1975). Unmasking the face: A guide to recognizing emotions from facial clues. New York: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Ellsworth, C. P., Muir, D., and Hains, S. (1993). Social competence and person–object differentiation: An analysis of the still-face effect. Developmental Psychology, 39, 63–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fagan, J. F. (1972). Infants' recognition memory for faces. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 14, 453–476.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feinman, S. (1982). Social referencing in infancy. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 28, 445–470.Google Scholar
Feinman, S., and Lewis, M. (1983). Social referencing at 10-months. A second order effect on infants' responses to strangers. Child Development, 54, 878–887.Google Scholar
Field, T. (1984). Early interactions between infants and their postpartum depressed mothers. Infant Behaviour and Development, 7, 527–532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Field, T.(1990). Neonatal stress and coping in intensive care. Infant Mental Health Journal, 11, 57–65.3.0.CO;2-Y>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Field, T.(1992). Psychobiological attunement in close relationships. In Lerner, R., Featherman, D. L., and Perlmutter, M. (eds.), Life-span development and behavior (pp. 2–22). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Field, T.(1994). The effects of mother's physical and emotional unavailability on emotion regulation. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59 (23).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Field, T.(1995). Infant of depressed mothers (Presidential address). Infant Behavior and Development, 18, 1–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Field, T., Cohen, D., Garcia, , and Greenberg, R. (1985). Mother–stranger face discrimination by the newborn. Annual Progress in Child Psychiatry and Child Development, 3–10.Google Scholar
Field, T., Guy, L., and Umbel, V. (1985). Infants' responses to mothers' imitative behaviors. Infant Mental Health Journal, 6, 40–44.3.0.CO;2-L>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Field, T., Healy, B., Goldstein, S., Perry, S., Berry, D., Schanberg, S., Zimmerman, E. A., and Kuhn, C. (1998). Infants of depressed mothers show “depressed” behavior even with nondepressed adults. Child Development, 59, 1569–1579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Field, T., Woodson, R., Greenberg, R., and Cohen, D. (1982). Discrimination and imitation of facial expressions of neonates. Science, 218, 179–181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Finkelstein, N. W., and Ramey, C. T. (1977). Learning to control the environment in infancy. Child Development, 48, 806–819.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, K. W., Shaver, P. R., and Carnochan, P. (1990). How emotions develop and how they organize development. Cognition and Emotion, 4, 81–127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fisher, L., Ames, E. W., Chisholm, K., and Savoie, L. (1997). Problems reported by parents of Romanians orphans adopted to British Columbia. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 20, 67–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flavell, J. H. (1988). The development of the children's knowledge about the mind: From cognitive connections to mental representations. In Astington, J. W. and Harris, P. L. (eds.), Developing theories of mind. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Flavell, J. H.(1999). Cognitive development: Children's knowledge about the mind. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 21–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flavell, J. H., Flavell, E. R., Green, F. L., and Moses, L. J. (1990). Young children's understanding of false beliefs versus value beliefs. Child Development, 61, 915–928.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flavell, J. H., Green, F. L., and Flavell, E. R. (1986). Development of knowledge about the appearance–reality distinction. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 51, 1–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of the mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A.(1992). A theory of the child's theory of mind. Cognition, 44, 283–296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fogel, A. (1977). Temporal organization in mother–infant face-to-face interaction. In Schaffer, H. Rudolph (ed.), Studies in mother–infant interaction (pp. 5–56). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Fogel, A.(1993). Developing through relationships: Origins of communication, self and culture. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Fogel, A.(2001). The history (and future) of infancy. Blackwell handbook of infant development. Handbooks of developmental psychology (pp. 726–757). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishers.Google Scholar
Fogel, A., and Hannan, T. E. (1985). Manual actions of nine to fifteen-week-old human infants during face-to-face interaction with their mothers. Child Development, 56, 1271–1279.Google ScholarPubMed
Fogel, A., and Thelen, E. (1987). Development of early expressive and communicative action: Reinterpreting the evidence from a dynamic systems perspective. Developmental Psychology, 23, 747–761.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franco, F., and Butterworth, G. (1996). Pointing and social awareness: Declaring and requesting in the second year. Journal of Child Language, 23, 307–336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, W. J. (2000). Emotion is essential to all intentional behaviors. In Granic, I. and Lewis, M. (eds.), Emotion, development, and self-organization: Dynamic systems approaches to emotional development (pp. 209–235). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1949). An outline of psychoanalysis (rev. edn, Strachey). New York: Norton (original work published 1940).Google Scholar
Freud, S.(1961). The ego and the id. New York: W. W. Norton and Co.Google Scholar
Frye, D. (1981). Developmental changes in strategies of social interaction. In Lamb, M. and Sherrod, L. (eds.), Infant social cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gallagher, S. (1996). The moral significance of primitive self-consciousness: The response to Bermúdez. Ethics, 107, 129–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallup, G. G. (1982). Self-awareness and the emergence of mind in primates. American Journal of Primatology, 2, 237–248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gekoski, M. J., and Fagen, J. W. (1984). Noncontingent stimulation, stimulus familiarization, and subsequent learning in young infants. Child Development, 55, 2226–2233.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gelman, R., and Spelke, E. (1981). The development of thoughts about animate and inanimate objects: Implications for research on social cognition. In Flavell, J. H. and Ross, L. (eds.), Social cognition development: Frontiers and possible futures. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gelman, S. A., and Coley, J. D. (1990). The importance of knowing a dodo is a bird: Categories and inferences in 2-year-old children. Developmental Psychology, 26, 796–804.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gelman, S. A., Durgin, F., and Kaufman, L. (1995). Distinguishing between animates and inanimates: Not by notion alone. In Sperber, D. and Premack, D. (eds.), Causal cognition: A multidisciplinary debate (pp. 150–184). Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Gelman, S. A., and Markman, E. M. (1986). Categories and induction in young children. Cognition, 23, 183–209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gergely, G. (2001). Is early differentiation of human behavior a precursor to the 1-year-old's understanding of intentional action? Comment on Legerstee, Barna, and DiAdamo (2000). Developmental Psychology, 37, 579–582.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gergely, G.(2002). The development of understanding self and agency. In Goswamia, Usha (ed.), Blackwell's Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development. Malden, MA: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gergely, G., and Watson, J. (1996). The social biofeedback theory of parental affect-mirroring: The development of emotional self-awareness and self-control in infancy. International Journal of Psycho-Analysis, 77, 1181–1212.Google ScholarPubMed
Gergely, G., and Watson, J.(1999). Early socio-emotional development: Contingency perception and the social biofeedback model. In Rochat, P. (ed.), Early social cognition: Understanding others in the first months of life (pp. 101–136). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gergely, G., Nadasdy, Z., Csibry, G., and Biro, S. (1995). Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition, 56, 165–193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibson, E. J. (1969). Principles of perceptual learning and development. East Norwalk, US: Appleton Century Crofts.Google Scholar
Gibson, E. J.(1993). Ontogenesis of the perceived self. In Neisser, U. (ed.), The perceived self: Ecological and interpersonal sources of self-knowledge (pp. 25–42). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gibson, E. J.(1995). Are we automata? In Rochat, P. (ed.), The self in infancy: Theory and research. Advances in psychology (pp. 13–15). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Glick, J. (1978). Cognition and social cognition: an introduction. In Glick, J. L. and Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (eds.), The development of social understanding, (pp. 1–9). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Glick, J., and Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (eds.) (1978). The development of social understanding. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Goldberg, S., Lojkasek, M., Gartner, G., and Corter, C. (1989). Maternal responsiveness and social development in pre-term infants. In Bornstein, M. H. (ed.), Maternal responsiveness: Characteristics and consequences (pp. 89–103). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Goldfarb, W. (1943). Infant rearing and problem behavior. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 13, 249–265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldsmith, D. F., and Rogoff, B. (1997). Mothers' and toddlers' coordinated joint focus of attention: Variations with maternal dysphoric symptoms. Developmental Psychology, 33, 113–119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gopnik, A. (1995). How to understand beliefs. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18, 398–400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, A., and Meltzoff, A. N. (1997). Words, thoughts, and theories. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A., and Wellman, H. M. (1992). Why the child's theory of mind really is a theory. Mind and Language, 7, 145–171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goren, C. C., Sarty, M., and Wu, P. Y. K. (1975). Visual following and pattern discrimination of face-like stimuli by newborn infants. Pediatrics, 56, 544–549.Google ScholarPubMed
Gotlieb, S. J. (1991). Visual memory in neonates. Dissertation Abstracts International, 51 (10-B), 5049–5050.Google Scholar
Granic, I. (2000) Emotion, development, and self-organization: dynamic systems approaches to emotional development. In Lewis, M. D. and Granic, I. (eds.), The self-organization of parent–child relations: Beyond bidirectional models (pp. 267–297). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Grant, K. W., Ardell, L. H., Kuhl, P. K., and Sparks, D. W. (1986). The transmission of prosodic information via an electrotactile speedreading aid. Ear and Hearing, 7, 243–251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hains, S. M., and Muir, D. W. (1996). Effects of stimulus contingency in infant–adult interactions. Infant Behavior and Development, 19, 49–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haith, M. M. (1966). The response of the human newborn to visual movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Child Psychology, 3, 243–253.Google ScholarPubMed
Haith, M. M., Bergman, T., and Moore, M. J. (1977). Eye contact and face scanning in early infancy. Science, 198, 853–855.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harding, C. G. (1982). Development of the intention to communicate. Human Development, 25, 140–151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harlow, H. F. (1958). The nature of love. American Psychologist, 13, 673–685.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harlow, H. F.(1961). The development of affectional patterns in infant monkeys. In Foss, B. M. (ed.), Determinants of infant behaviour (Vol. I). London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Harlow, H. F. and Harlow, M. K. (1965). The affectional systems. In Schrier, A. M., Harlow, H. F., and Stollnitz, F. (eds.), Behavior of nonhuman primates (Vol. II, pp. 287–334). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Harlow, H. F., and Zimmermann, R. R. (1959). Affectional responses in the infant monkey. Science, 130, 421–432.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, P. L. (1989). Children and emotion: The development of psychological understanding. Oxford, England: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Harris, P. L.(1992). From simulation to folk psychology: The case for development. Mind Language, 7, 120–144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haviland, J. M., and Lewica, M. (1987). The induced affect response: 10-week-old infants' responses to three emotion expressions. Developmental Psychology, 23, 97–104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heyes, C. M. (1998). Theory of mind in nonhuman primates. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, bbsonline.org.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hobson, R. P. (1989). Beyond cognition: A theory of autism. In Dawson, G. (ed.), Autism, Nature, diagnosis and treatment (pp. 48–). New York, Guilford.Google Scholar
Hobson, R. P.(1989). On sharing experiences. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 197–203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobson, R. P.(1990). On the origins of self and the case of autism. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 163–181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobson, R. P.(1993). Autism and the development of mind. Hove: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hobson, R. P.(1998). The intersubjective foundations of thought. In Braten, S. (ed.), Intersubjective communication and emotion in early ontogeny: Studies in emotion and social interaction (2nd series, pp. 283–296). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hobson, R. P.(2002). The cradle of thought: Exploring the origins of thinking. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hoffman, M. (1981). Perspectives on the difference between understanding people and understanding things: the role of affect. In Flavell, H. and Ross, L. (eds.), Social cognitive development: Frontiers and possible futures, pp. 67–81. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hofsten, C. (1980). Predictive reaching for moving objects by human infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 30, 369–382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hood, B. M., Willen, J. D., and Driver, J. (1998). Adults' eyes trigger shifts of visual attention in human infants. Psychological Science, 9, 131–134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hornick, R., Risenhoover, N., and Gunnar, M. (1987). The effects of maternal positive, neutral, and negative affective communications on infant responses to new toys. Child Development, 58, 937–944.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hsu, H. C., and Fogel, A., (2003). Stability and transitions in mother–infant face-to-face communication during the first 6 months: A micro-historical approach. Developmental Psychology, 39, 1061–1082.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hsu, H. C., Fogel, A., and Messinger, D. S. (2001). Infant non-distress vocalization during mother–infant face-to-face interaction: Factors associated with quantitative and qualitative differences. Infant Behavior and Development, 24, 107–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hume, D. (1739/1888). A treatise of human nature. Ed. Selby-Bigge, L. A.. Oxford: Clarendon.Google Scholar
Izard, C. E. (1971). The face of emotion. East Norwalk: Appleton Century Crofts.Google Scholar
Izard, C. E.(1978). Emotions as motivations: An evolutionary-developmental perspective. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 26, 163–200.Google Scholar
Jaffe, J., Beebe, B., Feldstein, S., Crown, C. L., and Jasnow, M. D. (2001). Rhythms of dialogue in infancy. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 66, 2001.Google ScholarPubMed
James, W. (1890). Principles of psychology. New York: Holt.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. H., Dziurawiec, S., Ellis, H. D., and Morton, J. (1991). Newborns' preferential tracking of face-like stimuli and its subsequent decline. Cognition, 40, 1–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, M. H., and Morton, J. (1991). Biology and cognitive development: The case of face recognition. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Johnson, S. C., Booth, A., and O'Hearn, L. (2001). Inferring the goals of a non-human agent. Cognitive Development, 16, 637–656.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, S., Slaughter, V., and Carey, S. (1998). Whose gaze will infants follow? The elicitation of gaze following in 12-month-olds. Developmental Science, 1, 233–238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Juscyk, P. W., Kennedy, L. J., and Juscyk, A. (1995). Young infants' retention of information about syllables. Infant Behavior and Development, 18, 24–41.Google Scholar
Kagan, J. (1976). Resilience and continuity in psychological development. In Clarke, A. M. and Clarke, A. D. B. (eds.), Early experience: Myth and evidence (pp. 97–121). New York: The Free Press.Google Scholar
Kalins, I. V., and Bruner, J. S. (1973). The coordination of visual observation and instrumental behavior in early infancy. Perception, 2, 307–314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kant, I. (1781/1996). Critique of pure reason. Trans. W. S. Pluhar. Indianapolis: Hackett.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A.(1998). Is atypical development necessarily a window on the normal mind/brain? The case of Williams Syndrome. Developmental-Science, 1, 273–277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A., Klima, E., Bellugi, U., Grant, J., and Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Is there a social module? Language, face processing, and theory of mind in individuals with Williams syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 196–208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaye, K. (1982). Self-image formation in adopted children: The environment within. Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy, 13, 175–181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaye, K., and Fogel, A. (1980). The temporal structure of face-to-face communication between mothers and infants. Developmental Psychology, 16, 454–464.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaye, K., and Markus, J. (1981). Infant imitation: The sensory-motor agenda. Developmental Psychology, 3, 258–265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keil, C. K., Smith, W. C., Simons, D. J., and Levin, D. T. (1998). Two dogmas of conceptual empiricism: Implications for hybrid models of the structure of knowledge. Cognition, 65, 103–135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenny, J. (1988), The self. Marquette: Marquette University Press.Google Scholar
Killen, M., and Uzgiris, I. C. (1981). Imitation of action with objects: The role of social meaning. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 138, 219–229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koluchova, J. (1972). Severe deprivation in twins: A case study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 13, 107–114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuchuk, A., Vibbert, N. M., and Bornstein, M. H. (1986). The perception of smiling and its experiential correlates in three-month-old infants. Child Development, 57, 1054–1061.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhl, P., and Meltzoff, A. N. (1982). The bimodal perception of speech in infancy. Science, 218, 1138–1141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LaBarbera, J. D., Izard, C. E., Vietze, P., and Parisi, S. A. (1976). Four- and six-month-old infants' visual responses to joy, anger, and neutral expressions. Child Development, 47, 535–538.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Millar-Loncar, C. L., and Swank, P. R. (1998). The relation of change in maternal interactive styles to the developing social competence in full-term and pre-term children. Child Development, 69, 105–123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legerstee, M. (1990). Infants use multimodal stimulation to imitate speech sounds. Infant Behavior and Development, 13, 345–356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legerstee, M.(1991a). Changes in the quality of infant sounds as a function of social and nonsocial stimulation. First Language, 11, 327–343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legerstee, M.(1991b). The role of people and objects in early imitation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 51, 423–433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legerstee, M.(1992). A review of the animate/inanimate distinction in infancy: Implications for models of social and cognitive knowing. Early Development and Parenting, 1, 59–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legerstee, M.(1994a). Patterns of 4-month-old infant responses to hidden silent and sounding people and objects. Early Development and Parenting, 3, 71–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legerstee, M.(1994b). The role of familiarity and sound in the development of person and object permanence. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 12, 455–468.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legerstee, M.(1997a). Changes in social-conceptual development: domain specific structures, self-organization and indeterminism. In Fogel, A., Lyra, M. C. D. P., and Valsiner, J. (eds.), Dynamics and indeterminism in development and social processes (pp. 245–260). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Legerstee, M.(1997b). Contingency effects of people and objects on subsequent cognitive functioning in three-month-old infants. Social Development, 6, 307–321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legerstee, M.(1998). Mental and bodily awareness in infancy: Consciousness of self-existence. Journal of Consciousness, 5, 627–644.Google Scholar
Legerstee, M.(2001a). Domain specificity and the epistemic triangle in the development of animism in infancy. In Lacerda, F., Hofsten, C., and Heinemann, M. (eds.), Emerging cognitive abilities in early infancy (pp. 193–212). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Legerstee, M.(2001b). Six-month-old infants rely on explanatory inference when relating communication to people and manipulatory actions to inanimate objects: Reply to Gergely. Developmental Psychology, 5, 583–586.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legerstee, M., Anderson, D., and Schaffer, A. (1998). Five- and eight-month-old infants recognize their faces and voices as familiar and social stimuli. Child Development, 69, 37–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Legerstee, M., and Barillas, Y. (2003). Sharing attention and pointing to objects at 12 months: Is the intentional stance implied?Cognitive Development, 18, 91–110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legerstee, M., Barna, J., and DiAdamo, C. (2000). Precursors to the development of intention at 6 months: Understanding people and their actions. Developmental Psychology, 3, 627–634.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legerstee, M., and Bowman, T. (1989). The development responses to people and a toy in infants with Down syndrome. Infant Behaviour and Development, 12, 462–473.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legerstee, M., Bowman, T., and Fels, S. (1992). People and objects affect the quality of vocalizations in infants with Down syndrome. Early Development and Parenting, 1, 149–156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legerstee, M., Corter, C., and Kienapple, K. (1990). Hand, arm and facial actions of young infants to a social and nonsocial stimulus. Child Development, 61, 774–784.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Legerstee, M., Fahy, L., Blake, J., Fisher, T., and Markova, G. (May, 2004). Maternal Factors Contributing to Toddlers' Early Development of Internal State Language: A Longitudinal Study. Poster presented at 14th Biennial International Conference on Infant Studies (ICIS), Chicago, USA.Google Scholar
Legerstee, M., and Feider, H. (1986). The acquisition of person pronouns in French-speaking children. International Journal of Psychology, 21, 629–639.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legerstee, M., Fisher, T., and Markova, G. (2005). The development of attention during dyadic and triadic interactions: The role of affect attunement. Paper presented at 35th Annual Meeting of the Jean Piaget Society, Vancouver, Canada, June, 2005.
Legerstee, M., and Markova, G. (2005). Variation in imitation in 10-month-old infants: Awareness of intentional action. Paper presented at 35th annual meeting of the Jean Piaget Society, Vancouver, Canada, June, 2005.
Legerstee, M., Pasic, N., Barillas, Y., and Fahy, L. (2004). Social emotional development: The basis for mentalism. In Gallagher, S., Watson, S., LeBrun,, P. and Romanski, P.. (eds.) Ipseity and alterity: Interdisciplinary approaches to intersubjectivity (pp. 33–46). Rouen, France: Publications de l'Université de Rouen.Google Scholar
Legerstee, M., Pomerlau, A., Malacuit, G., and Feider, H. (1987). The development of infants' responses to people and a doll: Implications for research in communication. Infant Behavior and Development, 10, 81–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legerstee, M., Beek, Y., and Varghese, M. (2002). Effects of maintaining and redirecting infant attention on the production of referential communication in infants with and without Down syndrome. Journal of Child Language, 29, 23–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Legerstee, M., and Varghese, J. (2001). The role of affect mirroring on social expectancies in three-month-old infants. Child Development, 72, 1301–1313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Legerstee, M., and Weintraub, J. (1997). The integration of person and object attention in infants with and without Down Syndrome. Infant Behavior and Development, 20, 71–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leslie, A. M. (1984). Spatiotemporal continuity and the perception of causality in infants. Perception, 13, 287–305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leung, E. H., and Rheingold, H. L. (1981). Development of pointing as a social gesture. Developmental Psychology, 17, 215–220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, M. and Brooks-Gunn, J. (1979). Social cognition and the acquisition of self., New York, Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, M. D. (1997). Personality self-organization: Cascading constraints on cognition-emotion interaction. In Lyra, M. (ed.), et-al. Fogel, A. (ed.), Dynamics and indeterminism in developmental and social processes (pp. 193–216). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
Lewis, M. D.(2000). Emotional self-organization at three time scales, The dynamics of emotion-related behaviors in infancy (pp. 37–69). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lewis, M. D., and Granic, I. (2000). The dynamics of emotion-related behaviors in infancy. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Leyendecker, B., Lamb, M. E., Schölmerich, A., and Fricke, D. M. (1997). Contexts as moderators of observed interactions: A study of Costa Rican mothers and infants from differing socioeconomic backgrounds. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 21, 15–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lillard, J., and Flavell, J. H. (1990). Young children's preference for mental state versus behavioral descriptions of human actions. Child Development, 61, 731–741.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Locke, A. (1980). The Guided Reinvention of Language. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Locke, J. (1710/1975). An essay concerning human understanding (ed. with a foreword by P. H. Nidditsch). Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Lollis, S., and Kuczynski, L. (1997). Beyond one hand clapping: Seeing bidirectionality in parent–child relations. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 14, 441–461.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loveland, K. A. (1986). Discovering the affordances of a reflecting surface. Developmental Review, 6, 1–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lukesch, H., and Lukesch, M. (1976). Fragebogen zur Messung von Einstellungen zu Schwangerschaft, Sexualität und Geburt (SSG). Göttingen: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
MacKain, K., Studder-Kennedy, M., Spieker, S., and Stern, D. (1983). Infant intermodal speech perception is a left-hemisphere function. Science, 218, 1138–1141.Google Scholar
Mahler, M. S., Pine, F., and Bergman, A. (1975). Stages in the infant's separation from the mother. In Handel, G. and Whitchurch, G. G. (eds.), The psychosocial interior of the family (4th edn, pp. 419–448). Hawthorne, US: Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Malatesta, C. Z., and Izard, C. E. (1984). The ontogenesis of human social signals: From biological imperative to symbol utilization. In Fox, N. A. and Davidson, R. J. (eds.), The Psychobiology of Affective Development (pp. 161–206). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Mandler, J. M. (1992). How to build a baby: Conceptual primitives. Psychological Review, 99, 587–604.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maratos, O. (1973). The origin and development of imitation in the first six months of life. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Geneva.
Markova, G. (Winter, 2004). PhD student participant in Graduate seminar, “Development of Affect, Consciousness and Social Cognition,” (M. Legerstee). York University.
Markova, G., and Legerstee, M. (in press). Contingency, imitation or affect sharing? Foundations of infant's social awareness, Developmental Psychology.
Markova, G., and Legerstee, M.(2004). Foundations of infant's social awareness. Paper presented at the Conference on Developmental relations between Executive Functioning, Social Understanding, and Social Interaction, September, Vancouver, BC).
Markus, J., Mundy, P., Morales, M., Delgado, C. E. F., and Yale, M. (2000). Individual differences in infant skills as predictors of child–caregiver joint attention and language. Social Development, 9, 302–315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maurer, D., and Salapatek, P. (1976). Developmental changes in the scanning of faces by young infants. Child Development, 47, 523–527.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCall, R., and Kagan, J. (1970). Fixation time and tempo of play in infants. Developmental Psychology, 3 (3, pt. 1).Google Scholar
McGurk, H. and McDonald, J. (1976). Hearing lips and seeing voices. Nature, 264, 746–748.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Fradley, E., and Tuckey, M. (2001). Rethinking maternal sensitivity: Mothers' comments on infants' mental processes predict security attachment at 12 months. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Sciences, 42, 637–648.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Wainwright, R., Clark-Carter, D., Gupta, M. D., Fradley, E., and Tuckey, M. (2002). Maternal mind-mindedness and attachment security as predictors of theory of mind understanding. Child Development, 73, 1715–1726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Wainwright, R., Clark-Carter, D., Gupta, M. D., Fradley, E., and Tuckey, M.(2003). Pathways to understanding mind: Construct validity and predictive validity of maternal mind-mindedness. Child Development, 74, 1194–1211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A. N. (1985). Immediate and deferred imitation in fourteen- and twenty-four-month-old infants. Child Development, 56, 62–72.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N.(1988). Infant imitation and memory: Nine-month-olds in immediate and deferred tests. Child Development, 59, 217–225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A. N.(1990). Foundations for developing a concept of self: The role of imitation in relating self to other and the value of social mirroring, social modelling, and self practice in infancy. In Cicchetti, D. and Beeghly, M. (eds.), The self in transition: Infancy to childhood (pp. 139–164). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N.(1995). Understanding the intentions of others: Reenactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology, 31, 838–850.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N., and Borton, R. (1979). Intermodal matching of human neonates. Nature, 282, 403–404.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A. N., and Brooks, R. (2001). “Like me” as a building block for understanding other minds: Bodily acts, attention, and intention. In Malle, B. F. and Bertram, L. J. (eds.), Intentions and intentionality: Foundations of social cognition (pp. 171–195). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N., and Gopnik, A. (1993). The role of imitation in understanding persons and developing a theory of mind. In Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., and Cohen, D. J. (eds.), Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from Autism (pp. 335–365). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N., and Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures of human neonates. Science, 198, 75–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A. N., and Moore, M. K.(1983). The origins of imitation in infancy: Paradigm, phenomena, and theories. In Lipsitt, L. P. and Rovee-Collier, C. (eds.), Advances in infancy research, 2. Norwood: NJ, Ablex.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N., and Moore, M. K.(1997). Explaining facial imitation: A theoretical model. Early Development and Parenting, 6, 179–192.3.0.CO;2-R>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A. N., and Moore, M. K.(1999). Persons and representation: Why infant imitation is important for theories of human development. In Nadel, J. and Butterworth, G. (eds.), Imitation in infancy (pp. 9–35). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Merleau-Ponty, M. (1942/1963). The structure of behavior. Trans. Fisher, A. L.. USA: Beacon Press.Google Scholar
Messer, D. (1997). Referential communication: Making sense of the social and physical worlds. In Bremner, G., Slater, A., and Butterworth, G. (eds.), Infant development: Recent advances (pp. 291–309). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Messer, S. B., Kagan, J., and McCall, R. B. (1970). Fixation time and tempo of play in infants. Developmental Psychology, 3, 406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Millar, W. S. (1972). A study of operant conditioning under delayed reinforcements in early infancy. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 37, 1–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moerk, E. L. (1989). The fuzzy set called “imitation.” In Speidel, G. E. and Nelson, K. E. (eds.), The many faces of imitation in language learning (pp. 277–303). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Molina, M., Van de Walle, G. A., Condry, K., and Spelke, E. S. (in press). The animate/inanimate distinction in infancy: Developing sensitivity to constraints on human actions. Journal of Cognition and Development.
Montague, D. P. F., and Walker-Andrews, A. S. (2001). Peekaboo: A new look at infants' perception of emotion expression. Developmental Psychology, 37, 826–838.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, C. (1996). Theories of mind in infancy. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 14, 19–40. In C. Moore, and Dunham, P. J. (eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 251–271). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Moore, C., and Corkum, V. (1994). Social understanding at the end of the first year of life. Developmental Review, 14, 349–372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, C., and D' Entremont, B. (2001). Developmental changes in pointing as a function of attentional focus. Journal of Cognition and Development, 2, 109–129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, D. G., Hobson, R. P., Lee, A., and Anderson, M. (1992, September). IQ-independent person perception: Evidence from developmental psychopathology. Paper presented at the meeting of the 5th European Conference on Developmental Psychology, Seville, Spain.
Morrissette, P., Ricard, M., and Decarie, T. G. (1995). Joint visual attention and pointing in infancy: A longitudinal study of comprehension. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13, 163–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moses, L. J. (1993). Young children's understanding of belief constraints on intention. Cognitive Development, 8, 1–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moses, L. J., Baldwin, D. A., Rosicky, J. G., and Tidball, G. (2001). Evidence for referential understanding in the emotions domain at twelve and eighteen months. Child Development, 72, 718–735.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moses, L. J., and Flavell, J. H. (1990). Inferring false beliefs from actions and reactions. Child Development, 61, 929–945.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muir, D., Cao, Y., and Entremont, B. (1994, June). Infant social perception revealed during adult face-to-face interaction. Infant Behavior and Development, 17, 86.Google Scholar
Muir, D., and Hains, S. (1999). Young infants' perception of adult intentionality: Adult contingency and eye direction. In Rochat, P. (ed.), Early social cognition: Understanding others in the first months of life (pp. 155–187). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Munakata, Y. (2000). Challenges to the violation-of-expectation paradigm: Throwing the conceptual baby out with the perceptual processing bathwater?Infancy, 1, 471–477.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mundy, P., and Sigman, M. (1989). The theoretical implications of joint-attention deficits in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 173–183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murphy, C. M., and Messer, D. J. (1977). Mothers, infants, and pointing: A study of gesture. In Schaffer, H. R. (ed.), Studies in mother–infant interaction (pp. 325–354). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Murray, L., Fior-Cowley, A., Hooper, R., and Cooper, P. (1996). The impact of postnatal depression and associated adversity on early mother–infant interactions and later infant outcomes. Child Development, 67, 2512–2526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murray, L., and Trevarthen, C. (1985). Emotional regulations of interactions between two-month-olds and their mothers. In Field, T. M. and Fox, N. (eds.), Social perception in infants. New Jersey: Ablex.Google Scholar
Nadel, J., Carchon, I., Kervella, C., Marcelli, D., and Reserbat-Plantey, D. (1999). Expectancies for social contingency in 2-month-olds. Developmental Science, 2, 164–173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakano, S. and Kanaya, Y. (1993). The effects of mothers' teasing: Do Japanese infants read their mothers' play intention in teasing?Early Development and Parenting, 2, 7–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neisser, U. (1993). The self perceived. In Neisser, U. (ed.), The perceived self: Ecological and interpersonal sources of self-knowledge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Neisser, U.(1995). Criteria for an ecological self. In Rochat, P. (eds.), The Self in Infancy: Theory and Research (pp. 17–34). Amsterdam, Netherlands: North-Holland/ Elsevier Science Publishers.Google Scholar
Nelson, L. A. (1987). The recognition of facial expression in the first two years of life: Mechanisms of development. Child Development, 58, 889–909.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nicely, P., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., and Bornstein, M. H. (1999). Mothers' attuned responses to infant affect expressivity promote earlier achievement of language milestones. Infant Behavior and Development, 22, 557–568.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicely, P., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., and Grolnick, W. S. (1999). Maternal responses to infant affect: Stability and prediction. Infant Behavior and Development, 22, 103–117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. (1986). Local traditions and cultural transmission. In Smuts, B. B., Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Wrangham, R. W., and Struhsaker, T. T. (eds.), Primate Societies (pp. 462–471).Google Scholar
Papousek, M., Papousek, H., and Symmes, D. (1991). The meanings of melodies in motherese in tone and stress languages. Infant Behavior and Development, 14, 415–440.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pascual-Leone, J., and Johnson, J. (1998). A dialectical constructivist view of representation: The role of mental attention, executives, and symbols. In Sigel, I. E. (ed.), The development of representational thought: Theoretical perspectives (pp. 169–200). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Perry, J. C., and Stern, D. N. (1976). Gaze duration frequency distributions during mother–infant interaction. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 129, 45–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representational mind. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Phillips, A. T., Wellman, H. M., and Spelke, E. (2002). Infant's ability to connect gaze and emotional expression to intentional action. Cognition, 85, 53–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phillips, W., Baron-Cohen, S., and Rutter, M. (1992). The role of eye contact in goal detection: Evidence from normal infants and children with autism or mental handicaps. Development and Psychopathology, 4, 375–383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1929). The child's conception of the world. Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Piaget, J.(1952). Play, dreams and imitation. New York: W. W. Norton and Co. Inc.Google Scholar
Piaget, J.(1954). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Piaget, J.(1981). Intelligence and affectivity: Their relationship during child development. Oxford: Annual Reviews.Google Scholar
Poulin-Dubois, D. (1999). Infants' distinction between animate and inanimate objects: The origins of naïve psychology. In Rochat, P. (ed.), Early social cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J., and Eddy, T. J. (1996). What young chimpanzees know about seeing. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 61, (3, Serial No. 247).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Premack, D. (1990). The infant's theory of self-propelled objects. Cognition, 36, 1–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Premack, D.(1991). The infant's theory of self-propelled objects. In Frye, D. and Moore, C. (eds.), Children's theories of mind: Mental states and social understanding (pp. 39–48). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rader, N., and Stern, J. D. (1982). Visually elicited reaching in neonates. Child Development, 53, 1004–1007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rakison, D. H., and Poulin-Dubois, D. (2000). Developmental origin of the animate/inanimate distinction. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 209–226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramey, C. T., and Finkelstein, N. W. (1978). Contingent stimulation and infant competence. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 3, 89–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raver, C. C., and Leadbeater, B. J. (1995). Factors influencing joint attention between socioeconomically disadvantaged adolescent mothers and their infants. In Moore, C. and Dunham, P. (eds.), Joint attention: Its origin and role in development (pp. 251–271). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Reddy, V. (1991). Playing with others' expectations: Teaching and mucking about in the first year. In Whitten, A. (ed.), Natural theories of mind: Evolution, development and simulation of everyday mindreading. Cambridge, MA: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Reddy, V.(1999). Prelinguistic communication. In Barrett, M. (ed.), The development of language. Studies in developmental psychology (pp. 25–50). Philadelphia: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Reddy, V.(2003). On being the object of attention: Implications for self–other consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 397–402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reddy, V., Hay, D., Murray, L., and Trevarthen, C. (1997). Communication in infancy: Mutual regulation of affect and attention. In Bremner, G., Slater, A., and Butterworth, G. (eds.), Infant development: Recent advances (pp. 247–273). Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Reed, E. S. (1995). Becoming a self. In Rochat, P. (ed.), The self in infancy: Theory and research. Amsterdam, Netherlands: North-Holland/Elsevier Science Publishers.Google Scholar
Repacholi, B. M. (1998). Infants' use of attentional cues to identify the referent of another person's emotional expression. Developmental Psychology, 34, 1017–1025.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Repacholi, B. M., and Gopnik, A. (1997). Early reasoning about desires: Evidence from 14– and 18-month-olds. Developmental Psychology, 33, 12–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rheingold, H. L. (1961). The effect of environmental stimulation upon social and exploratory behaviour in the human infant. In Foss, B. M. (ed.), Determinants of infant behavior (pp. 143–171). Oxford, England: Wiley.Google Scholar
Ribble, M. (1944). Infant experience in relation to personality development. In Hunt, M. V. (ed.), Personality and the Behavior Disorders (pp. 621–651). Ronald Press.Google Scholar
Rogers, S. J., and Pennington, B. F. (1991). A theoretical approach to the deficits in infantile autism. Development and Psychopathology, 3, 137–162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothbart, M. K. (1981). IBQ. The Infant Behavior Questionnaire: Measurement of temperament in infancy. Child Development, 52, 569–578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rovee-Collier, C. K., and Fagan, J. W. (1981). The retrieval of memory in early infancy. Advances in Infancy Research, 1, 225–254.Google Scholar
Ruddy, M. G., and Bornstein, M. H. (1982). Cognitive correlates of infant attention and maternal stimulation over the first year of life. Child Development, 53, 183–188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M. (1971). Parent–child separation: Psychological effects on the children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 12, 233–260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rutter, M.(1972). Maternal deprivation reassessed. Harmondsworth: Penguin.Google Scholar
Salapatek, P., and Kessen, W. (1966). Visual scanning of triangles by the human newborn. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 3, 155–167.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saxon, T. F., Frick, J. E., and Colombo, J. (1997). A longitudinal study of maternal interactional styles and infant visual attention. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 43, 48–66.Google Scholar
Scaife, M., and Bruner, J. S. (1975). The capacity for joint visual attention in the infant. Nature, 253, 265–266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schaffer, H. R. (1971). The Growth of Sociability. Harmondsworth: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Schaffer, H. R.(1984). The child's entry into a social world. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Schaffer, H. R., Collins, G. M., and Parsons, G. (1977). Vocal interchange and visual regard in verbal and preverbal children. In Schaffer, H. R. (ed.), Studies in mother–infant interaction (pp. 291–324). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Schmuckler, M. (1981). Visual habituation programme. Ontario: University of Toronto.Google Scholar
Schore, A. N. (2000). Emotion, development, and self-organization: Dynamic systems approaches to emotional development. In Lewis, M. D. and Granic, I. (eds.), Neurobiological perspectives: The self-organization of the right brain and the neurobiology of emotional development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Schultz, S., and Wellman, H. M. (1997). Explaining human movements and actions: Children's understanding of the limits of psychological explanation. Cognition, 62, 291–324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1983). Intentionality: An essay in the philosophy of mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seifer, R., Schiller, M., Sameroff, A. J., Resnick, S., and Riordan, K. (1996). Attachment, maternal sensitivity, and infant temperament during the first year of life. Developmental Psychology, 32, 12–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Helplessness: On depression, development and death. San Francisco: Freeman.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1948). Concurrent operants. American Psychologist, 3, 359.Google Scholar
Smith, P. B., and Pederson, D. R. (1988). Maternal sensitivity and patterns of infant–mother attachment. Child Development, 59, 1097–1101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snow, C. (1977). The development of conversation between mothers and babies. Child Language, 4, 1–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Soken, N. H., and Pick, A. D. (1992). Intermodal perception of happy and angry expressive behaviors by seven-month-old infants. Child Development, 63, 787–795.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spelke, E. S. (1981). The infant's acquisition of knowledge of bimodally specified events. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 31, 179–299.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spelke, E. S.(1985). Preferential-looking methods as tools for the study of cognition in infancy. In Gotlieb, G. and Krasnegor, N. A. (eds.), Measurement of audition and vision in the first year of postnatal life: A methodological overview. Westport, US: Ablex Publishing.Google Scholar
Spelke, E. S.(1988). Where perceiving ends and thinking begins: The apprehension of objects in infancy. In Yonas, A. (ed.), Perceptual development in infancy. The Minnesota symposia on child psychology (vol. 20, pp. 197–234). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Spelke, E. S., Breinlinger, K., Macomber, J., and Jacobson, K. (1992). Origins of knowledge. Psychological Review, 99, 605–632.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spelke, E. S., and Cortelyou, A. (1981). Perceptual aspects of social knowing: Looking and listening in infancy. In Lamb, M. E. and Sherrod, L. R. (eds.), Infant social cognition: Empirical and theoretical considerations. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Spelke, E. S., Phillips, A. T., and Woodward, A. L. (1995). Infants, knowledge of object motion and human action. In Sperber, D., Premack, D., and Premack, A. J. (eds.), Causal cognition: A multi-disciplinary debate (pp. 44–78). Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Sperber, D., Premack, D., and Premack, A. J. (1995). Causal cognition: A multi-disciplinary debate. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Spitz, R. (1963). Hospitalism: An inquiry into the genesis of psychiatric conditions in early childhood. Psychoanalytic Study of Child, 1, 53–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stankov, L. (1983). Attention and intelligence. Educational Psychology, 75, 471–490.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stern, D. N. (1977). The first relationship: Infant and mother. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Stern, D. N.(1984). Affect attunement. In Call, J. D., Galenson, E., and Tyson, R. T. (eds.), Frontiers of infant psychiatry (Vol. II, pp. 3–14). New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Stern, D. N.(1985). The interpersonal world of the infant. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Stern, D. N.(1995). Self/other distinction in the domain of intimate socio-affective interaction: Some considerations. In Rochat, P. (ed.), The self in infancy: Theory and research. Advances in psychology. Amsterdam: North Holland-Elsevier.Google Scholar
Sullivan, J. W., and Horowitz, F. D. (1983). Infant intermodal perception and maternal multimodal stimulation: Implications for language development. In Lipsitt, L. P. and Rovee-Collier, C. K. (eds.), Advances in infancy research (Vol. 2). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Summerfeld, Q. (1979). Use of visual information for phonetic perception. Phonetica, 36, 314–331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Szajnberg, N. M., Skrinjaric, J., and Moore, A. (1989). Affect attunement, attachment, temperament and zygosity: A twin study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 249–253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tager-Flusberg, H. (1989). An analysis of discourse ability and internal state lexicons in a longitudinal study of autistic children. Paper presented at Biennial Meeting of Society for Research in Child Development, Kansas City.
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., and Bornstein, M. H. (1989). Habituation and maternal encouragement of attention in infancy as predictors of toddler language, play, and representational competence. Child Development, 60, 738–751.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bornstein, M. H., and Baumwell, L. (2001). Maternal responsiveness and children's achievement of language milestones. Child Development, 72, 748–767.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tarabulsy, G. M., Tessier, R., and Kappas, A. (1996). Contingency detection and the contingent organization of behavior in interactions: Implications for socioemotional development in infancy. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 25–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teo, T. (1997). Developmental psychology and the relevance of a critical metatheoretical reflection. Human Development, 40, 195–210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tizard, B., and Rees, J. (1974). A comparison of the effects of adoption, restoration, to the natural mother, and continued institutionalization of the cognitive development of four-year-old children. Child Development, 45, 92–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In Moore, C. and Dunham, P. (eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 103–130). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M.(1996). Do apes ape? In Heyes, C. M. and Galef, B. G. (eds.), Social learning in animals: The roots of culture (pp. 319–346). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M.(1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M.(2000). Culture and cognitive development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 37–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M.(January, 2003), personal communication.
Tomasello, M., and Call, J. (1997). Primate cognition. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., and Camaioni, L. (1997). A comparison of the gestural communication of apes and human infants. Human Development, 40, 7–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., and Farrar, M. J. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development, 57, 1454–1463.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C., and Ratner, H. H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 495–552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trehub, S. (1976). The discrimination of foreign speech contrasts by infants and adults. Child Development, 47, 466–472.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in early infancy. A description of primary intersubjectivity. In Bullowa, M. (ed.), Before speech: The beginning of human communication (pp. 321–347). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C.(1991). The self born in intersubjectivity: The psychology of an infant communicating. In Neisser, U. (ed.), The perceived self: Ecological and interpersonal sources of self-knowledge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C., and Hubley, P. (1978). Secondary intersubjectivity: Confidence, confiding and acts of meaning in the second year. In Lock, A. (ed.), Action, gesture and symbol: The emergence of language (pp. 183–229). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Tronick, E. Z. (1989). Emotions and emotional communication in infants. American Psychologist, 44, 112–119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tronick, E. Z.(2002). A model of infant mood states: Long lasting organizing affective states and emotional representational processes without language or symbols. Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 12, 73–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tronick, E. Z.(2003). Things still to be done on the still-face effect. Infancy, 4, 475–482.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tronick, E. Z.(2004). Selective coherence and dyadic states of consciousness. In Nadel, J. and Muir, D. (eds.), Developmental studies of emotions (pp. 293–315). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valsiner, J. (1997). Culture and the development of children's action. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Boom, D. C. (1994). The influence of temperament and mothering on attachment and exploration: An experimental manipulation of sensitive responsiveness among lower-class mothers with irritable infants. Child Development, 65, 1457–1477.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vecera, S. P., and Johnson, M. H. (1995). Gaze detection and the cortical processing of faces: Evidence from infants and adults. Visual Cognition, 2, 59–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vedeler, D. (1994). Infant intentionality as object-directedness: A method for observation. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 35, 343–366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verschueren, K., Marcoen, A., and Schoef, V. (1996). The internal working model of the self, attachment, and competence in five year olds. Child Development, 67, 2493–2511.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofsten, C. (1982). Eye–hand coordination in newborns. Developmental Psychology, 18, 450–461.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. (Edited and translated by Haufmann, Eugenia and Vakar, Gertrude.) Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S.(1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. In Cole, M., John-Steiner, V., Criner, S., and Souberman, E. (eds.), Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Walden, T. A., and Ogan, T. A. (1988). The development of social referencing. Child Development, 59, 1230–1240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walker-Andrews, A. S. (1997). Infants' perception of expressive behaviors: Differentiation of multimodal information. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 437–456.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, J. B. (1928). Psychological care of infant and child. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Watson, J. S. (1972). Smiling, cooing, and “the game.”Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 18, 323–339.Google Scholar
Watson, J. S.(1979). Perception of contingency as a determinant of social responsiveness. In Thoman, E. B. (ed.), The origins of the infant's social responsiveness (pp. 33–64). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Watson, J. S.(1985). Contingency perception in early social development. In Field, T. M. and Fox, N. A. (eds.), Social perception (pp. 157–176). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing.Google Scholar
Watson, J. S., and Ramey, C. T. (1972). Reactions to response-contingent stimulation in early infancy. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 18, 219–227.Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M. (1990). The child's theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M.(1993). Early understanding of mind: The normal case. In Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., and Cohen, D. J. (eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism (pp. 10–39). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M., and Banerjee, M. (1991). Mind and emotion: Children's understanding of the emotional consequences of beliefs and desires. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 191–224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wellman, H. M., and Estes, D. (1986). Early understanding of mental entities: A reexamination of childhood realism. Child Development, 57, 910–923.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wellman, H. M., Harris, P. L., Banerjee, M., and Sinclair, A. (1995). Early understanding of emotion. Evidence from natural language. Cognition and Emotion, 9, 117–149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wellman, H. M., and Woolley, J. P. (1990). From simple desires to ordinary beliefs: The early development of everyday psychology. Cognition, 35, 245–275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Werker, J. F., and McLeod, P. J. (1989). Infant preference for both male and female infant-directed talk: A developmental study of attentional and affective responsiveness. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 43, 230–246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wimmer, H., and Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103–128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolff, P. H. (1966). The causes, controls, and organization of behavior in the neonate. Psychological Issue, 5 (Monograph 17).Google ScholarPubMed
Wolff, P. H.(1987). The development of behavioral states and the expression of emotions in early infancy: New proposals for investigation. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Woodward, A. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach. Cognition, 69, 1–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yonas, A., Petterson, L, and Lockman, J. J. (1979). Young infants' sensitivity to optical information for collision. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 33, 268–276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zeedyk, S. M. (1996). Developmental accounts of intentionality: Toward integration. Developmental Review, 16, 416–461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • References
  • Maria Legerstee, York University, Toronto
  • Book: Infants' Sense of People
  • Online publication: 22 September 2009
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511489747.012
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • References
  • Maria Legerstee, York University, Toronto
  • Book: Infants' Sense of People
  • Online publication: 22 September 2009
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511489747.012
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • References
  • Maria Legerstee, York University, Toronto
  • Book: Infants' Sense of People
  • Online publication: 22 September 2009
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511489747.012
Available formats
×