Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-wzw2p Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-08T09:52:49.940Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

8 - The Psychology of the Trial Judge

from II - Ontology and Epistemology

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 April 2021

Bartosz Brożek
Affiliation:
Jagiellonian University, Krakow
Jaap Hage
Affiliation:
Universiteit Maastricht, Netherlands
Nicole Vincent
Affiliation:
Macquarie University, Sydney
Get access

Summary

The chapter on the psychology of the trial consists of three sections. The first section examines the question of whether the general insights of psychology and behavioral economics can be applied to trial judges, or whether, by virtue of their special training and experience or some other reason, trial judges are different. The second section surveys the relatively scant behavioral literature on judging and places it in the larger context of non-judge decision making. It does so by considering four well-studied “heuristics,” or cognitive shortcuts that allow people to make quick, intuitive decisions with little or no deliberation, but that can sometimes also result in errors in reasoning. The four heuristics considered are: anchoring, hindsight, compromise and contrast, and representativeness. The third section finishes the chapter with a series of reflections about specific challenges the author has thought about over the twenty-nine years that he has been a trial judge, coupled with a few suggestions about how the system might better accommodate some of those challenges.

Type
Chapter
Information
Law and Mind
A Survey of Law and the Cognitive Sciences
, pp. 165 - 192
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2021

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abramowicz, M. (2001). A Compromise Approach to Compromise Verdicts. California Law Review 89(2), 231314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aharoni, E., & Hoffman, M. (in press). Evolutionary Psychology, Jurisprudence, and Sentencing. In Shackelford, T. (ed.), Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications.Google Scholar
Albanese, J. S. (1984). Concern about Variation in Criminal Sentences: a Cyclical History of Reform. Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology 75, 260–271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arkes, H. R., Faust, D., Guilmette, T. J., & Hart, K. (1988). Eliminating the Hindsight Bias. Journal of Applied Psychology 72(2), 305307.Google Scholar
Bar-Hillel, M. (1980). The Base-Rate Fallacy in Probability Judgments. Acta Psychologica 44, 211233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bennett, M. (2014). Confronting Cognitive “Anchoring Effect” and “Blind Spot Bias” in Federal Sentencing: a Modest Solution for Reforming a Fundamental Flaw. Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology 104(3), 489534.Google Scholar
Bennett, M., & Earwaker, D. (2010). Victims’ Responses to Apologies: the Effects of Offender Responsibility and Offense Severity. Journal of Social Psychology 134(4), 457464.Google Scholar
Blumstein, A. (1982). On the Racial Disproportionality of United States’ Prison Population. Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology 73, 12591281.Google Scholar
Bornstein, B. H., Rung, L. M., & Miller, M. K. (2002). The Effects of Defendant Remorse on Mock Juror Decisions in a Malpractice Case. Behavioral Sciences and the Law 20(4), 393409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bouton, L., Llorente-Saguer, A., & Malherbe, F. (2018). Get Rid of Unanimity Rule: the Superiority of Majority Rules with Veto Power. Journal of Political Economy 126(1), 107149.Google Scholar
Boyd, C. L. (2016). Representation on the Courts? The Effects of Trial Judges’ Sex and Race. Political Research Quarterly 69(4), 788799.Google Scholar
Brillon, Y. (1983). Fear of Crime an Punitive Attitudes Among the Elderly. Criminologie 36(1), 729.Google Scholar
Buckholtz, J. W., Asplund, C. L., Dux, P. E., et al. (2008). The Neural Correlates of Third-Party Punishment. Neuron 60, 930940.Google Scholar
Buckholtz, J. W., Martin, J. W., Treadway, M. T., et al. (2015). From Blame to Punishment: Disrupting Prefrontal Cortex Activity Reveals Norm Enforcement Mechanisms. Neuron 87(6), 13691380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chapman, G. B., & Bornstein, B. H. (1996). The More You Ask the More You Get: Anchoring in Personal Injury Verdicts. Applied Cognitive Psychology 10(6), 519540.3.0.CO;2-5>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chapman, G. B., & Johnson, E. J. (2002). Incorporating the Irrelevant: Anchors in Judgments of Belief and Value. In Gilovich, T., Griffin, D., & Kahneman, D. (eds.), Heuristics and Biases: the Psychology of Intuitive Judgment. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 120138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, D. L., Moskowitz, T. J. & Shue, K. (2016). Decision Making Under the Gambler’s Fallacy: Evidence from Asylum Judges, Loan Officers, and Baseball Umpires. The Quarterly Journal of Economics 131(3), 11811242.Google Scholar
Chew, P., & Kelley, R. (2009). The Myth of the Color-Blind Judge: an Empirical Analysis of Racial Harassment Cases. Washington University Law Review 86, pp. 11171166.Google Scholar
Chien, Y. L., Huang, C. J., & Shaw, D. (2005). A General Model of Starting Point Bias in Double-Bounded Dichotomous Contingent Valuation Surveys. Journal of Environmental Economics and Management 50(2), 362377.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Desantts, A., & Kayson, W. A. (1997). Defendants’ Characteristics of Attractiveness, Race, and Sex and Sentencing Decisions. Psychological Reports 81(2), 679683.Google Scholar
Diamond, S. S., Rose, M. R., Murphy, B., & Meixner, J. (2011). Damage Anchors on Real Juries. Journal of Empirical Legal Studies 8(1s), 148178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dietrich, D., & Olson, M. (1993). A Demonstration of Hindsight Bias Using the Thomas Confirmation Vote. Psychological Reports 72(2), 377378.Google Scholar
Englich, B., Mussweiler, T., & Strack, F. (2006). Playing Dice With Criminal Sentences: the Influence of Irrelevant Anchors on Experts’ Judicial Decision Making. Society for Personality and Social Psychology 32, 188200.Google Scholar
Epstein, L., Landes, W. M., & Posner, R. A. (2013). The Behavior of Federal Judges: A Theoretical and Empirical Study. Cambridge, MA & London: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Faigman, D. L., Monahan, J., & Slobogin, C. (2014). Group to Individual (G2i) Inference in Scientific Expert Testimony. University of Chicago Law Review 81(2), 417480.Google Scholar
Feldman, G., Chandrashekar, S. P., & Wong, K. F. E. (2016). The Freedom to Excel: Belief in Free Will Predicts Better Academic Performance. Personality and Individual Differences 90, 377383.Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B. (1975). Hindsight Is Not Equal to Foresight: the Effect of Outcome Knowledge on Judgment Under Uncertainty. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 1(3), 288299.Google Scholar
Gault, B. A., & Sabini, J. (2000). The Roles of Empathy, Anger, and Gender in Predicting Attitudes Toward Punitive, Reparative, and Preventative Public Policies. Cognition and Emotion 14(4), 495–-520.Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G., & Todd, P. M. (1999). Simple Heuristics that Make Us Smart. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gruhl, J., Spohn, C., & Welch, S. (1981). Women as Policymakers: the Case of Trial Judges. American Journal of Political Science 25(2), 308322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guthrie, C., Rachlinski, J. J., & Wistrich, A. J. (2001). Inside the Judicial Mind. Cornell Law Review, 86, 777830.Google Scholar
Hell, W., Gigerenzer, G., Gauggel, S., Mall, M., & Müller, M. (1988). Hindsight Bias: an Interaction of Automatic and Motivational Factors? Memory and Cognition 16(6), 553–538.Google Scholar
Hertwig, R., & Gigerenzer, G. (1999). The “Conjunction Fallacy” Revisited: How Intelligent Inferences Look Like Reasoning Errors. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 12(4), 275305.Google Scholar
Hoffman, M. B. (2007). The Myth of Factual Innocence. Chicago-Kent Law Review 82(2), 663690.Google Scholar
Hoffman, M. B. (2014). The Punisher’s Brain: the Evolution of Judge and Jury. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoffman, M. B., & Goldsmith, T. H. (2004). The Biological Roots of Punishment. Ohio State Journal of Criminal Law 1(2), 627641.Google Scholar
Hoffman, M., Shen, F., Iyengar, V., & Krueger, F. (2020). The Intersectionality of Age and Gender on the Bench: Are Younger Female Judges Harsher with Serious Crimes? The Columbia Journal of Law and Gender 40(1), 128–165.Google Scholar
Johnson, B. (2006). The Multilevel Context of Criminal Sentencing: Integrating Judge- and County-Level Influences. Criminology 44(2), 259298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1972). Subjective Probability: a Judgment of Representativeness. Cognitive Psychology 3(3), 430454.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1973). On the Psychology of Prediction. Psychological Review 80(4), 237251.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1974). Judgments Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. Science 184(4157), 11241131.Google Scholar
Kamin, K., & Rachlinski, J. J. (1995). Ex Post ≠ Ex Ante. Law and Human Behavior 19(1), 89104.Google Scholar
Kelman, M., Rottenstreich, Y., & Tversky, A. (1996). Context-Dependence in Legal Decision Making. Journal of Legal Studies 25, 287318.Google Scholar
Keren, G., & Wu, G. (2015). A Bird’s-Eye View of the History of Judgment and Decision-Making. In Keren, G. & Wu, G. (eds.) The Wiley Blackwell Handbook of Judgment and Decision Making. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons, pp. 140.Google Scholar
Kerr, N. L., & Tindale, R. S. (2004). Group Performance and Decision Making. Annual Review of Psychology 55, pp. 623655.Google Scholar
Klein, D. E., & Mitchell, G. (2010). The Psychology of Judicial Decision-Making. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kritzer, H. M., & Uhlman, T. M. (1977). Sisterhood in the Courtroom: Sex and Judge and Defendant in Criminal Case Disposition. Social Science Journal 14, 366379.Google Scholar
Krueger, F., & Hoffman, M. (2016). The Emerging Neuroscience of Third-Party Punishment. Trends in Neurosciences 39(6), 499501.Google Scholar
Krueger, F., Hoffman, M., Walter, H., & Grafman, J. (2014). An fMRI Investigation of the Effects of Belief in Free Will on Third-Party Punishment. Social, Cognitive, and Affective Neuroscience 9(8), 11431149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landsman, S., & Rakos, R. F. (1994). A Preliminary Inquiry Into the Effect of Potentially Biasing Information on Judges and Jurors in Civil Litigation. Behavioral Sciences 12(2), 113126.Google Scholar
Leibovitch, A. (2016). Relative Judgments. Journal of Legal Studies 45(2), 281330.Google Scholar
MacCoun, R. J. (1984). Experimental Research on Jury Decision-Making. Science 244, 10461050.Google Scholar
McCoy, C. (2005). The Trial Penalty and Plea Bargaining Reform. Criminal Law Quarterly 50, 67107.Google Scholar
McCullough, M., & van Oyen Witvliet, C. (2001). The Psychology of Forgiveness. In Snyder, C. R. & Lopez, Shane J. (eds.), The Handbook of Positive Psychology. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 446458.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGregor, I., & Holmes, J. G. (1999). How Storytelling Shapes Memory and Impressions of Relationship Events Over Time. Journal of Personal and Social Psychology 76(3), 403419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mustard, D. B. (2001). Racial, Ethnic, and Gender Disparities in Sentencing: Evidence from the US Federal Courts. Journal of Law and Economics 44, 285314.Google Scholar
Nadelhoffer, T., Shepard, J., Nahmias, E., Sripada, C., & Thompson-Ross, L. (2014). The Free Will Inventory: Measuring Beliefs About Agency and Responsibility. Consciousness and Cognition 25, 2741.Google Scholar
Nettler, G. (1959). Cruelty, Dignity, and Determinism. American Sociological Review 24, 375384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paulus, D. L., & Carey, J. M. (2011). The FAD-PLUS: Measuring Lay Beliefs Regarding Free Will and Related Constructs. Journal of Personality Assessment 93(1), 96104.Google Scholar
Pennington, N., & Hastie, R. (1991). A Cognitive Theory of Juror Decision Making: the Story Model. Cardozo Law Review 13, 519557.Google Scholar
Peter-Hagene, L. C., & Salerno, J. M. (2019). Jury Decision Making. In Brewer, N. & Bradfield-Douglass, A. (eds.), Psychological Science and the Law. New York: Guilford, pp. 338366.Google Scholar
Pietrantoni, G. (2017). Jury Deliberation. The Review: a Journal of Undergraduate Student Research 18. https://fisherpub.sjfc.edu/ur/vol18/iss1/7Google Scholar
Pizzi, W. T., Blair, I. V., & Judd, C. M. (2004). Discrimination in Sentencing on the Basis of Afrocentric Features. Michigan Journal of Race and Law 10, 327354.Google Scholar
Posner, R. A. (1993). What Do Judges and Justices Maximize? (the Same Thing Everybody Else Does). Supreme Court Economic Review 3, 141.Google Scholar
Posner, R. A. (1998). Rational Choice, Behavioral Economics, and the Law. Stanford Law Review 50, 15511575.Google Scholar
Rachlinski, J. J., Guthrie, C., & Wistrich, A. J. (2007). Heuristics and Biases in Bankruptcy Judges. Journal of Institutional and Theoretical Economics 163, 167186.Google Scholar
Rachlinski, J. J., Guthrie, C., & Wistrich, A. J. (2011). Probable Cause, Probability, and Hindsight. Journal of Empirical Legal Studies 8, 7298.Google Scholar
Rachlinski, J. J., Johnson, S., Wistrich, A., & Guthrie, C. (2009). Does Unconscious Racial Bias Affect Trial Judges? Notre Dame Law Review 84, 11951246.Google Scholar
Rachlinski, J. J., & Jourdan, F. (2003). The Cognitive Components of Punishment. Cornell Law Review 88, 457485.Google Scholar
Robbennolt, J. K. (1999). Anchoring in the Courtroom: the Effects of Caps on Punitive Damages. Law and Human Behavior 23, 353373.Google Scholar
Robbennolt, J. K. (2014). Litigation and Settlement. In Zamir, E. & Teichman, D. (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Behavioral Economics and the Law. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 623642.Google Scholar
Robinson, P. H., & Kurzban, R. (2007). Concordance and Conflict in Intuitions of Justice. Minnesota Law Review 91, 18211907.Google Scholar
Roese, N. J., & Vohs, K. D. (2012). Hindsight Bias. Perspectives on Psychological Science 7(5), 411426.Google Scholar
Saks, M., Hollinger, L., Wissler, Evans D., & Hart, A. (1997). Reducing Variability in Civil Jury Awards. Law and Human Behavior 21(3), 243256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schauer, F., & Spellman, B. (2017). Analogy, Expertise, and Experience. University of Chicago Law Review 84, 249268.Google Scholar
Shen, F. X., Hoffman, M. B., Jones, O. D., Greene, J. D., & Marois, R. (2011). Sorting Guilty Minds. New York University Law Review 86, 13061360.Google Scholar
Simon, D. (2004). A Third View of the Black Box: Cognitive Coherence in Legal Decision Making. University of Chicago Law Review 71, 511–586.Google Scholar
Simonson, I., & Tversky, A. (1992). Choice in Context: Tradeoff Contrast and Extremeness Aversion. Journal of Marketing Research 29(3), 281295.Google Scholar
Slovic, P., Finucane, M., Peters, E., & MacGregor, D. (2002). Rational Actors or Rational Fools: Implications of the Affect Heuristic for Behavioral Economics. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics 31(4), 329342.Google Scholar
Smith, E. D., & Hed, A. (1979). Effects of Offenders’ Age and Attractiveness on Sentencing by Mock Jurors. Psychological Reports 44(3), 691694.Google Scholar
Spohn, C. (1990). The Sentencing Decisions of Black and White Judges: Expected and Unexpected Similarities. Law and Society Review 24(5), 11971216.Google Scholar
Steffensmeier, D., & Britt, C. L. (2001). Judges’ Race and Judicial Decision Making: Do Black Judges Sentence Differently? Social Science Quarterly 82(4), 749764.Google Scholar
Stein, C. T., & Drouin, M. (2018). Cognitive Bias in the Courtroom: Combating the Anchoring Effect Through Tactical Debiasing. University of San Francisco Law Review 52, 393428.Google Scholar
Stillman, T. F., Baumeister, R. F., & Vohs, K. D. (2010). Personal Philosophy and Personal Achievement: Belief in Free Will Predicts Better Job Performance. Social Psychological & Personality Science 1, 4350.Google Scholar
Teichman, D., & Zamir, E. (2014). Judicial Decision-Making: a Behavioral Perspective. In Zamir, E. & Teichman, D. (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Behavioral Economics and the Law. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 664702.Google Scholar
Thompson, W. C., & Schumann, E. L. (1987). Interpretation of Statistical Evidence in Criminal Trials. Law and Human Behavior 11(3), 167187.Google Scholar
Tierney, J. (1991). Behind Monty Hall’s Doors: Puzzle, Debate, and Answer? The New York Times, July 21, 1991.Google Scholar
Uhlman, T. M. (1978). Black Elite Decision Making: the Case of Trial Judges. American Journal of Political Science 22(4), 884895.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
United Nations (1974). Growth in United Nations Membership 1945–Present. www.un.org/en/sections/member-states/growth-united-nations-membership-1945-present/index.htmlGoogle Scholar
United States Sentencing Guidelines (2019), §3E1.1 (Acceptance of Responsibility).Google Scholar
Vars, F. E. (2014). Evidence Law. In Zamir, E. & Teichman, D. (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Behavioral Economics and the Law. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 703718.Google Scholar
Vidmar, N., & Hans, V. P. (2007). American Juries: the Verdict. Amherst, NY: Prometheus, pp. 158168, 175176, 236240, 260262.Google Scholar
Vincent, C. A., Young, M., & Phillips, A. (1994). Why Do People Sue Doctors? A Study of Patients and Relatives Taking Legal Action. Lancet 343, 16091613.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Viney, W., Waldman, D. A., & Barchilon, J. (1982). Attitudes Toward Punishment in Relation to Beliefs in Free Will and Determinism. Human Relations 35, 939949.Google Scholar
Viney, W., Parker-Martin, P., & Dotten, S. (1988). Belief in Free Will and Determinism and Lack of Relation to Punishment Rationale and Magnitude. Journal of General Psychology 115, pp. 1523.Google Scholar
Vohs, K. D., & Schooler, J. W. (2008). The Value of Believing in Free Will: Encouraging a Belief in Determinism Increases Cheating. Psychological Science 19, 4954.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wexler, D. B., & Schopp, R. F. (1989). How and When to Correct for Juror Hindsight Bias in Mental Health Litigation: Some Preliminary Observations. Behavioral Sciences & the Law 7, 485504.Google Scholar
Wistrich, A. J., Guthrie, C., & Rachlinski, J. J. (2005). Can Judges Ignore Inadmissible Evidence? The Difficulty of Deliberately Disregarding. University of Pennsylvania Law Review 153, 12511345.Google Scholar
Wood, G. (1978). The Knew-It-All-Along Effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 4, 345353.Google Scholar
Wrightsman, L. S. (2010). The Psychology of the Supreme Court. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Zamir, E. (2017). Law and Behavioral Economics. In Sellers, M. & Kirste, S. (eds.), Encyclopedia of the Philosophy of Law and Social Philosophy . New York: Springer, pp. 111.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×