Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-ndmmz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-01T04:16:25.765Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

13 - Functional Load and Vowel Merger in Toronto Heritage Cantonese

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 February 2024

Rajiv Rao
Affiliation:
University of Wisconsin, Madison
Get access

Summary

The functional load hypothesis predicts that phonemes with lower functional load are more likely to merge than higher functional load phonemes. Using Cantonese spontaneous speech data from the Heritage Language Variation and Change Corpus (Nagy, 2011), this chapter addresses the functional load hypothesis in a heritage language context by comparing F1/F2 production patterns of two vowel pairs (i.e., /y/~/u/ and /a/~/ɔ/) across thirty-two speakers based on generational group (i.e., Gen0, Gen1, and Gen2) and dominant language (i.e., Cantonese versus English). An analysis of Pillai score values (Nycz & Hall-Lew, 2015) shows a significant decrease in the phonetic distinctiveness of /y/~/u/ for English-dominant speakers (i.e., all Gen2). The /y/~/u/ pair also has a lower functional load. Dominant language, however, was not a significant Pillai score predictor for /a/~/ɔ/. Overall, the results support the functional load hypothesis in a heritage language context and show how it can complement accounts of heritage languages based on dominant language transfer.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2024

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aalberse, S., Backus, A., & Muysken, P. (2019). Heritage languages: A language contact approach (Vol. 58). John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amengual, M., & Simonet, M. (2020). Language dominance does not always predict cross-linguistic interactions in bilingual speech production. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 10(6), 847872.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amery, H., & Cartwright, S. (1987). The first hundred words. Usborne.Google Scholar
Becker, K. (Ed.). (2019). The low-back-merger shift: Uniting the Canadian vowel shift, the California vowel shift, and short front vowel shifts across North America (Vol. 104). Duke University Press.Google Scholar
Boberg, C. (2008). Regional phonetic differentiation in Standard Canadian English. Journal of English Linguistics, 36(2), 129154. https://doi.org/10.1177/0075424208316648.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2016). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer [Computer program]. Version 6.0.15. www.praat.org/.Google Scholar
Bullock, B. E., & Gerfen, C. (2004). Frenchville French: A case study in phonological attrition. International Journal of Bilingualism, 8(3), 303320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ceolin, A. (2020). Functionalism, lexical contrast and sound change [Doctoral dissertation, University of Pennsylvania]. https://repository.upenn.edu/edissertations/3991.Google Scholar
Chao, Y.-R. (1930), ə sistim əv "toun-letəz" [A system of "tone-letters"]. Le Maître Phonétique, 30, 2427.Google Scholar
DeFrancis, J. (1984). The Chinese language: Fact and fantasy. University of Hawaii Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gorman, K., Howell, J., & Wagner, M. (2011). Prosodylab-aligner: A tool for forced alignment of laboratory speech. Canadian Acoustics, 39(3), 192193.Google Scholar
Hall-Lew, L. (2009). Ethnicity and phonetic variation in a San Francisco neighborhood [Doctoral dissertation, Stanford University].Google Scholar
Hay, J., Warren, P., & Drager, K. (2006). Factors influencing speech perception in the context of a merger-in-progress. Journal of Phonetics, 34(4), 458484.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hockett, C. F. (1967). The quantification of functional load. Word, 23(1–3), 300320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labov, W. (1984). Field methods of the project on linguistic change and variation. In Baugh, J. & Sherzer, J. (Eds.), Language in use: Readings in sociolinguistics (pp. 2853). Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Li, D. C. S., Wong, C. S. P., Leung, W. M., & Wong, S. T. S. (2016). Facilitation of transference: The case of monosyllabic salience in Hong Kong Cantonese. Linguistics, 54(1), 158. https://doi.org/10.1515/ling-2015-0037.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matthews, S., & Yip, V. (2011). Cantonese: A comprehensive grammar (2nd ed.). Routledge.Google Scholar
Meyerhoff, M., & Nagy, N. (2019). Panel Discussion: What’s so standard about standards? New Ways of Analyzing Variation 48, Eugene, OR. https://nwav48.uoregon.edu/.Google Scholar
Nagy, N. (2011). A multilingual corpus to explore variation in language contact situations. Rassegna Italiana Di Linguistica Applicata, 43(1/2), 6584.Google Scholar
Nagy, N., & Meyerhoff, M. (2008). The social life of sociolinguistics. In Meyerhoff, M. & Nagy, N. (Eds.), Social lives in language: Sociolinguistics and multilingual speech communities (pp. 117). John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Nagy, N., Kang, Y., Kochetov, A., & Walker, J. (2009). Heritage languages in Toronto: A new project. Heritage Language Workshop, University of Toronto.Google Scholar
Nycz, J., & Hall-Lew, L. (2015). Best practices in measuring vowel merger. Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics, 20(1), 060008. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4894063.Google Scholar
Polinsky, M. (2018). Heritage languages and their speakers (Vol. 159). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ravindranath, M. (2015). Sociolinguistic variation and language contact. Language and Linguistic Compass, 9(6), 243255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sloetjes, H., & Wittenburg, P. (2008). Annotation by category – ELAN and ISO DCR. Proceedings of the 6th International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation (LREC 2008), 816–820.Google Scholar
Smakman, D., & Heinrich, P. (Eds.). (2015). Globalising sociolinguistics: Challenging and expanding theory. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanford, J., & Preston, D. (Eds.). (2007). Variation in Indigenous minority languages (Vol. 25). John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Surendran, D., & Niyogi, P. (2006). Quantifying the functional load of phonemic oppositions, distinctive features, and suprasegmentals. In Thomsen, O. N. (Ed.), Competing models of linguistic change: Evolution and beyond (pp. 4358). John Benjamins. https://doi.org/10.1075/cilt.279.05sur.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, E., & Kendall, T. (2007). NORM: The vowel normalization and plotting suite. http://lingtools.uoregon.edu/norm/norm1.php.Google Scholar
Tse, H. (2019a). Beyond the monolingual core and out into the wild: A variationist study of early bilingualism and sound change in Toronto heritage Cantonese [Doctoral dissertation, University of Pittsburgh]. http://d-scholarship.pitt.edu/35721/.Google Scholar
Tse, H. (2019b). Vowel shifts in Cantonese? Asia-Pacific Language Variation, 5(1), 6783. https://doi.org/10.1075/aplv.19001.tse.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tse, H. (2022). What can Cantonese heritage speakers tell us about age of acquisition, linguistic dominance, and sociophonetic variation? In Bayley, R., Richard, D.. Preston, , & Li, X. (Eds.), Variation in second and heritage languages: Crosslinguistic perspectives (pp. 97126). John Benjamins. https://doi.org/10.1075/silv.28.05tse.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walker, J. A. (2015). Canadian English: A sociolinguistic perspective. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wedel, A., Kaplan, A., & Jackson, S. (2013). High functional load inhibits phonological contrast loss: A corpus study. Cognition, 128(2), 179186. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.03.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yue-Hashimoto, A. O. (1972). Phonology of Cantonese. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Zee, E. (1999). Chinese (Hong Kong Cantonese). In Handbook of the International Phonetic Association: A guide to the use of the International Phonetic Alphabet (pp. 5860). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×