Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-dfsvx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T01:21:49.522Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

21 - Approach and Dominance as Social Signals for Affective Interfaces

from Part III - Machine Synthesis of Social Signals

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 July 2017

Marc Cavazza
Affiliation:
Tees University
Judee K. Burgoon
Affiliation:
University of Arizona
Nadia Magnenat-Thalmann
Affiliation:
Université de Genève
Maja Pantic
Affiliation:
Imperial College London
Alessandro Vinciarelli
Affiliation:
University of Glasgow
Get access

Summary

Introduction

Recent years have seen a growing interest in the development of affective interfaces. At the heart of these systems is an ability to capture social signals and analyse them in a way that meets the requirements and characteristics of the application. There has been a concerted effort in devising a principled approach which could benefit from the interdisciplinary nature of the affective computing endeavour. One common strategy has been to seek conceptual models of emotion that could mediate between the social signals to be captured and the knowledge content of the application. Early systems have largely relied on the intrinsic, natural properties of emotional communication with, for instance, an emphasis on facial expressions both on the output and the input side, together with some theoretical foundation for the acceptance of computers as interaction partners (Reeves & Nass, 1996). Without downplaying the importance of these systems in the development of the field or the practical interest of the applications they entailed (Prendinger & Ishizuka, 2005), it soon appeared that not all interactions could be modelled after interhuman communication, in particular when considering interaction with more complex applications. This complexity can be described at two principal levels: the interaction with complex data, knowledge, or task elements and the nature of the emotions themselves (and their departing from universal or primitive emotions to reflect more sophisticated ones). On the other hand, part of the problem rests with various simplifications that were necessary to get early prototypes off the ground. A good introduction to the full complexity of an affective response can be found in Sander and Scherer (2014), in particular its description of the various levels and components to be considered. These have not always been transposed into affective computing systems; however, as is often the case, the original description frameworks may not be computational enough to support direct and complete transposition.

Dimensional models of emotions have been enthusiastically adopted in affective interfaces for their flexibility in terms of representation as well as the possibility of mapping input modalities onto their dimensions to provide a consistent representation of input.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2017

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allen, J. J., Harmon-Jones, E., & Cavender, J. H. (2001).Manipulation of frontal EEG asymmetry through biofeedback alters self-reported emotional responses and facial EMG.Psychophysiology, 38(04), 685–693.Google Scholar
Amodio, D. M., Master, S. L., Yee, C. M., & Taylor, S. E. (2008). Neurocognitive components of the behavioral inhibition and activation systems: Implications for theories of self-regulation.Psychophysiology, 45(1), 11–19.Google Scholar
Andreassi, J. (2006). Psychophysiology: Human Behavior and Physiological Response. Hoboken, NJ: Routledge.
Baehr, E., Rosenfeld, J. P., & Baehr, R. (2001). Clinical use of an alpha asymmetry neurofeedback protocol in the treatment of mood disorders: Follow-up study one to five years post therapy.Journal of Neurotherapy, 4(4), 11–18.Google Scholar
Berkman, E. T. & Lieberman, M. D. (2010). Approaching the bad and avoiding the good: Lateral prefrontal cortical asymmetry distinguishes between action and valence.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(9), 1970–1979.Google Scholar
Briesemeister, B. B., Tamm, S., Heine, A., & Jacobs, A. M. (2013). Approach the good, withdraw from the bad – a review on frontal alpha asymmetry measures in applied psychological research.Psychology, 4(3), 261–267 Google Scholar
Broekens, J. (2012). In defense of dominance: PAD usage in computational representations of affect.International Journal of Synthetic Emotions, 3(1), 33–42.Google Scholar
Cavazza, M., Aranyi, G., Charles, F., et al. (2014a). Towards empathic neurofeedback for interactive storytelling.Open Access Series in Informatics, 41, 42–60.Google Scholar
Cavazza, M., Aranyi, G., Charles, F., et al. (2014b). Frontal alpha asymmetry neurofeedback for brain–computer interfaces. In Proceedings of the 6th International Brain–Computer Interface Conference, December. Graz, Austria.
Cavazza, M., Charles, F., Aranyi, G., et al. (2014). Towards emotional regulation through neurofeedback. In Proceedings of the 5th Augmented Human International Conference (p. 42).
Cavazza, M., Lugrin, J. L., & Buehner, M. (2007). Causal perception in virtual reality and its implications for presence factors.Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 16(6), 623–642.Google Scholar
Coan, J. A. & Allen, J. J. (2003). The state and trait nature of frontal EEG asymmetry in emotion. In K, Hugdahl & R. J, Davidson (Eds), The Asymmetrical Brain (pp. 565–615), Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Coan, J. A. & Allen, J. J. (2004). Frontal EEG asymmetry as a moderator and mediator of emotion.Biological psychology, 67(1), 7–50.Google Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1991). Flow: The Psychology of Optimal Experience (vol. 41). New York: Harper Perennial.
Davidson, R. J. (1992). Anterior cerebral asymmetry and the nature of emotion.Brain and Cognition, 20(1), 125–151.Google Scholar
Davidson, R. J. (1998). Anterior electrophysiological asymmetries, emotion, and depression: Conceptual and methodological conundrums.Psychophysiology, 35(5), 607–614.Google Scholar
Davidson, R. J. (2003). Darwin and the neural bases of emotion and affective style.Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1000(1), 316–336.Google Scholar
Davidson, R. J. (2004). What does the prefrontal cortex “do” in affect: Perspectives on frontal EEG asymmetry research.Biological Psychology, 67(1), 219–234.Google Scholar
Davidson, R. J., Ekman, P., Saron, C. D., Senulis, J. A., & Friesen, W. V. (1990). Approachwithdrawal and cerebral asymmetry: Emotional expression and brain physiology: I.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58(2), 330.Google Scholar
Decety, J. & Moriguchi, Y. (2007). The empathic brain and its dysfunction in psychiatric populations: Implications for intervention across different clinical conditions.BioPsychoSocial Medicine, 1(1), 22.Google Scholar
Demaree, H. A., Everhart, D. E., Youngstrom, E. A., & Harrison, D. W. (2005). Brain lateralization of emotional processing: Historical roots and a future incorporating “dominance.” Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 4(1), 3–20.Google Scholar
De Waal, F. B. (2008). Putting the altruism back into altruism: The evolution of empathy.Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 279–300.Google Scholar
Elgendi, M., Dauwels, J., Rebsamen, B., et al. (2014). From auditory and visual to immersive neurofeedback: Application to diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease. In Z, Yang (Ed.), Neural Computation, Neural Devices, and Neural Prosthesis (pp. 63–97). New York: Springer.
Fox, N. A. & Davidson, R. J. (1988). Patterns of brain electrical activity during facial signs of emotion in 10-month-old infants.Developmental Psychology, 24(2), 230–236.Google Scholar
Gable, P. & Harmon-Jones, E. (2008). Relative left frontal activation to appetitive stimuli: Considering the role of individual differences.Psychophysiology, 45(2), 275–278.Google Scholar
Gaffary, Y., Eyharabide, V., Martin, J.-C., & Ammi, M. (2013). Clustering approach to characterize haptic expressions of emotions.ACM Transactions on Applied Perception, 10(4), 1–20.Google Scholar
Gatica-Perez, D. (2009). Automatic nonverbal analysis of social interaction in small groups: A review.Image and Vision Computing, 27(12), 1775–1787.Google Scholar
Gebhard, P. (2005). ALMA: a layered model of affect. In Proceedings of The Fourth International Joint Conference on Autonomous Agents and Multiagent Systems (pp. 29–36).
Gilroy, S.W., Cavazza, M., & Benayoun, M. (2009). Using affective trajectories to describe states of flow in interactive art. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Advances in Computer Entertainment Technology (pp. 165–172).
Gilroy, S. W., Cavazza, M., Niiranen, M., et al. (2009). PAD-based multimodal affective fusion. In 3rd International Conference on Affective Computing and Intelligent Interaction and Workshops (pp. 1–8).
Gilroy, S. W., Cavazza, M. O., & Vervondel, V. (2011). Evaluating multimodal affective fusion using physiological signals. In Proceedings of the 16th International Conference on Intelligent User Interfaces (pp. 53–62).
Gilroy, S. W., Porteous, J., Charles, F., et al. (2013). A brain-computer interface to a plan-based narrative. In Proceedings of the Twenty-Third International Joint Conference on Artificial Intelligence (pp. 1997–2005).
Glowinski, D., Coletta, P., Volpe, G., et al. (2010). Multi-scale entropy analysis of dominance in social creative activities. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Multimedia (pp. 1035–1038).
Gutsell, J. N. & Inzlicht, M. (2010). Empathy constrained: Prejudice predicts reduced mental simulation of actions during observation of outgroups.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(5), 841–845.Google Scholar
Harmon-Jones, E. (2004). Contributions from research on anger and cognitive dissonance to understanding the motivational functions of asymmetrical frontal brain activity.Biological Psychology, 67(1), 51–76.Google Scholar
Harmon-Jones, E., Gable, P. A., & Peterson, C. K. (2010). The role of asymmetric frontal cortical activity in emotion-related phenomena: A review and update.Biological Psychology, 84(3), 451–462.Google Scholar
Harmon-Jones, E. & Peterson, C. K. (2009). Supine body position reduces neural response to anger evocation.Psychological Science, 20(10), 1209–1210.Google Scholar
Huys, Q. J., Cools, R., Gölzer, M., et al. (2011). Disentangling the roles of approach, activation and valence in instrumental and Pavlovian responding.PLoS Computational Biology, 7(4), e1002028.Google Scholar
Kleinsmith, A. & Bianchi-Berthouze, N. (2007). Recognizing affective dimensions from body posture.Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 4738, 48–58.Google Scholar
Kleinsmith, A. & Bianchi-Berthouze, N. (2013). Affective body expression perception and recognition: A survey.IEEE Transactions on Affective Computing, 4(1), 15–33.Google Scholar
Lang, P. J., Greenwald, M. K., Bradley, M. M., & Hamm, A. O. (1993). Looking at pictures: Affective, facial, visceral, and behavioral reactions.Psychophysiology, 30(3), 261–273.Google Scholar
Light, S. N., Coan, J. A., Zahn-Waxler, C., et al. (2009). Empathy is associated with dynamic change in prefrontal brain electrical activity during positive emotion in children.Child Development, 80(4), 1210–1231.Google Scholar
Lindquist, K. A., Wager, T. D., Kober, H., Bliss-Moreau, E., & Barrett, L. F. (2012). The brain basis of emotion: A meta-analytic review.Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(3), 121–143.Google Scholar
Liu, Y. & Sourina, O. (2012). EEG-based dominance level recognition for emotion-enabled interaction. In Proceedings of IEEE International Conference on Multimedia and Expo (pp. 1039–1044).
Lowe, R. & Ziemke, T. (2011). The feeling of action tendencies: On the emotional regulation of goal-directed behavior. Frontiers in Psychology, 2.
Mehrabian, A. (1996). Pleasure-arousal-dominance: A general framework for describing and measuring individual differences in temperament.Current Psychology, 14, 261–292.Google Scholar
Mehrabian, A. & Russell, J. A. (1974). An Approach to Environmental Psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Prendinger, H. & Ishizuka, M. (2005). The empathic companion: A character-based interface that addresses users' affective states.Applied Artificial Intelligence, 19(3–4), 267–285.Google Scholar
Reeves, B. & Nass, C. (1996). The Media Equation: How People Treat Computers, Television, and New Media Like Real People and Places. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Reuderink, B., Mühl, C., & Poel, M. (2013). Valence, arousal and dominance in the EEG during game play.International Journal of Autonomous and Adaptive Communications Systems, 6(1), 45–62.Google Scholar
Rosenfeld, J. P., Cha, G., Blair, T., & Gotlib, I. H. (1995). Operant (biofeedback) control of leftright frontal alpha power differences: Potential neurotherapy for affective disorders.Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 20(3), 241–258.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. & Mehrabian, A. (1977). Evidence for a three-factor theory of emotions.Journal of Research in Personality, 11(3), 273–294.Google Scholar
Sander, D. & Scherer, K. R. (2014). Traité de psychologie des émotions. Paris: Dunod.
Sutton, S. K. & Davidson, R. J. (1997). Prefrontal brain asymmetry: A biological substrate of the behavioral approach and inhibition systems.Psychological Science, 8(3), 204–210.Google Scholar
Tan, E. S. H. (1995). Film-induced affect as a witness emotion.Poetics, 23(1), 7–32.Google Scholar
Tullett, A. M., Harmon-Jones, E., & Inzlicht, M. (2012). Right frontal cortical asymmetry predicts empathic reactions: Support for a link between withdrawal motivation and empathy.Psychophysiology, 49(8), 1145–1153.Google Scholar
Valenzi, S., Islam, T., Jurica, P., & Cichocki, A. (2014). Individual Classification of Emotions Using EEG. Journal of Biomedical Science and Engineering, 2014.
Wehbe, R. R., Kappen, D. L., Rojas, D., et al. (2013). EEG-based assessment of video and ingame learning. In Proceedings of CHI'13 Extended Abstracts on Human Factors in Computing Systems (pp. 667–672).
Wehbe, R. R., Zerebecki, C., Khattak, S., Hogue, A., & Nacke, L. E. (2014). User Research for 3D Display Settings with EEG Frontal Alpha Asymmetry. In CHI Play 2014 Games User Workshop, Toronto.Google Scholar
Witmer, B. G. & Singer, M. J. (1998). Measuring presence in virtual environments: A presence questionnaire.Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 7(3), 225–240.Google Scholar
Zotev, V., Phillips, R., Yuan, H., Misaki, M., & Bodurka, J. (2014). Self-regulation of human brain activity using simultaneous real-time fMRI and EEG neurofeedback.NeuroImage, 85, 985–995.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×