Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-wzw2p Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-01T05:33:02.456Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

6.1 - Commentary on “Time and Embodiment in the Process of Psychotherapy: A Dynamical Systems Perspective”The Musicality of Human Interaction

from Part I - Body and Time: General Aspects

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 October 2020

Christian Tewes
Affiliation:
Heidelberg University Hospital
Giovanni Stanghellini
Affiliation:
Chieti University
Get access

Summary

Minkowski described the therapeutic relationship in terms of “two melodies being played simultaneously” (Minkowski, 1933/1970, p. 182). The final aim of therapy, in his view, was to establish a certain balance between these two melodies and grasp the patient's psyche. Accordingly, the therapeutic process should be conceived as a participatory experience , a dialogical, intercorporeal space (Fuchs, 2007) where the primary components are prereflective elements that converge into a participatory “we-subject.”

Type
Chapter
Information
Time and Body
Phenomenological and Psychopathological Approaches
, pp. 117 - 122
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Binswanger, L. (1960). Melancholie und Manie [Melancholy and mania]. Pfullingen, Germany: Neske.Google Scholar
Bizzari, V., & Guareschi, C. (2017). Bodily memory and joint action in music practice and therapy. Quaderni della Ginestra, 3, 114121.Google Scholar
Bratman, M. (2013). Shared agency: A planning theory of acting together. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fuchs, T. (2007). Psychotherapy of the lived space: A phenomenological and ecological concept. American Journal of Psychotherapy, 61(4), 423439. doi:10.1176/appi.psychotherapy.2007.61.4.423CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuchs, T. (2016). Embodied knowledge – Embodied memory. In Rinofner-Kreidl, S. & Wiltsche, H. A. (Eds.), Analytic and continental philosophy: Methods and perspectives. Proceedings of the 37th international Wittgenstein symposium (pp. 215230). Berlin, Germany: De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Fuchs, T. (2017a). Intercorporeality and interaffectivity. In Meyer, C., Streeck, J., & Jordan, S. (Eds.), Intercorporeality: Emerging socialities in interaction (pp. 324). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuchs, T. (2017b). Self across time: The diachronic unity of bodily existence. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 16(2), 291315. doi:10.1007/s11097-015-9449-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuchs, T. (2018). Ecology of the brain: The phenomenology and biology of the embodied mind. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fuchs, T., & Pallagrosi, M. (2018). Phenomenology of temporality and dimensional psychopathology. In Biondi, M., Pasquini, M., & Picardi, A. (Eds.), Dimensional psychopathology (pp. 287300). New York, NY: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, M. (1989). On social facts. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Hobson, J. A., & Friston, K. J. (2014). Consciousness, dreams, and inference. The Cartesian theatre revisited. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 21(1–2), 632.Google Scholar
Jaspers, K. (1913). Allgemeine Psychopathologie [General psychopathology]. Berlin, Germany: Springer.Google Scholar
Koehne, S., Hatri, A., Cacioppo, J. T., & Dziobek, I. (2016). Perceived interpersonal synchrony increases empathy: Insights from autism spectrum disorder. Cognition, 146, 815. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2015.09.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Minkowski, E. (1970). Lived time: Phenomenological and psychopathological studies (Metzel, N., Trans.). Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press. (Original work published 1933)Google Scholar
Ramseyer, F., & Tschacher, W. (2011). Nonverbal synchrony in psychotherapy: Coordinated body movement reflects relationship quality and outcome. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 79(3), 284295. doi:10.1037/a0023419CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richardson, D. C., & Dale, R. (2005). Looking to understand: The coupling between speakers' and listeners' eye movements and its relationship to discourse comprehension. Cognitive Science, 29(6), 10451060. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog0000_29CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schilbach, L., Eickhoff, S., Cieslik, E., Kuzmanovic, B., & Vogeley, K. (2012). Shall we do this together? Social gaze influences action control in a comparison group, but not in individuals with high-functioning autism. Autism, 16(2), 151162. doi:10.1177/1362361311409258CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schilbach, L., Timmermans, B., Reddy, V., Costall, A., Bente, G., Schlicht, T., & Vogeley, K. (2013). Toward a second-person neuroscience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(4), 393414. doi:10.1017/S0140525X12000660Google Scholar
Searle, J. (1995). The construction of social reality. New York, NY: The Free Press.Google Scholar
Tatossian, A. (1979). La Phénoménologie des psychoses [The phenomenology of psychosis]. Paris, France: Masson.Google Scholar
Tollefsen, D., & Dale, R. (2012). Naturalizing joint action: A process-based approach, Philosophical Psychology, 25(3), 385407. doi:10.1080/09515089.2011.579418CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tschacher, W. (2021). Time and embodiment in the process of psychotherapy – A dynamical systems perspective. In Tewes, C. & Stanghellini, G. (Eds.), Time and body: Phenomenological and psychopathological approaches (pp. 104116). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tuomela, R. (2013). Social ontology: Collective intentionality and group agents. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Zahavi, D. (2014). Self and other: Exploring subjectivity, empathy and shame. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×