Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home

Vocabulary size and auditory word recognition in preschool children

  • FRANZO LAW (a1), TRISTAN MAHR (a1), ALISSA SCHNEEBERG (a1) and JAN EDWARDS (a1)

Abstract

Recognizing familiar words quickly and accurately facilitates learning new words, as well as other aspects of language acquisition. This study used the visual world paradigm with semantic and phonological competitors to study lexical processing efficiency in 2- to 5-year-old children. Experiment 1 found this paradigm was sensitive to vocabulary-size differences. Experiment 2 included a more diverse group of children who were tested in their native dialect (either African American English or mainstream American English). No effect of stimulus dialect was observed. The results showed that vocabulary size was a better predictor of eye gaze patterns than was maternal education, but that maternal education level had a moderating effect; as maternal education level increased, vocabulary size was less predictive of lexical processing efficiency.

Copyright

Corresponding author

ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Franzo Law II, Department of Psychology and Department of Communications Sciences and Disorders, University of Wisconsin–Madison, 1500 Highland Avenue, Room 489, Madison, WI 53705. E-mail: flaw@wisc.edu

References

Hide All
Adank, P., Evans, B. G., Stuart-Smith, J., & Scott, S. K. (2009). Comprehension of familiar and unfamiliar native accents under adverse listening conditions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 520529.
Adank, P., & McQueen, J. M. (2007). The effect of an unfamiliar regional accent on spoken comprehension. Paper presented at the 16th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Saarbrüken, Germany.
Allopenna, P. D., Magnuson, J. S., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1998). Tracking the time course of spoken word recognition: Evidence for continuous mapping models. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 419439.
Arias-Trejo, N., & Plunkett, K. (2010). The effects of perceptual similarity and category membership on early word-referent identification. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105, 6380. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2009.10.002
Barr, D. J. (2008). Analyzing “visual world” eyetracking data using multilevel logistic regression. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 457474. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2007.09.002
Bates, D. (2006). Lmer, p-values and all that. Paper presented at the ETH Seminar for Statistics. Retrieved from https://stat.ethz.ch/pipermail/r-help/2006-May/094765.html
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 148. doi:10.18637/jss.v067.i01
Bjorklund, D. F. (1987). How age changes in knowledge base contribute to the development of children's memory: An interpretive review. Developmental Review, 7, 193–130. doi:10.1016/0273-2297(87)90007-4
Bortfeld, H., Rathbun, K., Morgan, J., & Golinkoff, R. (2003). What's in a name? Highly familiar items anchor infants’ segmentation of fluent speech. In Beachley, B., Brown, A., & Conlin, F. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 27th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (Vol. 1, pp. 162172). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla.
Bornstein, M. H., Haynes, M. O., & Painter, K. M. (1998). Sources of child vocabulary competence: A multivariate model. Journal of Child Language, 25, 367393. doi:10.1017/S0305000998003456
Capone, N. C., & McGregor, K. K. (2005). The effect of semantic representations on toddlers’ word retrieval. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 14681480. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2005/102)
Clopper, C. G. (2012). Effects of dialect variation on the semantic predictability benefit. Language and Cognitive Processes, 27, 10021020.
Craig, H. K., Thompson, C. A., Washington, J. A., & Potter, S. L. (2003). Phonological features of child African American English. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 623635. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2003/049)
Craig, H. K., & Washington, J. A. (2002). Oral language expectations for African American pre- schoolers and kindergartners. American Journal of Speech–Language Pathology, 11, 5970. doi:10.1044/1058-0360(2002/007)
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, D. M. (2007). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (4th ed.). Minneapolis, MN: NCS Pearson.
Edwards, J., Beckman, M. E., & Munson, B. (2004). Vocabulary size and phonotactic production accuracy and fluency in nonword repetition. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 421436. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2004/034)
Ensminger, M. E., & Fothergill, K. (2003). A decade of measuring SES: What it tells us and where to go from here. In Bornstein, M. H. & Bradley, R. H. (Eds.), Socioeconomic status, parenting, and child development. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Felder, L. (2006). Aptitude, attitude and motivation in second language proficiency: A test of Clément's model. Unpublished manuscript.
Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D. J., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., & Bates, E. (2007). MacArthur–Bates Communicative Development Inventories: User's guide and technical manual (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
Fernald, A. (2010). Getting beyond the “convenience sample” in research on early cognitive development. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 9192. doi:10.1017/S0140525X10000294
Fernald, A., & Marchman, V. A. (2012). Individual differences in lexical processing at 18 months predict vocabulary growth in typically-developing and late-talking toddlers. Child Development, 83, 203222. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01692.x
Fernald, A., Marchman, V. A., & Weisleder, A. (2013). SES differences in language processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 months. Developmental Science, 16, 234248. doi:10.1111/desc.12019
Fernald, A., Perfors, A., & Marchman, V. A. (2006). Picking up speed in understanding: Speech processing efficiency and vocabulary growth across the 2nd year. Developmental Psychology, 42, 98116. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.42.1.98
Fernald, A., Swingley, D., & Pinto, J. P. (2001). When half a word is enough: Infants can recognize spoken words using partial phonetic information. Child Development, 72, 10031015. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00331
Fernald, A., Zangl, R., Portillo, A. L., & Marchman, V. A. (2008). Looking while listening: Using eye movements to monitor spoken language comprehension by infants and young children. In Sekerina, I. A., Fernández, E. M., & Clahsen, H. (Eds.), Developmental psycholinguistics: On-line methods in children's language processing. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Groothuis-Oudshoorn, K., & van Buuren, S. (2011). MICE: Multivariate imputation by chained equations in R. Journal of Statistical Software, 45.
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1991). Mother–child conversation in different social classes and communicative settings. Child Development, 62, 782796. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.ep9109162253
Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1998). The relation of birth order and socioeconomic status to children's language experience and language development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 603629. doi:10.1017/S0142716400010389
Huang, Y. T., & Snedeker, J. (2011). Cascading activation across levels of representation in children's lexical processing. Journal of Child Language, 38, 644661.
Huettig, F., & Altmann, G. T. M. (2005). Word meaning and the control of eye fixation: Semantic competitor effects and the visual world paradigm. Cognition, 96, B23B32. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2004.10.003
Huettig, F., & McQueen, J. M. (2007). The tug of war between phonological, semantic and shape information in language-mediated visual search. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 460482. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.001
Hurtado, N., Grüter, T., Marchman, V. A., & Fernald, A. (2014). Relative language exposure, processing efficiency and vocabulary in Spanish-English bilingual toddlers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17, 189202. doi:10.1017/S136672891300014X
Hurtado, N., Marchman, V. A., & Fernald, A. (2007). Spoken word recognition by Latino children learning Spanish as their first language. Journal of Child Language, 33, 227249. doi:10.1017/S0305000906007896
Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M., & Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology, 27, 236248. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.27.2.236
Inhoff, A. W., & Radach, R. (1998). Definition and computation of oculomotor measures in the study of cognitive processes. In Underwood, G. M. (Ed.), Eye guidance in reading and scene perception (pp. 2953). Oxford: Elsevier.
Law II, F., & Edwards, J. R. (2014). Effects of vocabulary size on online lexical processing by preschoolers. Language Learning and Development. Advance online publication. doi:10.1080/15475441.2014.961066
Magnuson, J. S., Mirman, D., & Myers, E. (2013). Spoken word recognition. In Reisberg, D. (Ed.), Oxford handbook of cognitive psychology (pp. 412441). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Mahr, T., McMillan, B., Saffran, J., Ellis Weismer, S., & Edwards, J. (2015). Anticipatory coartic- ulation facilitates word recognition in toddlers. Cognition, 142, 345350. doi:10:1016/j.cognition.2015.05.009
Marchman, V. A., & Fernald, A. (2008). Speed of word recognition and vocabulary knowledge in infancy predict cognitive and language outcomes in later childhood. Developmental Science, 11, F9F16. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00671.x
Mayor, J., & Plunkett, K. (2014). Infant word recognition: Insights from TRACE simulations. Journal of Memory and Language, 71, 89123. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2013.09.009
McClelland, J. L., & Elman, J. L. (1986). The TRACE model of speech perception. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 186. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(86)90015-0
McMurray, B., Samelson, V. M., Lee, S. H., & Tomblin, J. B. (2010). Individual differences in online spoken word recognition: Implications for SLI. Cognitive Psychology, 60, 139. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2009.06.003
Metsala, J. L. (1999). Young children's phonological awareness and nonword repetition as a function of vocabulary development. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 319. doi:10.1037//0022-0663.91.1.3
Mirman, D. (2014). Growth curve analysis and visualization using R. Boca Raton, FL: Chapman & Hall.
Mirman, D., Dixon, J. A., & Magnuson, J. S. (2008). Statistical and computational models of the visual world paradigm: Growth curves and individual differences. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 475494. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2007.11.006
Morrison, C. M., Chappell, T. D., & Ellis, A. W. (1997). Age of acquisition norms for a large set of object names and their relation to adult estimates and other variables. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 50, 528559.
Nathan, L., Wells, B., & Donlan, C. (1998). Children's comprehension of unfamiliar regional dialects: A preliminary investigation. Journal of Child Language, 24, 343365.
Noble, K. G., Norman, M. F., & Farah, M. J. (2005). Neurocognitive correlates of socio-economic status in kindergarten children. Developmental Science, 8, 7487. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00394.x
Pan, B. A., Rowe, M. L., Singer, J. D., & Snow, C. E. (2005). Maternal correlates of growth in toddler vocabulary production in low-income families. Child Development, 76, 763782. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00876.x
Radach, R., Heller, D., & Inhoff, A. (1999). Occurrence and function of very short fixation durations in reading. In Becker, W., Deubel, H., & Mergner, T. (Eds.), Current oculomotor research: Physiological and psychological aspects (pp. 321331). New York: Springer.
Rescorla, L. (2002). Language and reading outcomes to age 9 in late-talking toddlers. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 360371. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2002/028)
Rescorla, L. (2009). Outcomes in late-talking toddlers: Support for a dimensional perspective on language delay. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52, 1630. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2008/07-0171)
Salverda, A. P., Kleinschmidt, D., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2014). Immediate effects of anticipatory coarticulation in spoken-word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 71, 145163. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2013.11.002
Shi, R., Werker, J. F., & Cutler, A. (2003). Function words in early speech perception. Paper presented at the 15th International Conference of Phonetic Sciences, Adelaide, Australia.
Shipley, K. G., & McAfee, J. G. (2015). Assessment in speech–language pathology: A resource manual (5th ed.). Boston: Cengage Learning.
Swingley, D., Pinto, J. P., & Fernald, A. (1999). Continuous processing in word recognition at 24 months. Cognition, 71, 73108. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00021-9
Thompson, M., & Thompson, G. (1972). Response of infants and young children as a function of auditory stimuli and test methods. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 15, 699707. doi:10.1044/jshr.1504.699
van Heugten, M., Krieger, D., & Johnson, E. K. (2015). The developmental trajectory of toddlers’ comprehension of unfamiliar regional accents. Language Learning and Development, 11, 4165. doi: 10.1080/15475441.2013.879636
van Heugten, M., & Johnson, E. K. (2014). Learning to contend with accents in infancy: Benefits of brief speaker exposure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 340350. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032192
Vermeer, A. (2001). Breadth and depth of vocabulary in relationship to L1/L2 acquisition and frequency of input. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22, 217234.
Weisleder, A., & Fernald, A. (2013). Talking to children matters: Early language experience streng-thens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science, 24, 21432152. doi:10.1177/0956797613488145
Werker, J. F., Fennell, C. T., Corcoran, K. M., & Stager, C. L. (2002). Infants’ ability to learn phonetically similar words: Effects of age and vocabulary size. Infancy, 3, 239273. doi:10.1207/15250000252828226
White, K. S., & Morgan, J. L. (2008). Sub-segmental detail in early lexical representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 114132. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2008.03.001
Williams, K. T. (2007). Expressive Vocabulary Test (2nd ed.). Minneapolis, MN: Pearson Assessments.

Vocabulary size and auditory word recognition in preschool children

  • FRANZO LAW (a1), TRISTAN MAHR (a1), ALISSA SCHNEEBERG (a1) and JAN EDWARDS (a1)

Metrics

Altmetric attention score

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between <date>. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed