Skip to main content
×
Home
    • Aa
    • Aa

Self-deception is adaptive in itself

  • Louisa C. Egan (a1)
Abstract
Abstract

Von Hippel & Trivers reason that the potential benefits of successfully deceiving others provide a basis for the evolution of self-deception. However, as self-deceptive processes themselves provide considerable adaptive value to an individual, self-deception may have evolved as an end in itself, rather than as the means to an end of improving other-deception.

Copyright
Linked references
Hide All

This list contains references from the content that can be linked to their source. For a full set of references and notes please see the PDF or HTML where available.

L. G. Aspinwall & L. Richter (1999) Optimism and self-mastery predict more rapid disengagement from unsolvable tasks in presence of alternatives. Motivation and Emotion 23:221–45.

E. Babad (1997) Wishful thinking among voters: Motivational and cognitive influences. International Journal of Public Opinion Research 9:105–25.

E. Balcetis & D. Dunning (2010) Wishful seeing: Desired objects are seen as closer. Psychological Science 21:147–52.

E. Balcetis , D. Dunning & R. L. Miller (2008) Do collectivists know themselves better than individualists? Cross-cultural studies of the holier than thou phenomenon. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 95:1252–67.

A. Bandura (1989) Human agency in social cognitive theory. American Psychologist 44:1175–84.

C. D. Batson & E. R. Thompson (2001) Why don't moral people act morally? Motivational considerations. Current Directions in Psychological Science 10:5457.

J. S. Bruner & C. C. Goodman (1947) Value and need as organizing factors in perception. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 42:3344.

L. Egan , P. Bloom & L. R. Santos (2010) Choice-induced preferences in the absence of choice: Evidence from a blind two choice paradigm with young children and capuchin monkeys. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 46:204207.

L. Festinger & J. M. Carlsmith (1959) Cognitive consequences of forced compliance. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 58:203–11.

P. Johansson , L. Hall , S. Sikstrom & A. Olsson (2005) Failure to detect mismatches between intention and outcome in a simple decision task. Science 310:116–19.

Z. Krizan & P. D. Windschitl (2009) Wishful thinking about the future: Does desire impact optimism? Social and Personality Psychology Compass 3:227–43.

J. H. Riskind , R. Moore & L. Bowley (1995) The looming of spiders: The fearful perceptual distortion of movement and menace. Behaviour Research and Therapy 33:171–78.

C. M. Steele & T. J. Liu (1983) Dissonance processes as self-affirmation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 45:519.

L. S. Sugiyama , J. Tooby , L. Cosmides (2002) Cross-cultural evidence of cognitive adaptations for social exchange among the Shiwiar of Ecuadorian Amazonia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 99:11537–45. Available at: http://www.uoregon.edu/~sugiyama/docs/cogadaptsocex.pdf.

S. E. Taylor & D. A. Armor (1996) Positive illusions and coping with adversity. Journal of Personality 64:873–98.

S. E. Taylor & J. D. Brown (1988) Illusion and well-being: A social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin 103:193210.

P. Valdesolo & D. A. DeSteno (2007) Moral hypocrisy: Social groups and the flexibility of virtue. Psychological Science 18:689–90.

Recommend this journal

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this journal to your organisation's collection.

Behavioral and Brain Sciences
  • ISSN: 0140-525X
  • EISSN: 1469-1825
  • URL: /core/journals/behavioral-and-brain-sciences
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to? *
×